Skip to main content

The effect of justice sensitivity on malevolent creativity: the mediating role of anger and the moderating role of emotion regulation strategies

Abstract

Background

the AMORAL model emphasizes the close connection of individuals’ belief system and malevolent creativity. Belief in a just world theory (BJW) states that people have a basic need to believe that the world they live in is just, and everyone gets what they deserve. Therefore, justice matters to all people. Justice sensitivity, as one of individual trait, has been found associated with negative goals. However, relevant studies have not tested whether justice sensitivity can affect malevolent creativity and its psychological mechanisms. Additionally, researchers have found that both anger and emotion regulation linked with justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity, but their contribution to the relationship between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity remained unclear. The current study aims to explore the influence of justice sensitivity on malevolent creativity, the mediating effect of trait anger/state anger on the relationship between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity, and the moderating effect of emotion regulation on this mediating effect.

Methods

A moderated mediating model was constructed to test the relationship between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity. A sample of 395 Chinese college students were enrolled to complete the questionnaire survey.

Results

Justice sensitivity positively correlated with malevolent creativity, both trait anger and state anger partly mediated the connection between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity. Moreover, emotion regulation moderated the indirect effect of the mediation model. The indirect effect of justice sensitivity on malevolent creativity through trait anger/state anger increased as the level of emotion regulation increased. The results indicated that justice sensitivity can affect malevolent creativity directly and indirectly through the anger. The level of emotion regulation differentiated the indirect paths of justice sensitivity on malevolent creativity.

Conclusions

Justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity was mediated by trait anger/state anger. The higher sensitivity to justice, the higher level of trait anger/state anger, which in turn boosted the tendency of malevolent creativity. This indirect connection was moderated by emotion regulation, individuals with high emotion regulation are better able to buffer anger from justice sensitivity.

Peer Review reports

Background

Creativity is generally believed that it is beneficial and positive. Researchers pointed out that creativity refers to the ability to generate novel and appropriate ideas or products in a specific environment [1]. However, creativity is not always positive. People also have some novel but negative ideas and behaviors—malevolent creativity, which refers to original and premeditated ideation deliberately performed in order to realize one’s own goals and desires, and it always leads to negative consequences, such as new types of fraud, murder, etc [2]. A wide variety of malevolent creativity instances can be found everywhere and cause damage in original or innovative ways, and it is hard to detect and prevent [3]. Therefore, it is of great social significance to reveal the influence factors of malevolent creativity and explore the effective regulation strategies to reduce the potential harm.

The AMORAL model emphasized individuals’ belief system, or their interconnected set of beliefs helped determine whether and to what extent they engage in malevolent creativity. Moreover, the drivers of malevolent creativity also included the need to align actions with belief systems [4]. At the same time, belief in a just world theory (BJW) stated that people had a basic need to believe that the world they lived in is just, and everyone got what they deserved [5, 6]. Researchers had found that individuals were more frequently exhibit malevolent creativity in hostile, angry, injustice, and vengeful situation [7]. Meanwhile, social exchange theory stated that justice was the basis of social exchange and an essential element of effective social interaction. Injustice in an organization or group was a source of stress for its members which were provoked into negative emotions and even outright antisocial aggression by differential treatment [8]. Therefore, the feeling of injustice may matter to malevolent creativity.

A variety of studies examining the distributive, interpersonal, and procedural justice showed that it was perceived justice, not objective circumstances, shaped responses to injustice [9]. Justice sensitivity is an individual trait, which is reflected in the difficulty of detecting injustices and the intensity of the response to injustices. Individuals with high justice sensitivity are more likely to perceive injustice than those with low justice sensitivity [10]. Schmitt et al. categorized justice sensitivity into four types: victim sensitivity, observer sensitivity, beneficiary sensitivity and perpetrator sensitivity [11]. Mohiyeddini and Schmitt found that justice sensitivity performed better than other variables (e.g., trait anger, anger out, and self-assertiveness) in predicting reactions to unfair treatment [12]. There was a study found that individuals tend to establish negative goals when they encountered unfair situations, which may lead to the emergence of malevolent creativity [13]. Another studies also showed that justice sensitivity closely positively correlated kinds of externalizing problems, such as relational, proactive, and reactive aggression in adults [14] and peer victimization [15]. What’s more, Gollwitzer et al. found victim sensitivity was associated negatively with prosocial behavior and positively with antisocial behavior [16]. Prior studies verified that people who have encountered an injustice situation would show more malevolent creativity. However, it remains unknown whether justice sensitivity can affect malevolent creativity and how it affects malevolent creativity. Therefore, the current study focused on the influence of justice sensitivity on malevolent creativity and explored the underlying mechanisms.

