Skip to main content

Navigating an unpredictable environment: the moderating role of perceived environmental unpredictability in the effectiveness of ecological resource scarcity information on pro-environmental behavior

Abstract

Background

The global issue of ecological resource scarcity, worsened by climate change, necessitates effective methods to promote resource conservation. One commonly used approach is presenting ecological resource scarcity information. However, the effectiveness of this method remains uncertain, particularly in an unpredictable world. This research aims to examine the role of perceived environmental unpredictability in moderating the impact of ecological resource scarcity information on pro-environmental behavior (PEB).

Methods

We conducted three studies to test our hypothesis on moderation. Study 1 (N = 256) measured perceived general environmental unpredictability, perceived resource scarcity and daily PEB frequencies in a cross-sectional survey. Study 2 (N = 107) took it a step further by manipulating resource scarcity. Importantly, to increase ecological validity, Study 3 (N = 135) manipulated the information on both ecological resource scarcity and nature-related environmental unpredictability, and measured real water and paper consumption using a newly developed washing-hands paradigm.

Results

In Study 1, we discovered that perceived resource scarcity positively predicted PEB, but only when individuals perceive the environment as less unpredictable (interaction effect: 95% CI = [-0.09, -0.01], ΔR2 = 0.018). Furthermore, by manipulating scarcity information, Study 2 revealed that only for individuals with lower levels of environmental unpredictability presenting ecological resource scarcity information could decrease forest resource consumption intention (interaction effect: 95%CI = [-0.025, -0.031], ΔR2 = .04). Moreover, Study 3 found that the negative effect of water resource scarcity information on actual water and (interaction effect: 95%CI = [3.037, 22.097], ηp2 = .050) paper saving behaviors (interaction effect: 95%CI = [0.021, 0.275], ηp2 = .040), as well as hypothetical forest resource consumption (interaction effect: 95%CI = [-0.053, 0.849], ηp2 = .023) emerged only for people who receiving weaker environmental unpredictability information.

Conclusion

Across three studies, we provide evidence to support the moderation hypothesis that environmental unpredictability weakens the positive effect of ecological resource scarcity information on PEB, offering important theoretical and practical implications on the optimal use of resource scarcity to enhance PEB.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Ecological resource scarcity, such as water and energy, poses significant challenges in our current times. The reduction of renewable freshwater resources per capita by 55% from 1993 to 2014 emphasizes the urgency of addressing this issue [1]. According to the World Economic Forum (2019), water shortages remain a top concern for policymakers and business leaders worldwide. In response to resource scarcity, various entities, including governments, water utilities, and community-based organizations, have employed different strategies to promote resource conservation [2]. One of the most common approaches is to raise problem awareness by conveying information about resource scarcity [2]. For example, the fact that billions of people lack access to safe water is utilized in the World Water Day campaign in 2023 to encourage more people to take action. Additionally, the Hong Kong SAR Government’s “Let’s Save 10L Water 2.0” campaign emphasizes the importance of conserving water by highlighting the limited availability of this resource.

Despite these efforts, it is important to recognize the complexity and interconnectedness of the world we live in, which makes predicting future environmental conditions challenging. Unforeseen events such as pathogen prevalence, natural disasters, wars, and financial crises illustrate the dynamic nature of our environment. In such an unpredictable world, can simply providing information about ecological resource scarcity lead to a significant increase in pro-environmental behaviors?

In the current research, we aimed to explore whether ecological resource scarcity information could promote pro-environmental behaviors effectively in the unpredictable world. We argued that ecological resource scarcity information is not necessarily useful in promoting pro-environmental behaviors and proposed that environmental unpredictability is a vital factor weakening the effect of ecological resource scarcity on resource consumption.

Uncertain association between ecological resource scarcity information and pro-environmental behaviors

Based on the information-motivation-behavioral skills (IMB) model, individuals are more likely to change their behavior when they are informed about a problem, along with being motivated to act and have skills to act [3]. In the environmental protection domain, there is a general lack of problem awareness about ecological resource scarcity [4, 5]. This lack of awareness hinders individuals from engaging in pro-environmental behaviors (PEB), which refers to the actions that enhance the quality of the environment, regardless of the intent behind them [6]. Resource conservation campaigns often focus on resource scarcity information to encourage PEB [7]. In some empirical studies, the resource scarcity information was found to be effective. For example, individuals living in regions that experience drought have a higher tendency to make behavioral changes to conserve water [8, 9]. People who perceived stronger ecological resource scarcity reported higher resource-saving behavioral frequencies [10], and indicated a higher frequency of PEB [11]. And water scarcity information was linked to a significant decrease in water use [12,13,14].

However, we identified some conflicting evidence. Information about resource scarcity is often not sufficient to reduce resource consumption in intervention [15], and the effectiveness of awareness campaigns is unclear [16]. For example, presenting the information about water resource scarcity only was evaluated as ineffective to promote water-saving behaviors by lay people [10]. Energy scarcity information was not strong enough to affect attitudes, intentions, and behaviors toward electricity energy saving [17]. Moreover, resource scarcity information failed to modify resource consumption behaviors in experimental settings [2, 18].

The uncertain relationship between resource scarcity and PEB can be understood through an evolutionary psychological approach. According to the life history theory, individuals may adopt various strategies for allocating resources [19,20,21,22,23]. Those who choose a slow life history strategy prioritize long-term benefits and future planning, which leads them to behave in an environmentally friendly manner for the sake of future generations. On the other hand, individuals adopting a fast life history strategy prioritize immediate gains over long-term consequences [24], resulting in less PEB.

This theory, combined with empirical evidence, suggest that the impact of resource scarcity on PEB may vary depending on the situation, implying that promoting pro-environmental actions may require considering factors beyond simply informing individuals about scarcity. If PEB is seen as an investment in the environment, people engaging in PEB expect long-term benefits from it. However, the environment does not always provide consistent long-term benefits, particularly in today’s unpredictable world. When the expected advantages of environmental protection become uncertain, individuals may prioritize immediate gains, exploit natural resources, and reduce their commitment to PEB. This study hence focuses on the situational factor related to the unpredictable environment, testing its importance in influencing individuals’ PEB under resource scarcity.

