Skip to main content

Digital media use, depressive symptoms and support for violent radicalization among young Canadians: a latent profile analysis

Abstract

Background

Despite the prominent role that digital media play in the lives and mental health of young people as well as in violent radicalization (VR) processes, empirical research aimed to investigate the association between Internet use, depressive symptoms and support for VR among young people is scant. We adopt a person-centered approach to investigate patterns of digital media use and their association with depressive symptoms and support for VR.

Methods

A sample of 2,324 Canadian young people (Mage = 30.10; SDage = 5.44 ; 59% women) responded to an online questionnaire. We used latent profile analysis to identify patterns of digital media use and linear regression to estimate the associations between class membership, depressive symptoms and support for VR.

Results

We identified four classes of individuals with regards to digital media use, named

Average Internet Use/Institutional trust, Average internet use/Undifferentiated Trust, Limited Internet Use/Low Trust and Online Relational and Political Engagement/Social Media Trust. Linear regression indicated that individuals in the Online Relational and Political Engagement/Social Media Trust and Average Internet Use/Institutional trust profiles reported the highest and lowest scores of both depression and support for VR, respectively.

Conclusions

It is essential to tailor prevention and intervention efforts to mitigate risks of VR to the specific needs and experiences of different groups in society, within a socio-ecological perspective. Prevention should consider both strengths and risks of digital media use and simulteaneously target both online and offline experiences and networks, with a focus on the sociopolitical and relational/emotional components of Internet use.

Peer Review reports

Background

The recent increase in support for – and direct engagement in – ideologically motivated violence among youth can be associated with the increase in social polarization in society [1] as well as the specificities of adolescence and early adulthood, a seminal period for the development of ideologies [2, 3]. Violent radicalization (VR) is a complex and multidimensional phenomenon [4] defined as a process whereby an individual or a group increases support for violence as a legitimate means to reach a specific (e.g., political, social, religious) goal [5]. Noteably, VR processes are increasingly occurring online [6, 7]. Internet use has been primarily investigated in the field of terrorism studies and with samples of radicalized individuals [6, 8]. Less is known about the association of digital media use, social polarization and attitudes towards support for VR among young people. Although the association between attitudes and behaviors is not a linear one, positive attitudes towards VR can contribute to the creation of socially polarized environments that fuel conflicts and shatter social solidarities, resulting in some cases in extremist ideologies and the normalization of violence. In such contexts, vulnerable individuals - such as those experiencing significant social grievances - are at higher risk of engaging in violent acts and extremism. Thus, in a primary prevention perspective, a reduction in support of VR among youth can result in an overall decrease of violence in our societies in the short and long-term [9,10,11].

Although numerous interventions target online literacy and social media use as potential ways to counter violent extremism [7], empirical research on their effectiveness is scarce and the role that Internet use plays in the development of positive attitudes towards VR among young people is largely understudied. While depressive symptomatology, which has also been increasing among young people in the past decade [12, 13], is associated with both digital media use and support for VR [14,15,16,17], empirical research has not yet examined the associations between these variables simultaneously in one study. The current study aims to fill this gap in the literature by empirically investigating if and how patterns of digital media use are differently associated with depressive symptoms and support for VR among a sample of Canadian young people via a person-centered approach. Given the prominent role that digital media use play in both VR processes and mental health among young people, a better understanding of risk and protective factors associated with digital media use is warranted to inform and tailor evidence-based prevention programs that could significantly help reduce social ruptures and the associated risk of violence.

Digital media use and support for VR

The online space has become a central developmental context for young people [18, 19]. Empirical evidence remains mixed, suggesting that digital media use can be either a risk or protective factor across multiple developmental outcomes depending on a complex interplay between both online and offline factors [18]. A consensus is now emerging that the specific behaviors in which youth engage online, rather than overall digital media per se, are key determinants of well-being. Yet, gaps in knowledge remain [20].

On the one hand, digital media can be used to connect with peers and to counter isolation, thus extending or reinforcing one’s social support network and possibly one’s trust in institutions and in democracy. On the other hand, the Internet can provide instant and unfiltered access to content and groups that propagate fake news, extreme beliefs and encourage violent actions, representing one of the main settings that can facilitate disaffiliation phenomena and recruitment of young people by extremist groups [7, 21,22,23,24]. Notably, whereas the majority of young people go online, only a minority of them get involved in VR processes. As such, it is likely that digital media use does not have a linear relationship with support for VR, but that specific constellations of digital media use are differentially associated with support for VR [8, 25].

Young people’s use of digital media is complex and heterogeneous [18], making the measurement and conceptualization of digital media use a challenging area [26, 27]. In this study, we focus on some aspects of digital media use that have been theoretically and/or empirically associated with VR, namely time on social media, reasons for Internet use (work, informational, entertainment, social), news literacy, trust in specific online sources of information (news, peers, influencers, government, youtube), preference for online social interactions and online political interactions.