Anger is a basic emotional state, according to State-Trait Anger theory, which can be divided into state anger and trait anger [17]. State anger is a temporary emotional state which composed of subjective feelings and physiological activities. On the other hand, trait anger is defined as a stable personality characteristic, a general tendency of angry reaction under the induced stimulus, and a relatively stable individual difference in frequency, intensity and duration of state anger [18]. High-trait angry individuals are more inflamed and easily develop state anger, then show more maladaptive cope including verbal and physical confrontation [19, 20].

Equity theory stated that negative emotions such as anger and resentment were aroused when individuals realized they had been treated unfairly [21]. Social psychological researches indicated that anger was the predominant emotional response to perceiving injustice [22, 23]. A number of empirical studies also examined the relationship between anger and injustice, and indicated that the level of anger was higher when individuals perceived injustice or had been treated unfairly [24,25,26,27]. Furthermore, researches showed that facets of anger (i.e., state, trait, expression, inhibition) linked with perceived injustice [28, 29]. Schmitt et al. also found justice sensitivity related with trait anger [30]. Additionally, individuals with high justice sensitivity may be more likely to have a stronger reaction when they accounted injustice events, which might in turn produce a higher degree of state anger.

It has been shown that feeling unfair treatment can give people a sense of relative deprivation [31], which lead to anger and criminal behavior [10, 32, 33]. Anger was an emotion with high arousal and approach orientation which could reinforce cognitive activation state, and allowed the person to mobilize more adequate cognitive resources to engage in the current cognitive activity (e.g., creative thinking). Therefore, anger could facilitate creative performance [34,35,36,37]. Cheng et al. conducted an experimental study with the malevolent creativity task (MCT) and found that malevolent creativity performance can be significantly promoted in anger group [38]. Therefore, anger may be a potential mediating variable between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity.

Previous studies explored induced anger emotion in the laboratory, but few researches examined the relationship between anger and malevolent creativity under natural conditions. There was a study shown that trait anger could significantly and positively predicted aggression [39]. And other study also found that state anger could influence an individual’s tendency to aggression through anger rumination [40]. Thus, the current study speculated that both trait anger and state anger may influence the tendency to malevolent creativity. Additionally, whether trait anger and state anger play a different role between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity is still unknown. Therefore, both state anger and trait anger deserve attention. Considering the differences between the two kinds of anger, the current study separately examined their roles between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity.

However, in realistic situations, justice-sensitive individuals do not always produce extreme anger emotion and generate tendency to malevolent creative behavior when they faced with injustice events. This may closely rely on the regulation and control of emotion production, perception, and expression.

Emotion regulation, composed of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression, is defined as a series of cognitive processes adjusting or changing the appearance, intensity and duration of emotion [41]. Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) approaches proved that angry emotion can be best downregulated by those emotion regulation strategies, such as modifying negative thoughts, or reappraising the anger-provoking situation [42]. Furthermore, according to Gross’s process model of emotion regulation, strategies that act early in the emotion-generative process might differ from the later one in consequences [43]. Cognitive reappraisal is an antecedent-focused strategy used before an emotion occurs, individual can change the emotional experience by altering the perception of a negative event. Expressive suppression, on the other hand, refers to an individual’s ability to alter the external manifestation of emotion by inhibiting expression. That is to say, both can work in the early stages of emotion production. Researchers examined the effects of emotion regulation strategies on both trait anger and state anger, andresults showed that both cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression can counteract short-term anger arousal following provocation [42]. Numerous studies showed that high level emotion regulation could effectively down-regulate an individual’s anger mood and the related physiological responses [42, 44,45,46]. Cheng et al. also found that cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression could effectively reduce the emotional arousal and significantly reduce the malevolent creativity of angry individuals [38]. Based on previous findings, it is reasonable to expect that cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression may also attenuate the possible effects of justice sensitivity on anger and then weak the impact on malevolent creativity. Therefore, the current study hypothesizes that emotion regulation can play a moderating role between justice sensitivity and anger.

In conclusion, the aims of the present study were: (1) to reveal the influence of justice sensitivity on malevolent creativity; (2) to investigate whether trait anger as well as state anger played mediating role in the association between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity; (3) to explore whether emotion regulation moderated the correlation between justice sensitivity and anger. The hypothetical moderated mediation model was shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The hypothetical moderated mediation model

Methods

Participants

Prior to the beginning of the study, we used the G*Power 3.1. provided by Faul et al. to estimate the required sample size [47]. With setting the medium effect size f2 = 0.15, α = 0.05, 95% power (1-β err probability), and the number of predictors = 7, the total sample size was 153.

505 college students participated in this study and volunteered for an online survey on the website. A total of 395 valid questionnaires were collected for the study, out of those, 229 (58%) were from male students and 166 (42%) from female students. The major of participants included science and engineering (23%), medicine (25%), literature and history (22%), arts and sports (22%), and others (15%). 347 (88%) were undergraduates, 25 (6%) were postgraduates and 23 (6%) were others.