Moderating role of environmental unpredictability

Environmental unpredictability is defined as the level of spatial–temporal variation in environmental harshness [24]. Past empirical studies measured environmental unpredictability in diverse ways [25]. In the current research, we tried to capture both individual-related and nature-related environmental unpredictability in temporal or spatial dimensions. Individual-related environmental unpredictability is mostly indicated by residential changes, and changes in parental financial status for children [19, 24, 26]. It shows whether the structure of an environment, such as the social or economic environment in which one lives, changes over time. Nature-related environmental unpredictability focuses on the pattern of variation that makes environments unpredictable, such as unpredictability of weather and the unpredictability of natural disasters [25].

Based on the life history theory, the environment plays a crucial role in shaping individuals’ life history strategies [19,20,21,22,23]. In predictable environments individuals are more likely to adopt a slow life-history strategy, while highly unpredictable environments promote a fast life-history strategy [24]. Importantly, environmental unpredictability during childhood can influence short-sighted tendencies [27,28,29,30], and this effect can also be observed in adulthood [31]. In an unpredictable environment, individuals prioritize immediate desires over future needs because investing in long-term environmental protection may not yield future benefits. This has implications for PEB, as present efforts on environmental protection may not be effective in improving resource scarcity in the future when the environment is unpredictable.

There are two aspects that illustrate the expectation that PEB efforts may not pay off in unpredictable environments. Firstly, in an unpredictable environment, there is a flow of uncontrollable information, which makes it challenging for individuals to maintain strong beliefs that their actions can bring about positive outcomes, such as improving resource scarcity [32]. According to the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior, the impact of awareness of the problem on behavior is greater when individuals perceive a higher level of control over their actions [33]. Hence, environmental unpredictability not only reduces the perceived personal control but also creates a barrier between scarcity awareness and PEB.

Secondly, in unpredictable environments, individuals are more likely to fear free riders, which further hinders behavioral change towards environmental protection under resource scarcity. When deciding whether to take action to protect the environment, people consider whether others will cooperate. However, in unpredictable environments, the likelihood of others investing in PEB becomes uncertain as well, which induces a heightened fear of free riders. For instance, experimental games have shown that individuals behave less cooperatively and invest fewer public goods when the probability of benefiting from them is uncertain [34]. Moreover, studies have demonstrated that individuals are less likely to prioritize the interests of others over their own when environmental unpredictability is primed [31, 35]. Due to the fear that others will not take action in an unpredictable environment, individual efforts to protect the environment may appear less effective in solving the issue of resource scarcity.

Taken together, stronger environmental unpredictability is associated with a fast life-history strategy characterized by low self-efficacy and high fear of free riders, which ultimately leads to less PEB performance in the face of resource scarcity. Both multilevel and individual-level studies have indicated that psychological traits similar to the fast life history strategy weaken the association between environmental problem awareness and actual PEB [10, 36]. Besides, some indirect evidence revealed that resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability could lead to some psychological outcomes that go against promoting PEB. Specifically, poorer childhood and economic uncertainty jointly increase the present orientation and decrease the sense of control [37, 38]. A strong present orientation and low sense of control discourage people from taking actions to save resources [39]. With the above in mind, the following moderation hypothesis was proposed:

Hypothesis: Environmental unpredictability will moderate the effect of ecological resource scarcity on PEB. Specifically, ecological resource scarcity information would play a less effective role in promoting PEB when environmental unpredictability is stronger.

Current research

In the current research, we conducted three studies to test our hypothesis on moderation. In Study 1, we examined whether perceived general environmental unpredictability would moderate the relationship between perceived resource scarcity and daily PEB frequencies. Study 2 took it a step further by manipulating resource scarcity to test whether the positive effect of ecological resource scarcity information on forest resource consumption intention would be weakened by individual-related environmental unpredictability, specifically the frequency of residential changes. Importantly, to increase ecological validity, Study 3 manipulated the information on both ecological resource scarcity and nature-related environmental unpredictability, and measured real water and paper consumption using a newly developed washing-hands paradigm.

Study 1

To examine the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability on the relationship between ecological resource scarcity and daily PEB frequency, we conducted a cross-sectional survey for Study 1. We hypothesised that ecological resource scarcity would predict higher frequencies of daily PEB for individuals who perceived the environment as predictable. However, we expected this positive association to diminish for individuals who perceived high levels of environmental unpredictability.

Participants

To ensure sufficient statistical power (80% power, α = .05) to detect a small-to-medium-sized effect for our moderation hypothesis, based on previous research in the same domain [10], we estimated that a sample size of 256 participants would be required using G*Power 3.1 [40]. Participants were recruited from a Chinese online survey platform (www.wjx.cn) and received monetary compensation for their participation. The survey platform utilized a voluntary opt-in panel, inviting users to complete the questionnaire. A total of 263 participants from China completed the survey. It is important to note that data collection was planned to conclude once 256 observations were collected within a three-week period.

The average age of the participants was 32.21 ± 7.11 years (ranging from 18 to 66 years), with 44.1% of them being male (N = 116). In terms of educational attainment, 1.9% held a middle-school degree or below, 1.9% had a high school degree, 8.7% held a junior college degree, 79.8% had a bachelor’s degree, and 7.6% had a master’s degree or higher. The average annual family income was 23.65 ± 21.04 ten thousand yuan.

Procedure and measures

To address the potential influence of priming participants’ perceived resource scarcity through items expressing the seriousness of resource scarcity [11, 41, 42], we carefully structured the data collection process. Firstly, we measured the dependent variable, PEB frequencies. Following this, participants completed the measure of perceived environmental unpredictability, and subsequently rated their perceived ecological resource scarcity. Additionally, to account for potential bias in self-reported PEB due to social desirability [43], we included a measurement of social desirability as a control variable. Finally, participants provided their demographic information, including age, gender, educational attainment, and annual personal income.

Perceived resource scarcity

The measurement of perceived ecological resource scarcity, consisting of 5 items, was adapted from a previous study conducted by Gu and her colleagues [10] (Cronbach’s α in the current study is 0.79). Participants were asked to indicate their level of agreement with statements such as “There are not enough resources for everyone in the place where I live” and “In the place where I live, I have already noticed some signs of resource scarcity.” Each item was rated on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The mean score of the entire scale was computed. Higher scores on this scale indicated higher levels of perceived ecological resource scarcity.

Perceived general environmental unpredictability

The item “For me, the environment we live in is unpredictable” developed by Reynolds and McCrea [44], was used to measure how participants perceived the general unpredictability of their environment. Participants rated this item on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Higher score indicated stronger perceived unpredictability.