Measures

Justice sensitivity inventory (JSI)

The Justice Sensitivity Inventory developed by Schmitt was used to measure justice sensitivity [11, 48, 49]. Previous studies had shown that the scale had good reliability and was widely used [10, 50]. This scale consisted of four subscales: victim sensitivity, observer sensitivity, beneficiary sensitivity, and perpetrator sensitivity. Each subscale consisted of 10 questions and was scored on a 6-point Likert scale (e.g. I cannot easily bear it when others profit unilaterally from me.). This scale was scored from strongly disagree to strongly agree as 1–6. The JSI score is the sum of all the item scores. Higher scores indicated higher justice sensitivity. Cronbach’s α for justice sensitivity in this study was 0.97.

Trait anger scale (TAS)

Trait anger was measured by the Chinese version of the Trait Anger Scale [20, 51], which consisted of 10 items (e.g. I’m easily irritated.). Studies showed that the scale had good reliability and validity, and widely used in China [52, 53]. This scale was scored on a 4-point Likert scale, and the higher total score indicated higher levels of trait anger. The Cronbach’s α for this scale in this study was 0.87.

State anger scale (SAS)

State Anger Scale was developed by Spielberger and revised into Chinese version by Liu [54,55,56]. The scale had been widely used in China [57]. This scale consisted of 15 items (e.g. I’m angry.) and included three subscales: anger feelings, anger words, and anger actions. This scale was scored on a 4-point scale, with 1 (not at all), 2 (a little), 3 (moderately), and 4 (very strongly). The higher score, the more pronounced state anger. Cronbach’s α for this scale in this study was 0.92.

Emotion regulation questionnaire (ERQ)

Emotion regulation was evaluated by a 10-item self-report version of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ). The scale was developed by Gross and revised into Chinese version by Wang et al. [58, 59]. The Chinese version of ERQ had good construct validity, retest reliability, and internal consistency reliability [60, 61]. ERQ was consisted of two subscales, including 6 items (e.g. I control my emotions by changing the way I think about the situation I’m in.) for cognitive reappraisal and 4 items for expressive suppression (e.g. I don’t show my emotions.). This scale was rated on a 7-point. The higher total score indicated more frequent use of emotion regulation strategies. Cronbach’s α for this scale in this study was 0.72.

Malevolent creativity behavior scale (MCBS)

Malevolent creativity was measured by MCBS, which developed by Hao et al. and could be used to measure the tendency of individuals to exhibit malevolent creativity behaviors in their daily lives [62]. The scale had a good ecological validity, covered various forms of malevolent creativity (e.g., deception, tricks, lies), and was easy to administer [62]. This scale consisted of 13 items and was scored on a 5-point scale, with 1 (not at all)  5 (always) (e.g., When I am treated unfairly, I will retaliate in a different way). The scores of all items were summed to obtain the total score. The higher total score indicated that the individual showed more malevolent creativity in daily life. Cronbach’s α for this scale in this study was 0.92.

Analysis

The descriptive statistical analysis and correlation analysis were conducted using SPSS 26.0. Regression analyses were used to test the mediating role of trait anger / state anger between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity. PROCESS 3.3 was used to test the moderating role of emotion regulation. The demographic variables (gender, major and grade) were entered in the model as covariates.

Results

Common method bias assessment

Harman’s single-factor test was used for exploring the common method bias of the data. All of items of JSI, TAS, SAS, ERQ and MCBS were put into the un-rotated exploratory factor analysis. The results showed that the number of factors with an eigenvalue greater than 1.00 was 17, and the explained variance of the first factor was 28.85, which was lower than the critical criterion of 40% [63]. The results indicated that there was no obvious common method bias in the data of this study.

Descriptive statistical and correlational analysis

As shown in Table 1, all of the variables were significantly correlated with each other. The score of JSI was significantly positively correlated with the score of TAS, SAS and MCBS, and were significantly negatively correlated with the score of ERQ. Both TAS and SAS were significantly negatively correlated with ERQ and positively correlated with MCBS.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and correlations among study variables (n = 395)

Analysis of the mediating role of trait anger and state anger

Regression analysis was used to test the mediating role of trait anger and state anger between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity [64, 65].

Trait anger as the mediator

Three regression models were constructed to test the mediating role of trait anger. Firstly, malevolent creativity entered the model as a dependent variable, then demographic variables (gender, major and grade) entered the first block as control variables, and justice sensitivity entered the equation as predictor variable. Secondly, trait anger entered the model as a dependent variable, then demographic variables (gender, major and grade) entered the first block as control variables, and justice sensitivity entered the second block as predictor variable. Finally, malevolent creativity entered the model as a dependent variable, then demographic variables (gender, major and grade) entered the first block as control variables, and justice sensitivity and trait anger entered the equation as predictor variables. The results were shown in Table 2. It showed that justice sensitivity significantly positively predicted malevolent creativity (c = 0.345, t = 7.262, p < 0.001) and trait anger (a = 0.342, t = 7.392, p < 0.001), while trait anger significantly and positively predicted malevolent creativity (b = 0.458, t = 9.839, p < 0.001). Additionally, the direct effect (path c’) of justice sensitivity on malevolent creativity was statistically significant (c’ = 0.188, p < 0.001). Therefore, the mediation model was confirmed, which indicated that the relationship between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity was partially mediated by trait anger. The indirect effect was 0.157 (95% CI [0.111, 0.209]), which accounted for 45.51% of the total effect. The model diagram was shown in Fig. 2.