Daily PEB frequency

Participants were asked to rate the frequency of PEB in their daily lives on a scale from 1 (never) to 5 (always). They were presented with six common resource conservation actions and asked to consider their behaviors in the year prior to the survey. The items are “do not turn the tap to the maximum when using water”, “switch off the lights when you leave”, “set the air conditioner’s temperature to 26–28 degrees centigrade in summer”, “buy and use energy-efficient appliances”, “avoid using disposable tableware whenever possible”. These six PEB were then converted into a PEB frequency scale, and a mean score was calculated for each participant. Higher scores indicated a higher frequency of PEB. Although the Cronbach’s α for the PEB scale was relatively low at .50, we decided to keep the measure because the items were face-valid. It is worth noting that removing any of the items did not improve the Cronbach’s alpha. Consistent with findings from previous studies, different types of PEB were not completely consistent [45, 46]. And importantly, using the common score derived from the six items did not significantly alter the results.

Social desirability

Social desirability was measured using the liar subscale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) [47]. This subscale consists of 12 items, with participants answering each question with a “Yes” or “No” response. A code of 1 was assigned to “Yes” and 0 to “No”. Higher scores on this subscale indicated a stronger tendency towards social desirability. The measure demonstrated good internal consistency with a Cronbach’s α of .75.

Results

Correlation analyses

Prior to conducting hypothesis testing, all variables exhibited normal distributions, as indicated by skewness values ranging from -0.89 to + 0.05 and kurtosis values ranging from -0.72 to + 0.77. We computed Pearson’s correlation coefficients to explore the relations among the studied variables (see Table 1 for descriptive statistics and intercorrelation coefficients). We found a marginally significant positive relationship between perceived ecological resource scarcity and PEB frequency (r = 0.12, p = .058). And there was no correlation between environmental unpredictability and PEB frequency (r = -0.07, p = .29). Importantly, as expected, social desirability was positively associated with PEB (r = 0.30, p < .001), indicating that it should be controlled for in subsequent analyses.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations between variables in study 1

Moderation analyses

To examine the impact of environmental unpredictability on the relationship between perceived ecological resource scarcity and PEB, we used the PROCESS macro for SPSS [48]. Controlling for social desirability, we found a significant interaction effect between perceived ecological resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability (b = -0.05, SE = 0.02, t = -2.26, p = .025, 95% CI = [-0.09, -0.01], ΔR2 = 0.018). To further understand this interaction, we conducted a floodlight analysis [49]. The results showed that perceived ecological resource scarcity was positively and significantly associated with PEB when environmental unpredictability was below 4.41 (b = 0.07, SE = 0.03, t = 1.97, p = .05, 95% CI = [0.000, 0.136]), but not when it was above 4.41.

Additionally, we performed a simple slope analysis to examine the relationship between perceived ecological resource scarcity and PEB for individuals with different levels of perceived environmental unpredictability with social desirability controlled (see Fig. 1). The results indicated that perceived ecological resource scarcity positively predicted PEB for individuals with lower levels of environmental unpredictability (-1 SD), b = 0.13, SE = 0.05, t = 2.83, p = .005, 95% CI = [0.039, 0.219]. However, this relationship was not significant for individuals with higher levels of environmental unpredictability (+ 1 SD), (b = -0.02, SE = 0.05, t = -0.35, p = 0.73, 95% CI = [-0.114, 0.079]).

Fig. 1
figure 1

The effect of resource scarcity on PEB at different levels of environmental unpredictability (Study 1)

Furthermore, controlling for demographic variables did not significantly change the results of moderation analysis. In summary, individuals who perceived the environment as more predictable were more likely to engage in PEB when facing ecological resource scarcity.

Brief discussion

Study 1 identified a moderating effect of environmental unpredictability on associations between perceived ecological resource scarcity and daily PEB. Individuals who perceived the environment as less unpredictable were more likely to adopt environmentally friendly ways to respond to ecological resource scarcity. However, it is important to consider the potential influence of responding to the PEB items on participants’ perceptions of ecological resource scarcity. The act of responding to these items may have directed participants’ attention towards environmental issues, potentially leading to an implicit increase in their perceived ecological resource scarcity. Therefore, it is not possible to infer the direction of the causal relationship between perceived ecological resource scarcity and PEB frequencies solely from correlational data. In addition, using a single item for measuring environmental unpredictability may raise concerns about the comprehensiveness of measurement. To address these limitations, we conducted Study 2, where we manipulated perceived ecological resource scarcity in order to demonstrate its causal effect, and further explore the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability by using another measurement.

Furthermore, it is important to note that the observed moderation effect size was small, which could be attributed to the fact that we measured various types of PEB in this study. According to the Goal System Theory, PEB can be motivated by multiple goals. In the context of resource scarcity, individuals who perceive the environment as more predictable are more likely to prioritize environmental protection for the benefit of future generations, especially if they themselves also stand to gain [50]. For instance, engaging in electricity-saving behaviors not only benefits the environment in the long run but also reduces personal electricity bills. In other words, personal benefits may matter. In our subsequent studies, we will focus on examining PEB that does not involve salient personal benefits in order to highlight the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability.

Study 2

In Study 2, we sought to replicate the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability on the link between ecological resource scarcity and PEB by manipulating resource scarcity information. We proposed that receiving ecological resource scarcity information would increase PEB intention for individuals with lower levels of environmental unpredictability but that the effect would disappear for individuals with higher levels of environmental unpredictability.

Participants

To test our moderation hypothesis, we determined that a sample size of 107 would be necessary to achieve 80% power (α = .05) in order to detect a small-to-medium-size effect (f2 = .075) based on previous research [10] using G*Power 3.1 [40]. We established the rule for ending data collection prior to gathering data, stipulating that the survey link would be closed after obtaining more than 150 observations. Ultimately, we recruited 155 Chinese adults who completed an anonymous online questionnaire and all of these responses were valid.

The participants had an average age of 32.91 ± 10.10 years (range = 18–59 years) and 41.90% of them were males (N = 65). In terms of educational attainment, 9.70% held a high school degree, 16.1% held a junior college degree, 66.6% held a bachelor’s degree, and 13.5% held a master’s degree or higher. The average annual personal income was 11.18 ± 44.58 ten thousand yuan.

Procedure and measures

In the present study, participants reported their demographic information first. Then, environmental unpredictability was measured. Next, participants were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions to read a news article, where exposure to the information of resource scarcity (vs. control condition) was the manipulated factor. Finally, PEB intention was measured using a forest management task.

Manipulation of ecological resource scarcity information

Participants were assigned at random to read one of two news articles. The articles were created specifically to manipulate perceptions of ecological resource scarcity. In the scarcity group (n = 77), participants read an article titled “Interpretation of China’s Resources through Big Data: Invisible Resource Scarcity in China”, which highlighted the severity of natural resource scarcity in China. In the control group (n = 78), participants read an article of similar length that aimed to evoke similar levels of negative arousal. This article was titled “Interpretation of Sleep through Big Data: Invisible Sleeping Problems in China” and discussed sleep issues in China. To ensure the credibility of the mock news articles, participants were informed that the articles were sourced from The People’s Daily, a reputable Chinese newspaper.

Immediately after reading their respective article, participants rated their perception of ecological resource scarcity using a 7-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” (1) to “strongly agree” (7). The item presented was: “Currently, I believe that we live in an environment where natural resources are extremely scarce.” Besides, participants also responded to one item on their mood at the moment for the manipulation check on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = “very negative” to 7 = “very positive”).

Environmental unpredictability

At the individual level, environmental unpredictability is mostly indicated by residential changes [24, 25]. The frequency of residential changes showed whether the structure of an environment one lives in changes over time, which is the important aspect of environmental unpredictability. Therefore, Study 2 used the frequency individuals moved in the past to represent their environmental unpredictability. Higher score indicates stronger environmental unpredictability (M = 3.59, SD = 2.17, Min = 0, Max = 11). The variable showed approximately normal distribution, with skewness = 0.64 and kurtosis = 0.65. Hence, the raw data of moving frequency are used for analysis.

PEB intention

A forest management task was used to measure PEB intention, specifically in relation to forest resource conservation intention [51]. Participants were asked to imagine that they were the owner of a timber company and must compete with three other companies to harvest timber in the same forest. They need to cut down as many trees as possible for their companies to profit and thrive. However, the rapid deforestation could lead to forest destruction. Then, participants were asked to answer one question about deforestation rate on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (very slow) to 7 (very fast), which asked, ‘How fast do you want your company to cut down trees?’ Additionally, they were asked one question about forest resource consumption, ranging from 1 to 100 acres, which asked, ‘How many acres of trees do you expect your company to cut down?’. Give that both questions indicate greedy for forest resources, the average of participants’ reversed standardized scores on the two questions was computed to represent PEB intention. Higher scores indicate stronger forest resource conservation intention. We also treated the two items separately to test our hypothesis, which can be found in the Additional file 1.

Results

Manipulation checks

The manipulation of resource scarcity information was successful. Specifically, participants in the scarcity condition (M = 5.17, SD = 1.25) compared to those in the control condition (M = 4.55, SD = 1.56), reported higher levels of awareness on ecological resource scarcity, t(153) = 2.72, p = .007, 95%CI = [0.169, 1.066], d = 0.44. Furthermore, there was no difference of mood between the two conditions (Mscarcity = 5.06, SDscarcity = 1.19; Mcontrol = 4.92, SDcontrol = 1.23), t(153) = 0.73, p > .05, 95%CI = [-0.526, 0.242].

Hypothesis test

To test for the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability, we regressed the forest resource conservation intention on ecological resource scarcity information (dummy coded: 1 = scarcity condition, 0 = control condition), environmental unpredictability and their interaction by employing the PROCESS macro (Model 1, 5000 bootstrap samples) for SPSS [48]. The results showed a significant main effect of ecological resource scarcity information (b = 0.63, SE = 0.23, t = 2.78, p = .006, 95%CI = [0.183, 1.078]). And there was no main effect of environmental unpredictability (b = 0.04, SE = .03, t = 1.23, p > .05, 95%CI = [-0.026, 0.109]).

Results showed a significant interaction effect (b = -0.14, SE = 0.06, t = -2.54, p = .012, 95%CI = [-0.025, -0.031], ΔR2 = .04), meaning that the effect of ecological resource scarcity information on forest resource conservation intention was moderated by environmental unpredictability. Specifically, for individuals with lower levels of environmental unpredictability (below 1 SD), participants in the scarcity condition exhibited stronger forest resource conservation intention relative to those in the control condition, b = 0.43, SE = 0.16, t = 2.63, p = .0095, 95% CI = [0.107, 0.755]. In contrast, for individuals with higher levels of environmental unpredictability (above 1 SD), the ecological resource scarcity manipulation had no effect on forest resource conservation intention, b = -0.17, SE = 0.17, t = -1.04, p > .05, 95% CI = [-0.512, 0.158] (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

The effect of resource scarcity × environmental unpredictability on forest resource conservation intention (Study 2)

Besides, a floodlight analysis was performed to decompose the interaction [49]. It revealed that ecological resource scarcity manipulation increased forest resource conservation intention for any value of environmental unpredictability less than 2.78 (b = 0.24, SE = 0.12, t = 1.98, p = .05, 95% CI = [0.000, 0.487]), but not for any value greater than 2.78. More importantly, the above findings did not significantly differ after controlling for demographic variables.

Brief discussion

Study 2 replicated results of Study 1 and identified that environmental unpredictability weakened the positive effect of ecological resource scarcity information on resource conservation. Presenting ecological resource scarcity information could effectively increase forest conservation intention, particularly for individuals who move less frequently, indicating lower levels of environmental unpredictability.

However, the results of Study 2 were limited in several aspects. First, environmental unpredictability can be caused either by individuals themselves, such as frequent relocation, or by nature, such as unforeseen natural disasters. The present study focused on individual-related environmental unpredictability only. Secondly, the measurement of resource conservation intention instead of actual behaviors may have restricted the ecological validity of the findings. Thirdly, it is possible that the moderation effect was underestimated. In the forest management task, the psychological experience of forest resource scarcity may have been primed in both conditions, as participants were informed about the need to compete with other companies for limited forest resources. Consequently, participants’ decisions may have been heavily influenced by the forest management scenario.

Study 3

Based on above discussions of Study 2, in Study 3, actual PEB was measured to increase ecological validity, and nature-caused environmental unpredictability was focused to improve generalizability. In addition, hypothetical forest resource conservation was also measured to replicate findings of Study 2. We proposed that receiving ecological resource scarcity information would increase actual resource conservation and forest resource conservation intention under predictable environmental conditions but that this effect would disappear under unpredictable environmental conditions.