Table 2 The mediating role of trait anger between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity
Fig. 2
figure 2

The mediating role of trait anger between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity

State anger as the mediator

Similar to trait anger, hierarchical regression analysis was used to examine the mediating role of state anger. The results were shown in Table 3. It showed that justice sensitivity significantly and positively predicted malevolent creativity (c = 0.345, t = 7.262, p < 0.001) and state anger (a = 0.277, t = 5.903, p < 0.001), while state anger significantly and positively predicted malevolent creativity (b = 0.492, t = 10.958, p < 0.001). Therefore, the mediation model was confirmed, which indicated that the relationship between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity was partially mediated by state anger. The indirect effect was 0.136 (95% CI [0.087, 0.188]), which accounted for 39.50% of the total effect. The model diagram was shown in Fig. 3.

Table 3 The mediating role of state anger between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity
Fig. 3
figure 3

The mediating role of state anger between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity

Analysis of the moderating role of emotion regulation

Model 7 in PROCESS 3.3 developed by Hayes was used to explore the hypothesized moderated mediation model [66], as shown in Figs. 4 and 5, the indirect association between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity was moderated by emotion regulation. The results showed that the interaction of justice sensitivity and emotion regulation significantly predicted trait anger (B = 0.003, t = 3.021, p = 0.003), as well as state anger (B = 0.004, t = 2.765, p = 0.006).

Fig. 4
figure 4

The moderated mediation models (trait anger as the mediator)

Fig. 5
figure 5

The moderated mediation models (state anger as the mediator)

As shown in Figs. 6 and 7, justice sensitivity could significantly and positively predict trait anger (β = 0.075, t = 7.024, p < 0.001) and state anger (β = 0.095, t = 5.532, p < 0.001), when the level of emotional regulation was high. Meanwhile, justice sensitivity could significantly predict trait anger positively (β = 0.028, t = 2.538, p = 0.012) instead of state anger (β = 0.028, t = 1.560, p = 0.120), when the level of emotional regulation was low. Additionally, justice sensitivity had a stronger predictive effect on trait anger and state anger when the level of emotion regulation was higher. The results suggested that higher levels of emotion regulation could serve as a buffer against the influences of justice sensitivity on trait anger and state anger among low justice sensitivity individuals. However, the moderating effect of emotion regulation was no longer significant when an individual’s justice sensitivity was high.

The effects of emotion regulation on the mediating pathway of justice sensitivity → trait anger → malevolent creativity (index = 0.0021, SE = 0.0007, 95% CI: [0.0008, 0.0036]) and justice sensitivity \(\to\) state anger \(\to\) malevolent creativity (index = 0.0021, SE = 0.0009, 95% CI: [0.0005, 0.0039]) were all statistical significant. The details were shown in Table 4. The indirect effects through trait anger were both significant in participants with high and low emotion regulation. Meanwhile, the indirect effects through state anger were significant in participants with high emotion regulation and not those with low emotion regulation.

Table 4 The moderated indirect effect
Fig. 6
figure 6

The interaction effect of JSI and ERQ on TAS

Fig. 7
figure 7

The interaction effect of JSI and ERQ on SAS

Discussion

To advance the understanding of malevolent creativity, the present study investigated a moderated mediation model to revealed the association between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity. As hypothesized, the correlation between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity was mediated by trait anger/state anger. The higher sensitivity to justice, the higher level of trait anger/state anger, which in turn boosted the tendency of malevolent creativity. Additionally, this indirect connection was moderated by emotion regulation. To be specific, the indirect effects through trait anger were both significant in participants with high and low emotion regulation, however, the indirect effects through state anger were significant in participants with high emotion regulation but not those with low emotion regulation.

The association between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity

The results of this study found that justice sensitivity significantly positively predicted malevolent creativity, which was in line with prior researches. Individuals who were not treated fairly would experience more negative emotions and show more negative behaviors. For example, Brebels et al. found that participants who faced unequal distributional outcomes stole more money from the manager [67]. Another study found that organizational injustice perception which included procedural justice and interpersonal justice could negatively predict workplace deviance, and the relationships mediated by negative emotion [68].