Participants

We conducted a power analysis through G*Power 3.1 with the moderating effect size in Study 2, which suggested that a sample size of 135 would be required to achieve 80% power (α = .05) [40]. A total of 142 college students in Beijing, China was recruited to participate in the experiment in exchange for monetary compensation. Six participants who failed to finish all experimental tasks were excluded from data analysis. It is worth noting that the rule for terminating data collection was decided before data collection began: the experiment was terminated when more than 135 observations were collected in two weeks.

The average age of the participants was 21.87 ± 2.67 years (range = 17–29 years), and 75.00% of them were female (N = 102). The average annual household income was 12.37 ± 17.10 thousand yuan.

Research design and procedure

A 2 (water resource scarcity vs. control) × 2 (unpredictable vs. predictable environment) between-subject design was used.

Before arriving at the lab, participants were asked to fill out their demographic information in an online survey. Upon arrival at the lab, participants were randomly assigned into one of four groups to read a newspaper. These newspapers were designed to be looked like real Beijing Daily newspapers. In each type of newspaper, there were two pieces of news. One was designed to manipulate the water resource scarcity information, and another was designed to manipulate environmental unpredictability information. Then, actual water and paper consumption data was recorded in a washing-hands paradigm. Finally, forest resource consumption intention was measured.

Materials

Manipulation of water resource scarcity information

Similar to Study 2, in the scarcity condition (n = 67), the news article described the seriousness of water resource scarcity in Beijing. While, in the control condition (n = 69), the news article described Beijing residents’ sleep problems. After reading the article, participants responded to 1 item on perceived ecological resource scarcity on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = “strongly disagree” to 7 = “strongly agree”), which was adapted from new ecological paradigm scale (NEP): “The earth has plenty of natural resources if we just learn how to develop them” [52].

Manipulation of environmental unpredictability information

In the unpredictable condition (n = 68), the news article was titled “Natural Disasters are Unpredictable and Difficult to Prevent: 9.578 million People were Affected by Various Natural Disasters in January”. The news conveyed the information that natural disasters happened frequently, which caused many people to be affected in January, and there was no way to predict and prevent disasters. By contrast, in the predictable condition (n = 68), the news stated that even though natural disasters are frequent in China and many people were affected, now some devices can help predict and prevent disasters. The title was “Predication and Prevention of the Occurrence of Natural Disasters is Possible: 9.578 million People were Affected by Various Natural Disasters in January”.

Manipulation check items were rated right after reading the news article. Participants responded to 2 items about perceived unpredictability on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = “strongly disagree” to 7 = “strongly agree”): “The environment where I live is unstable”, and “The environment where I live is unpredictable”. The average score of the two items was computed such that a higher score indicated stronger perceived unpredictability.

Actual water and paper resource consumption

To cover up our real purpose, participants were told that the research was attempting to study palms, so that we would collect their fingerprints in the study. In the washing-hands paradigm, participants were asked to use the inkpad and leave their fingerprints on a sheet of white paper to study their palms. After that, they had to wash their hands in the lab. The amount of water and paper they used was recorded.

To measure the water consumption, the experimenter placed one measuring cylinder under the washbasin, and the measuring cylinder was linked to the washbasin’s outlet pipe. Importantly, participants could not see the cylinder. To measure their paper consumption, a bag of paper was placed on the washbasin for the participants to use. Besides, to exclude the experimenter effect, participants washed their hands without experimenter observation. Importantly, participants did not know that their behaviors were recorded, and participants were not aware of the real purpose of the study (see Fig. 3). All of the participants were debriefed at the end of the study.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Set-up of washing-hands paradigm

Considering that water and paper consumption for washing ink from hands might be affected by palm size, we recorded the palm area for each participant based on their fingerprints. Then, actual resource consumption was represented by average water consumption and average paper consumption, calculated by water or paper consumption divided by palm area.

Hypothetical forest resource conservation

Same as Study 2, the forest management task was used. After reading the scenario, participants were asked to answer the question, “How many acres of trees do you expect your company to cut down?”, ranging from 1 to 100 acres. A higher score on the measurement indicates a lower intention for forest resource conservation.

Results

Manipulation checks

Perceived resource scarcity was significantly greater in the scarcity condition (n = 67, M = 5.61, SD = 1.19) than that in the control condition (n = 69, M = 5.13, SD = 1.38), t(134) = 2.17, p = .032, 95%CI = [0.043, 0.920], d = 0.37. Perceived unpredictability was also significantly greater in the unpredictable condition (n = 68, M = 5.13, SD = 1.28) compared to the predictable condition (n = 68, M = 4.55, SD = 1.39), t(134) = 2.51, p = .013, 95%CI = [0.121, 1.026], d = 0.43. Overall, the manipulations were successful and valid.

Hypothesis test

To examine the interaction effect between water resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability on resource conservation, two-factor MANOVAs were conducted.

Concerning the average water consumption, gender, age, household income, and cleanliness habits are included as control variables. The findings revealed that the main effect of scarcity was significant (F(1,128) = 5.44, p = 0.021, 95%CI = [-14.168, -0.673], ηp2 = .041), and the main effect of environmental unpredictability was not significant (F(1,128) = 0.23, p > .05, 95%CI = [-7.437, 5.984]). As expected, the interaction was significant (F(1,128) = 6.81, p = .01, 95%CI = [3.037, 22.097], ηp2 = .050). Then, simple effect analysis revealed that under the predictable condition, the average water consumption was significantly less under the scarcity condition (M = 25.29, SD = 13.87) than under the control condition (M = 37.13, SD = 13.91), F(1,128) = 12.25, p < .001, 95%CI = [-18.535, -5.146], ηp2 = .087. However, under the unpredictable condition, there was no significant difference between the scarcity condition (M = 32.71, SD = 13.93) and control condition (M = 31.98, SD = 13.90), F(1,128) = 0.05, p > .05, 95%CI = [-5.984, 7.437] (see Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Average water consumption as a function of resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability manipulations (Study 3)