Justice is an important means to defend self-benefit in society. The Sensitivity to Mean Intentions Model (SeMI) states that individuals with higher level of justice sensitivity have a lower threshold for perceiving malicious information in offensive or threatening situations compared to individuals with low justice sensitivity. That’s why high justice sensitivity individuals tend to actively search for or focus on information unfavorable to them, and then activate a suspicious mindset after perceiving malicious intentions [69]. Therefore, individuals with high justice sensitivity were more attentive to unfair stimuli and activated easily by the unfair information, which might prompt them to take steps to defend the fairness and self-benefit [10]. As a result, these people might tend to engage in more negative deviant behaviors [70], and be more likely to harm others, i.e., show more malevolent creativity. Previous studies demonstrated that people tend to exhibit malevolent creativity in threatened context, e.g. bullying victimization [71], unfair [13]. Clark and James found that perceptions of unfair treatment enhanced instances of negative creativity whereas perceptions of fair treatment yielded more positive creativity [13]. Another research also found individuals who were more implicitly aggressive and less premeditative were more likely to be malevolently creative in response to situations that provoke malevolent creativity [7]. These results might indicate that situational perceptions, such as justice and fairness, could influence the degree to which creative products are negative. Therefore, high level justice sensitive may generate high level malevolent creativity.

In other hand, justice sensitive individuals do not entirely behave in accordance with norms of justice, sometimes they could show protest and retaliate more strongly at once when they counter injustice [69]. For example, researchers found victim-sensitive individuals tended to make unfair offers when they had the power to distribute money at will between themselves and another person [72]. Another study also showed higher victim sensitivity predicted higher relational, proactive, and reactive aggression, and higher observer sensitivity predicted higher physical and verbal aggression [14]. Schmitt et al. found that vengeful reactions of laid-off employees toward their former employer depended directly and indirectly—mediated by the perceived fairness of the lay-off procedure—on justice sensitivity [73]. Meanwhile existing studies found that individuals who tend to break rules or had a weak sense of rule compliance were more likely to possess higher creativity [74].

In summary, it is plausible that justice sensitivity positively predicts the tendency of malevolent creativity. Individuals with high justice sensitivity are more likely to perceive information about injustice and generate aggressive thoughts and behaviors. This may mean that justice sensitivity individuals also have a tendency to break the rules. The aggressive performance may send a message to the perpetrator that what he has done is reprehensible, at the same time, the performance also is a way to respond injustice in order to avoid similar harm in the future [75]. Thus, individuals with higher justice sensitivity are more likely to generate aggressive thoughts or behaviors and harm others intentionally, so that resulting in higher level of malevolent creativity.

The mediating role of anger

The results of this study found that both trait anger and state anger mediated the relationship between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity. Specifically, justice sensitivity could not only directly affect malevolent creativity but also indirectly affect malevolent creativity through trait anger/state anger. This result was consistent with previous studies. Some researchers found that justice sensitivity positively predicted anger [38, 76, 77]. Schmitt et al. described justice sensitivity as: “Individuals differ in how sensitive they are to justice; how easily they are able to perceive injustice; and how strongly they react to perceived injustice” [11]. Thus, justice sensitivity was a good predictor of an individual’s response to injustice, those with high justice sensitivity tended to respond more strongly to injustice. To be specific, individuals with high justice sensitivity, when confronted with an injustice allocation scheme, would produce a significant increase in the level of negative emotional arousal, which further lead to an increase in anger [10]. The state of anger, on the other hand, exacerbated the conflict and mistrust in society, undermined the interpersonal interaction and cooperation. Under the emotion of anger, individuals were able to generate more creative and more damaging ideas, which were destructive to society and others [38]. Lastly, this increased the level of malevolent creativity tendency.

The moderating role of emotion regulation

Our results also revealed that emotion regulation moderated the effect of justice sensitivity on trait anger and state anger. Individuals with high levels of emotion regulation were more likely to avoid anger triggered by justice sensitivity than individuals with low emotion regulation. There is a plausible explanation regarding the moderate role of emotion regulation. As mentioned earlier, individuals who perceive unfairness typically experienced high levels of emotional arousal, while both cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression emotion regulation strategies were effective in decreasing emotional arousal and implicit aggression [38]. Thus, individuals with high levels of emotion regulation were better able to regulate anger arising from perceived injustice, which in turn reduced the level of malevolent creativity tendency.

Additionally, the current study found that the moderating effect of emotion regulation was different on trait anger and state anger. Justice sensitivity could positively predict trait anger when the level of emotional regulation was low. One possible explanation for this difference is that the lower level of ERQ might suggest that individuals do not need emotion regulation strategies to manage their emotions frequently. This might indicate that people do not receive external injustice information frequently, so that justice sensitivity as a stable personality trait only can predict the trait anger that has a tighter relationship to it [11], instead of state anger. Because state anger always depends on the environmental stimuli in the moment.

Although this study revealed possible mechanisms by exploring justice sensitivity influenced on malevolent creativity, there were still some shortcomings. Firstly, justice sensitivity contained multiple components that were not examined separately in this study. Future research could delve into the relationship between different components of justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity. Secondly, this study did not examine whether there was a difference in the role of cognitive reappraisal and expression suppression, which could be further explored in future studies. Future studies also could choose to incorporate other types of emotion regulation strategies and compare the effects of different emotion regulation strategies. Finally, the MCBS was utilized in current study to measure the level of malevolent creativity. Notably, the MCBS, as a measurement tool, could measure potential propensity of malevolent creativity. Some recent studies related to malevolent creativity used malevolent creativity tasks (MCT) to explore malevolent creativity performance. Future research could combine examination of malevolent creativity propensity and malevolent creativity performance to explore the influencing factors and internal mechanisms of malevolent creativity.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the present study validated the association between justice sensitivity and malevolent creativity. The findings illustrated the mediating effect of trait anger/state anger in the pathway from justice sensitivity to malevolent creativity. Additionally, the results also showed evidence of two-way interaction, indicating that emotion regulation moderated the relationship between justice sensitivity and anger. Individuals with high emotion regulation are better able to avoid anger from heightened justice sensitivity than individuals with low emotion regulation.

Data availability

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

Abbreviations

JSI:

Justice Sensitivity Inventory

TAS:

Trait Anger Scale

SAS:

State Anger Scale

ERQ:

Emotion Regulation Questionnaire

MCBS:

Malevolent Creativity Behavior Scale

SD:

Standardized deviation

SE:

Standard error

95%CI:

95% Bootstrap Confidence interval

References

  1. Runco MA, Jaeger GJ. The Standard Definition of Creativity. Creativity Res J. 2012;24(1):92–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Cropley DH, Kaufman JC, Cropley AJ. Malevolent creativity: a functional model of creativity in terrorism and crime. Creativity Res J. 2008;20(2):105–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Gutworth MB, Cushenbery L, Hunter ST. Creativity for Deliberate Harm: Malevolent Creativity and Social Information Processing Theory. J Creative Behav. 2018;52(4):305–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Kapoor H, Kaufman JC. The evil within: the AMORAL model of dark creativity. Theory Psychol. 2022;32(3):467–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Callan MJ, Ellard JH. Beyond blame and derogation of victims: Just-world dynamics in everyday life. The psychology of justice and legitimacy. Ontario symposium on personality and social psychology. New York, NY, US: Psychology Press; 2010. pp. 53–77.

  6. Lerner MJ, editor. The Belief in a Just World: A Fundamental Delusion 1980.

  7. Harris DJ, Reiter-Palmon R. Fast and furious: the influence of implicit aggression, premeditation, and provoking situations on malevolent creativity. Psychol Aesthet Creativity Arts. 2015;9(1):54–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Cropanzano R, Mitchell MS. Social Exchange Theory: an Interdisciplinary Review. J Manag. 2005;31(6):874–900.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Greenberg J. Organizational justice: yesterday, today, and tomorrow. J Manag. 1990;16(2):399–432.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Hu GX, Liu YZ, Wang H, He N, Chen XX. Effect of Ego-depletion on altruistic punishment: the role of anger and justice sensitivity. J Psychol Sci. 2020;43(01):117–24.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Schmitt M, Baumert A, Gollwitzer M, Maes J. The Justice Sensitivity Inventory: Factorial Validity, Location in the personality Facet Space, demographic pattern, and normative data. Soc Justice Res. 2010;23(2–3):211–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Mohiyeddini C, Schmitt MJ. Sensitivity to befallen injustice and reactions to unfair treatment in a laboratory situation. Soc Justice Res. 1997;10(3):333–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Clark K, James K. Justice and positive and negative creativity. Creativity Res J. 1999;12(4):311–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Bondü R, Richter P. Linking forms and functions of aggression in adults to justice and rejection sensitivity. Psychol Violence. 2016;6(2):292–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Bilgin A, Bondü R, Elsner B. Longitudinal associations between justice sensitivity, nonsuicidal self-injury, substance use, and victimization by peers. Dev Psychopathol. 2022;34(4):1560–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Gollwitzer M, Schmitt M, Schalke R, Maes J, Baer A. Asymmetrical effects of Justice Sensitivity perspectives on Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior. Soc Justice Res. 2005;18(2):183–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Deffenbacher JL, Oetting ER, Thwaites GA, Lynch RS, Baker DA, Stark RS, et al. State–trait anger theory and the utility of the trait anger scale. J Couns Psychol. 1996;43(2):131–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Fan ZG, Tao M, Li JR. The intergenerational transmission of trait anger and its influence on children’s anxiety and depression. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2023;31(2):407–12.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Liu W, Jiang Q, Ren J, Li S, Xu Y. The impact of trait anger on aggressive behavior: ModeratedMediating Effect of hostile cognition and impulsivity level. Acta Meteorologica Sinica. 2015;31(04):485–93.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory. The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology 1988. p. 1-.