Moreover, the results in average paper consumption showed a similar pattern. Main effects of scarcity (F(1,128) = 0.42, p > .05, 95%CI = [-0.137, 0.042]) and environmental unpredictability (F(1,128) = 0.70, p > .05, 95%CI = [-0.143, 0.036]) were not significant. A significant interaction effect was detected, F(1,128) = 5.30, p = .023, 95%CI = [0.021, 0.275], ηp2 = .040. As predicted, in the predictable condition, paper consumption in the scarcity condition (M = 0.28, SD = 0.18) was significantly less than in the control condition (M = 0.38, SD = 0.18), F(1,128) = 4.39, p = .038, 95%CI = [-0.184, -0.005], ηp2 = .033. No significant difference in paper consumption were observed between scarcity condition (M = 0.33, SD = 0.19) and control condition (M = 0.28, SD = 0.19) in unpredictable condition, F(1,128) = 1.39, p > .05, 95%CI = [-0.036, 0.143] (see Fig. 5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Average paper consumption as a function of resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability manipulations (Study 3)

More importantly, the above findings did not significantly differ without control variables in data analysis, and also did not significantly differ using the raw scores of water and paper consumption. Detailed results can be found in the Additional file 1.

Hypothetical forest resource consumption was log transformed as it showed non-normal distribution. The findings showed that the main effects of scarcity (F(1,130) = 1.800, p > .05, 95%CI = [-0.363, 0.271]) and environmental unpredictability (F(1,130) = 2.189, p > .05, 95%CI = [-0.688, 0.049]) were not significant. A marginally significant interaction effect was detected, F(1, 130) = 3.04, p = .084, 95%CI = [-0.053, 0.849], ηp2 = .023. As predicted, in the predictable condition, forest resource consumption in the scarcity condition (Mraw = 30.76, SDraw = 14.97) was significantly less than the control condition (Mraw = 40.74, SDraw = 25.41), F(1,130) = 4.71, p = .032, 95%CI = [-0.672, -0.031], ηp2 = .035. No significant difference of forest resource consumption was observed between scarcity condition (Mraw = 42.15, SDraw = 20.41) and control condition (Mraw = 44.00, SDraw = 28.17) in unpredictable condition, F(1,130) = 0.08, p > .05, 95%CI = [-0.027, 0.363] (see Fig. 6).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Forest resource consumption as a function of resource scarcity and environmental unpredictability manipulations (Study 3)

Brief discussion

As expected, Study 3 replicated the findings of the previous two studies. We identified a moderating effect of nature-caused environmental unpredictability on ecological resource scarcity information’s effect on actual PEB. Specifically, individuals who received lower levels of environmental unpredictability information exhibited more water-saving and paper-saving behaviors, and were inclined to harvest fewer forest resources in the face of water scarcity. Interestingly, even though our manipulation focused solely on water scarcity, both paper consumption and forest resource consumption were affected as well, despite their lack of direct association with water. These results highlight the robust influence of resource scarcity information and environmental unpredictability on PEB, thereby enhancing the ecological validity of our findings.

General discussion

Focusing on the global issue of environmental unpredictability, the current research explored when does showing resource scarcity information promote PEB. In Study 1, a cross-sectional study, we discovered that resource scarcity information effectively enhances PEB, but only when individuals perceive the environment as less unpredictable. Furthermore, by manipulating scarcity information, Study 2 revealed that only for individuals with lower levels of environmental unpredictability could presenting ecological resource scarcity information decrease forest resource consumption intention. Moreover, an experiment with high ecological validity was conducted in Study 3 and found that the negative effect of water resource scarcity information on actual water and paper saving behaviors, as well as hypothetical forest resource consumption emerged only for people who receiving weaker environmental unpredictability information.

Theoretical contribution and practical implication

Environmental unpredictability is an important concept in life history theory. Numerous studies have verified that childhood environmental unpredictability plays a crucial role in shaping life history strategies [27, 28, 30, 37, 53]. However, little is known about how adulthood environmental unpredictability functions. The current research provided preliminary evidence that unpredictability in adulthood can also function in shaping behaviors. Adulthood unpredictability, including both individual- and nature-related environmental unpredictability, demotivates individuals to sacrifice present interests for future environmental benefits when facing scarcity.

Some psychological factors, including those discussed earlier (such as short-sighted tendency, fear of free riders, and perceived lack of control), as well as self-interest and competitive orientation, can serve as potential mechanisms underlying the moderating effect of environmental unpredictability. Self-interest and competitive orientation are important ways for individuals to survive in a harsh environment. Individuals may adopt a competitive orientation to obtain more benefits for themselves to survive during periods of scarcity. In addition, they may also seek to weaken others’ interests. These factors have been identified as “Stone Age” psychological biases leading to environment destruction [54]. To better respond to ecological resource scarcity, the current research demonstrated the importance of creating a predictable and peaceful world by removing the psychological barriers to mitigate ecological resource scarcity.

The IMB model provides a comprehensive framework advancing resource conservation research and intervention implements [3]. Even though the IMB model captures three vital components, information, motivation, and behavioral skills on behavior change, the psychological barriers caused by environmental unpredictability were ignored. As illustrated in a recent meta-analysis [15], compared with the control group, of the 38 interventions including IMB components, water use was reduced by only 5.9% in average with a small effect size, and the magnitude of effect varied widely in different interventions. According to the findings in the current research, levels of environmental unpredictability may be the underlying reason for the varied efficacy. Therefore, to best strengthen reducing resource consumption interventions based on the IMB model, it’s necessary to take environmental unpredictability into consideration.

Importantly, the current research developed a new paradigm, washing-hands paradigm, to measure actual resource consumption in the lab. As illustrated in previous studies, there are gaps between self-reported behaviors, and objective behaviors [43]. However, over 80% of recent studies only relied on self-reported data [55]. The washing-hands paradigm sets up a situation to capture actual water and paper resource consumption data. Importantly, the confounding variables can be controlled in the paradigm, such as habits, individual difference on palm size, and social desirability. This paradigm can help to establish causality and improve ecological validity of lab experiments, advancing resource conservation research.

The current research also provides some vital practical implications for both policy makers and environmental organizations. Our data suggested that creating a predictable environment can help promote resource conservation when facing ecological resource scarcity information. Governments should try to eliminate unpredictable factors. However, some unpredictable factors are difficult to address, such as natural disasters and virus spread. In such conditions, individual-level practices appear to be more important. For countries with a predictable environment, the strategy of the reminders of the ecological resource scarcity information is effective. However, for countries with an unpredictable environment, governments and organizations can consider using public media to decrease residents’ perceived unpredictability. Moreover, inspired by our Study 2, emphasizing predictable environmental information when reminding residents of scarcity should be encouraged. Environmental organizations should provide information that the environment is predictable when calling for resource conservation to respond to scarcity.

Limitations and future directions

The current research faces the limitation that the measurement in the correlation study is restricted due to the use of only one item to measure the moderator, and the alpha level of the PEB measure is low. For future studies, one aspect to consider is the exploration potential mechanisms of the moderation hypothesis. The current research did not delve into psychological mechanisms. It is suggested that future research could investigate underlying potential mechanisms of the moderation hypothesis to enrich the framework. Another related issue pertains to the IMB model. In the current research, we mainly focused on the effectiveness of scarcity information component but didn’t include motivation and behavioral skills components. It’s worthy for future research to test if creating a predictable environment can still strengthen the effect of IMB intervention. Besides, there are various types of resource conservation behaviors that individuals can engage in. Importantly, different behaviors are not necessarily highly relevant. For example, factors predicting shutting down electronics at night could not predict upgrading to energy-efficient appliances because these behaviors may cluster into distinct dimensions [56, 57]. In the current research, we may not be able to generalize our findings to other types of behaviors. Thus, future research is encouraged to investigate whether the moderation hypothesis can be verified in other types of resource conservation behaviors.

Conclusion

Across three studies, we provided evidence to support the moderation hypothesis that environmental unpredictability weakens the positive effect of ecological resource scarcity information on PEB. Moving forward, it would be valuable to delve deeper into the underlying mechanisms, examine the moderation effect across various types of PEB, and investigate its potential application in PEB interventions.

Availability of data and materials

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

References

  1. World Bank. World development indicators 2016. Washington, DC: World Bank; 2016.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Syme GJ, Nancarrow BE, Seligman C. The evaluation of information campaigns to promote voluntary household water conservation. Eval Rev. 2000;24(6):539–78. https://doi.org/10.1177/0193841x0002400601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Fisher WA, Fisher JD, Shuper PA. Social psychology and the fight against AIDS: an information-motivation-behavioral skills model for the prediction and promotion of health behavior. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2014;50:105–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-800284-1.00003-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Famiglietti JS. The global groundwater crisis. Nat Clim Chang. 2014;4(11):945–8. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate2425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Fisher JD, Fisher WA, Amico KR, Harman J. An information-motivation- behavioral skills model of adherence to antiretroviral therapy. Health Psychol. 2006;25(4):462–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.25.4.462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Steg L, Perlaviciute G, van der Werff E, Lurvink J. The significance of hedonic values for environmentally relevant attitudes, preferences, and actions. Environ Behav. 2014;46(2):163–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916512454730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Ehret PJ, Hodges HE, Kuehl C, Brick C, Mueller S, Anderson SE. Systematic review of household water conservation interventions using the information–motivation–behavioral skills model. Environ Behav. 2020;53(5):001391651989686. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916519896868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Hannibal B, Sansom L, E. Portney K. The effect of local water scarcity and drought on water conservation behaviors. Environ Sociol. 2019;5(3):294–307. https://doi.org/10.1080/23251042.2018.1519882.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Rodriguez-Sanchez C, Sarabia-Sanchez FJ. Does water context matter in water conservation decision behaviour? Sustainability. 2020;12(7):3026. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12073026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Gu D, Jiang J, Zhang Y, Sun Y, Jiang W, Du X. Concern for the future and saving the earth: when does ecological resource scarcity promote pro-environmental behavior? J Environ Psychol. 2020;72(101501):101501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Berthold A, Cologna V, Siegrist M. The influence of scarcity perception on people’s pro-environmental behavior and their readiness to accept new sustainable technologies. Ecol Econ. 2022;196(107399):107399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2022.107399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Katz D, Grinstein A, Kronrod A, Nisan U. Evaluating the effectiveness of a water conservation campaign: combining experimental and field methods. J Environ Manage. 2016;180:335–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvman.2016.05.049.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. March H, Domènech L, Saurí D. Water conservation campaigns and citizen perceptions: the drought of 2007–2008 in the Metropolitan Area of Barcelona. Nat Hazards (Dordr). 2013;65(3):1951–66. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-012-0456-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Quesnel KJ, Ajami NK. Changes in water consumption linked to heavy news media coverage of extreme climatic events. Sci Adv. 2017;3(10):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.1700784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Ehret PJ, Hodges HE, Kuehl C, Brick C, Mueller S, Anderson SE. Systematic review of household water conservation interventions using the information–motivation–behavioral skills model. Environ Behav. 2021;53(5):485–519. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916519896868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Inman D, Jeffrey P. A review of residential water conservation tool performance and influences on implementation effectiveness. Urban Water J. 2006;3(3):127–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/15730620600961288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Fatmawati I, Dharmmesta BS, Purwanto BM, Nugroho SS. Promoting young adults to perform energy saving behavior through message framing: a lesson learned from Indonesia. Acad Strateg Manag J. 2018;17(5):1–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Kimbrough EO, Vostroknutov A. The social and ecological determinants of common pool resource sustainability. J Environ Econ Manag. 2015;72(C):38–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jeem.2015.04.004l.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Belsky J, Schlomer GL, Ellis BJ. Beyond cumulative risk: distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of parenting and early life history strategy. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(3):662–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024454.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Charnov EL. Life history invariants: some explorations of symmetry in evolutionary ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1993.