  21. Adams JS. Inequity in Social Exchange. In: Berkowitz L, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Volume 2. Academic; 1965. pp. 267–99.

  22. Mikula G, Scherer KR, Athenstaedt U. The role of injustice in the elicitation of Differential emotional reactions. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1998;24(7):769–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Miller DT. Disrespect and the experience of injustice. Ann Rev Psychol. 2001;52:527–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Gollwitzer M, Rothmund T. What exactly are victim-sensitive persons sensitive to? J Res Pers. 2011;45(5):448–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Xie XX,  Liu Y, Wu WJ. The Development and Prospect of researches on Justice sensitivity advances in. Psychol Sci. 2012;20(02):301–8.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Qin G, Zhang LH. Perceived overall injustice and organizational deviance-mediating effect of anger and moderating effect of moral disengagement. Front Psychol. 2022;13.

  27. Barclay LJ, Skarlicki DP, Pugh SD. Exploring the role of emotions in injustice perceptions and retaliation. J Appl Psychol. 2005;90(4):629–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Scott W, Trost Z, Bernier E, Sullivan MJL. Anger differentially mediates the relationship between perceived injustice and chronic pain outcomes. PAIN®. 2013;154(9):1691–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Trost Z, Scott W, Buelow MT, Nowlin L, Turan B, Boals A, et al. The association between injustice perception and psychological outcomes in an inpatient spinal cord injury sample: the mediating effects of anger. Spinal Cord. 2017;55(10):898–905.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Schmitt MJ, Neumann R, Montada L. Dispositional sensitivity to Befallen Injustice. Soc Justice Res. 1995;8(4):385–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Krakel M. Relative deprivation in rank-order tournaments. Labour Econ. 2000;7(4):385–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Guo X. The sense of relative deprivation and crime of vulnerable groups in the transition period. Jiangxi Social Sci. 2012;32(09):236–40.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Pillutla MM, Murnighan JK. Unfairness, anger, and spite: emotional rejections of ultimatum offers [https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1996.0100]. Netherlands: Elsevier Science; 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Russell JA. Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol Rev. 2003;110(1):145–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Lang PJ. The emotion probe. Studies of motivation and attention. Am Psychol. 1995;50(5):372–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Baas M, De Dreu CKW, Nijstad BA. Creative production by angry people peaks early on, decreases over time, and is relatively unstructured. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2011;47(6):1107–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Cheng R, Lu K, Hao N. The effects of anger on different forms of malevolent creative performance. J Psychol Sci. 2021;44(06):1336–45.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Cheng R, Lu K, Hao N. The effect of anger on malevolent creativity and strategies for its emotion regulation. Acta Physiol Sinica. 2021;53(8):847–60.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Wang XC, Yang L, Yang JP, Gao L, Zhao FQ, Xie XC, et al. Trait anger and aggression: a moderated mediation model of anger rumination and moral disengagement. Pers Indiv Differ. 2018;125:44–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Moron M, Biolik-Moron M. Emotional awareness and relational aggression: the roles of anger and maladaptive anger regulation. Pers Indiv Differ. 2021;173.

  41. Eisenberg N. Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Ann Rev Psychol. 2000;51:665–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Germain CL, Kangas M. Trait anger symptoms and emotion regulation: the effectiveness of Reappraisal, Acceptance and suppression strategies in regulating anger. Behav Change. 2015;32(1):35–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology. 2002;39(3):281–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Mauss IB, Cook CL, Cheng JYJ, Gross JJ. Individual differences in cognitive reappraisal: Experiential and physiological responses to an anger provocation. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;66(2):116–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. The effects of trait reappraisal and suppression on anger. and blood pressure in response to provocation [press release]. Netherlands: Elsevier Science 2010.

  46. Szasz PL, Szentagotai A, Hofmann SG. The effect of emotion regulation strategies on anger. Behav Res Ther. 2011;49(2):114–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Chen B, Yang R, Deng W. Justice Sensitivity and the Processing of Justice - related information: based on Chinese Culture. Psychol Explor. 2013;33(06):507–12.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Yang R. A study on the impact of justice sensitivity on the processing of unfair information. [Postgraduate]: Gannan Normal University; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Liu H, Yu Z, Huang D, Yang Q, Ye B, Liu L, et al. Social Exclusion and Depression: the role of victim justice sensitivity and envy. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2023;31(06):1410–3.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Luo YL, Zhang DJ, Liu YB, Liu YL. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of trait anger scale in college students. Chin Mental Health J. 2011;25(09):700–4.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Fan Z, Tao M, Li J. The intergenerational transmission of trait anger and its influence on children’s anxiety and depression. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2023;31(02):407–12.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Li R, Xia L. The mediating effect of aggression motivation on the relationship between trait anger and reactive aggression: a longitudinal study. Acta Physiol Sinica. 2021;53(07):788–97.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Tao H. Revise of state-trait anger expression inventory II and analyse of the coping style among anger-out group and anger-in group [Postgraduate]. Shandong University Of Traditional Chinese Medicine; 2009.

  55. Forgays DG, Forgays DK, Spielberger CD. Factor structure of the state-trait anger expression inventory. J Pers Assess. 1997;69(3):497–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Liu HJ, Gao HM. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of state-trait anger expression Inventory-2 in college students. Chin Mental Health J. 2012;26(01):70–6.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Dong L, Chen H. Physiological response and subjective experience in evoked anger between feminine and masculine women. Chin Mental Health J. 2017;31(06):500–4.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2003;85(2):348–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Wang L, Liu H, Li Z, Du W. Reliability and validity of emotion regulation questionnaire Chinese revised version. China J Health Psychol. 2007;06:503–5.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Guo X, Zheng H, Ruan D, Dd H, Wang WY. Associations between empathy and negative affect: effect of emotion regulation. Acta Physiol Sinica. 2023;55(06):892–904.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Wang Q, Fan C, Zhu X. The Relationship between adolescent cybervictimization and cyberbullying: a Moderated mediated Model. Psychol Dev Educ. 2020;36(02):216–27.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Hao N, Tang MY, Yang J, Wang QF, Runco MA. A New Tool to measure Malevolent Creativity: the Malevolent Creativity Behavior Scale. Front Psychol. 2016;7.

  63. Tang D, Wen Z. Statistical approaches for Testing Common Method Bias: problems and suggestions. J Psychol Sci. 2020;43(01):215–23.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51(6):1173–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Wen Z, Fang J, Xie J, Ouyang J. Methodological research on mediation effects in China’s mainland. Adv Psychol Sci. 2022;30(08):1692–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Brebels L, De Cremer D, Sedikides C. Retaliation as a response to Procedural unfairness: a Self-Regulatory Approach. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2008;95(6):1511–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Wang YQ, Long LR, Zhou H. Organizational injustice perception and workplace deviance: mechanisms of negative emotion and traditionality. Acta Physiol Sinica. 2012;44(12):1663–76.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Gollwitzer M, Rothmund T, Süssenbach P. The sensitivity to Mean intentions (SeMI) Model: Basic assumptions, recent findings, and potential avenues for Future Research. Soc Pers Psychol Compass. 2013;7(7):415–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Zheng H. The impact of Justice Sensitivity on democratic decision-making– the role of belief in Just World and Social class. [Postgraduate]: Southwest University; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  71. Tong D, Shi Y, Gu X, Lu P. Bullying victimization and Malevolent Creativity in Rural adolescents: the longitudinal Mediational role of hostile attribution. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw; 2024.

  72. Fetchenhauer D, Huang X. Justice sensitivity and distributive decisions in experimental games. Pers Indiv Differ. 2004;36(5):1015–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Schmitt M, Rebele J, Bennecke J, F¨orster N. Justice sensitivity, layoff fairness, and responsibility attributions as correlates of post citizenship behavior of laid off employees. Wirtschaftspsychologie. 2008;10:101–10.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Petrou P, van der Linden D, Salcescu OC. When breaking the rules relates to Creativity: the role of creative problem-solving demands and organizational constraints. J Creative Behav. 2020;54(1):184–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Gollwitzer M, Denzler M. What makes revenge sweet: seeing the offender suffer or delivering a message? J Exp Soc Psychol. 2009;45:840–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Baumert A, Gollwitzer M, Staubach M, Schmitt M. Justice Sensitivity and the Processing of Justice-related information. Eur J Pers. 2011;25(5):386–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Yang Z, Gu Z, Wang D, Tan X, Wang X. The effect of anger and sadness on prosocial decision making: the role of the interpersonal attribution of responsibility. Acta Physiol Sinica. 2017;49(03):393–403.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the college students for agreeing to participate in the study.

Funding

This research was supported by the Natural Science Foundation of Shandong Province (ZR2022MC113); the Project of Shandong Province Higher Educational Youth Innovation Science and Technology Program (2019RWF003); the Special Project of Innovation Quality of Educational Sciences Planning of Shandong Province (2022CYB207).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Y.W. and K.Z. contributed equally to this work. Y.W. was responsible for the data analysis, methodology, writing of the original draft, review and editing. K.Z. was responsible for the data analysis, and writing of the original draft preparation. F.X., Y. Z. and L. C. were responsible for data analysis and methodology. W.L. was responsible for the conceptualization, reviewing and editing the draft, and funding acquisition. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wenfu Li.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Jining Medical University. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wang, Y., Zhang, K., Xu, F. et al. The effect of justice sensitivity on malevolent creativity: the mediating role of anger and the moderating role of emotion regulation strategies. BMC Psychol 12, 265 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01759-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01759-w

Keywords