  21. Roff DA, ed. Evolution of life histories: theory and analysis. 1993rd ed. NY: Chapman and Hall; 1993.

  22. Roff DA. Life history evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002.

  23. Stearns SC. The evolution of life histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1992.

  24. Ellis BJ, Figueredo AJ, Brumbach BH, Schlomer GL. Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: the impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Hum Nat. 2009;20(2):204–68. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9063-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Young ES, Frankenhuis WE, Ellis BJ. Theory and measurement of environmental unpredictability. Evol Hum Behav. 2020;41(6):550–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.08.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Simpson JA, Griskevicius V, Kuo SIC, Sung S, Collins WA. Evolution, stress, and sensitive periods: the influence of unpredictability in early versus late childhood on sex and risky behavior. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(3):674–86. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Chen BB, Qu W. Life history strategies and procrastination: the role of environmental unpredictability. Pers Individ Dif. 2017;117:23–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.05.036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Chen BB, Shi Z, Sun S. Life history strategy as a mediator between childhood environmental unpredictability and adulthood personality. Pers Individ Dif. 2017;111:215–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Frankenhuis WE, Panchanathan K, Nettle D. Cognition in harsh and unpredictable environments. Curr Opin Psychol. 2016;7:76–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Hill EM, Jenkins J, Farmer L. Family unpredictability, future discounting, and risk taking. J Socio Econ. 2008;37(4):1381–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2006.12.081.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Zhang Y, Gao Y, Jiang J. An unpredictable environment reduces pro-environmental behavior: a dynamic public goods experiment on forest use. J Environ Psychol. 2021;78(101702):101702. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Brumbach BH, Figueredo AJ, Ellis BJ. Effects of harsh and unpredictable environments in adolescence on development of life history strategies: a longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. Hum Nat. 2009;20(1):25–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9059-3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Ajzen I, Fishbein M. The influence of attitudes on behavior. In: Albarracín D, ed. The handbook of attitudes, Vol 826. Mahwah: Psychology Press; 2005. p. 173–221.

  34. Wit A, Wilke H. Public good provision under environmental and social uncertainty. Eur J Soc Psychol. 1998;28(2):249–56. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199803/04)28:2%3C249::AID-EJSP868%3E3.0.CO;2-J.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Zhu N, Hawk ST, Chang L. Unpredictable and competitive cues affect prosocial behaviors and judgments. Pers Individ Dif. 2019;138:203–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.10.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Tam KP, Chan HW. Generalized trust narrows the gap between environmental concern and pro-environmental behavior: Multilevel evidence. Glob Environ Change. 2018;48:182–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2017.12.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Griskevicius V, Ackerman JM, Cantú SM, et al. When the economy falters, do people spend or save? Responses to resource scarcity depend on childhood environments. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(2):197–205. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612451471.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Mittal C, Griskevicius V. Sense of control under uncertainty depends on people’s childhood environment: a life history theory approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2014;107(4):621–37. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Gifford R. The dragons of inaction: psychological barriers that limit climate change mitigation and adaptation. Am Psychol. 2011;66(4):290–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023566.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–60. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Lee AJ, Zietsch BP. Experimental evidence that women’s mate preferences are directly influenced by cues of pathogen prevalence and resource scarcity. Biol Lett. 2011;7(6):892–5. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2011.0454.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Watkins CD, DeBruine LM, Little AC, Feinberg DR, Jones BC. Priming concerns about pathogen threat versus resource scarcity: dissociable effects on women’s perceptions of men’s attractiveness and dominance. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2012;66(12):1549–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-012-1408-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Kormos C, Gifford R. The validity of self-report measures of proenvironmental behavior: a meta-analytic review. J Environ Psychol. 2014;40:359–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.09.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Reynolds JJ, McCrea SM. Life history theory and exploitative strategies. Evol Psychol. 2016;14(3):1–16. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704916659483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Barr S. Factors influencing environmental attitudes and behaviors: a U.k. case study of household waste management. Environ Behav. 2007;39(4):435–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916505283421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Bratt C, Stern PC, Matthies E, Nenseth V. Home, car use, and vacation: the structure of environmentally significant individual behavior. Environ Behav. 2015;47(4):436–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/001391651452503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Eysenck H, Eysenck SB. Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire : (Epq-R Adult). CA: EdITS/Educational and Industrial Testing Service; 1994.

  48. Hancock GR, Mueller RO, eds. Structural equation modeling: a second course. 2nd ed. Charlotte: Information Age Publishing; 2013.

  49. Spiller SA, Fitzsimons GJ, Lynch JG Jr, Mcclelland GH. Spotlights, floodlights, and the magic number zero: simple effects tests in moderated regression. J Mark Res. 2013;50(2):277–88. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.12.0420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Kruglanski AW, Shah JY, Fishbach A, Friedman R, Chun WY, Sleeth-Keppler D. A theory of goal systems. In: Advances in experimental social psychology. NY: Elsevier; 2002. p. 331–378. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80008-9.

  51. Wang L, Gu D, Jiang J, Sun Y. The not-so-dark side of materialism: can public versus private contexts make materialists less Eco-unfriendly? Front Psychol. 2019;10:1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00790.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Dunlap RE, Van Liere KD, Mertig AG, Jones RE. New trends in measuring environmental attitudes: measuring endorsement of the new ecological paradigm: a revised NEP scale. J Soc Issues. 2000;56(3):425–42. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. White AE, Li YJ, Griskevicius V, Neuberg SL, Kenrick DT. Putting all your eggs in one basket: life-history strategies, bet hedging, and diversification. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(5):715–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612461919.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. van Vugt M, Griskevicius V, Schultz PW. Naturally green: harnessing stone age psychological biases to foster environmental behavior. Soc Issues Policy Rev. 2014;8(1):1–32. https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Lange F, Steinke A, Dewitte S. The pro-environmental behavior task: a laboratory measure of actual pro-environmental behavior. J Environ Psychol. 2018;56:46–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.02.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Karlin B, Davis N, Sanguinetti A, Gamble K, Kirkby D, Stokols D. Dimensions of conservation: exploring differences among energy behaviors. Environ Behav. 2014;46(4):423–52. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916512467532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Nair G, Gustavsson L, Mahapatra K. Factors influencing energy efficiency investments in existing Swedish residential buildings. Energy Policy. 2010;38(6):2956–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2010.01.033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The authors acknowledge the financial support from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31871126), Chongqing Normal University (23XWB043) and Social Science Fund of Chongqing, China (2023BS076).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

GD and JJ contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation, data collection and analysis were performed by GD. GD and JJ contributed to the interpretation of data. The first draft of the manuscript was written by GD. JJ commented on previous versions of the manuscript or revised it critically for important intellectual content. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jiang Jiang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was conducted after getting approval from the Institutional Review Board of the Beijing Normal University and was conducted following the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations of Beijing Normal University. Participants were informed that their names and institution names would be kept confidential and their privacy rights were protected. Participants were included in the process on a voluntary basis and informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gu, D., Jiang, J. Navigating an unpredictable environment: the moderating role of perceived environmental unpredictability in the effectiveness of ecological resource scarcity information on pro-environmental behavior. BMC Psychol 12, 261 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01762-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01762-1

Keywords