The Internet can be used for multiple purposes, spanning from work or entertainment, to relational maintenance and social interaction [18, 28]. Although spending more time online has been associated with increased exposure to extremist content [23], whether this exposure is associated with risks of VR is yet unclear [29]. Overall, the impact of time spent on social media on a variety of social and health outcomes including VR varies based on the specific online activities and experiences [8, 18, 20].

Of importance, the Internet is currently the most important source of information for young people [30, 31], but trust on the validity of information from official governmental websites as well as from social media (e.g., Instagram, Twitter, Youtube) can vary between individuals. Misinformation and beliefs in conspiracy theories have been associated with higher support for VR [32, 33]. News literacy is considered a potential avenue to countering both misinformation, social polarization and online extremism [34, 35]. News literacy is defined as the ability to find/identify/recognize news, critically evaluate and produce them [36]. However, empirical research that examines the association between news literacy and support for VR is lacking.

Prior research has found that preference for online social interactions over face-to-face relationships represents a risk factor for support for VR [37, 38]. Preference for online social interactions is characterized by beliefs that one is safer, more confident, more comfortable and appreciated when online as opposed to offline [39] and is considered a component of problematic Internet use as it implies problematic relational experiences offline.

Some studies suggest that actively seeking and engaging with extremist content online is associated with higher risk of VR [8, 22, 25]. Although online interactions with strangers have been associated with higher risk of psychological distress [17, 40], the extent to which interactions with known and unknown people around political or current issues are associated, if at all, with support for VR has yet to be explored [23].

Given the variety in online experiences and type of digital media use, a person-centered approach via a latent profile analysis (LPA) facilitates examining different constellations of digital media use among young adults and associations between latent groups and support for VR. As VR is the result of complex and unique interplays between personal and social/contextual variables [4, 41, 42], identifying patterns of vulnerabilities online via a person-centered approach can inform the development of tailored VR prevention programs targeting digital media use.

The present study

The present study adopts a person-centered approach to investigate: 1) patterns of digital media use among young Canadians. Specifically we focus on reasons for digital media use (i.e., work, entertainment, socialization, information), reported trust in different sources of online information (i.e., official government and news websites and social media), news literacy, time on social media, preference for online social interactions and online political interactions (e.g., posting/discussing with peers vs strangers, having conflicts online about these issues); 2) the association between patterns of digital media use and levels of depressive symptoms; and 3) the association between patterns of digital media use and support for VR. We expect to identify at least two groups of young people who differ in their reported digital media use. Given that we do not have a priori knowledge of the class structure in the data, we did not have a priori hypotheses about the association between each profile and depressive symptoms. However, we anticipate that the group(s) that will report the highest levels of depressive symptoms will also be at higher risk of supporting VR.

Method

Participants

A total of 2,695 participants answered an online survey; missing outcome data (n=362) and individuals identifying as “other” gender (n=9) were removed for methodological concerns given the very small sample size of this gender group. Final sample size was 2,324 participants (59.3% women; mean age = 30.10; SD = 5.44 ). Socio-demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Characteristics of Study Participants (N = 2,324)

Procedure

Data were collected in November 2021, during the COVID-19 pandemic in Alberta, Ontario, and Quebec. Participants were recruited through the Leger360 online platform with over 500,0000 registered members and answered the survey in either English or French [43]. informed consent to participate was obtained electronically from all of the participants in the study, and response rate was 53.8%. Exclusion criteria were individuals under the age of 18 or above 41. Study protocol and procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of.

Measures

Support for VR

The Radicalism Intention Scale (RIS) is a 4-item subscale of the Activism and Radicalism Intention Scales (ARIS) [44]. It assesses an individual’s readiness to participate in illegal and violent behavior in the name of one’s group or organization. Respondents rated their agreement with four statements on a seven-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating more support for VR (range 4-28). The scale has good psychometric properties among young adults [45] (α = .89; Ω = 0.89).

Time spent on social media (daily)

Participants were asked to identify how many hours they spend on social media on a typical day (i.e., less than 2 hours, 2-4 hours, 4-6 hours, and 6 hours or more).

Reasons for Internet use

Four statements on Internet use were presented (i.e., using Internet: for personal relationships, to actively search for information/news, for entertainment, and for work). Participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point Likert scale how much they used the Internet for each reason (not at all, a little, moderately, a lot, most of the time).

News literacy

Was measured as a subscale of the literacy scale by Jones-Jang et al. [36]. Participants were asked using a 5-point Likert scale how much they agreed with each statement (six items, from 1-strongly disagree to 5- strongly agree, range 6 - 30)(α = .80; Ω = 0.80).

Trust on online sources of information

Five statements around trusting different sources of online information were presented, namely trust in news, peers, influencers, government, and YouTube sources of information. Participants were asked to indicate how often they trust each source of information on a 4-point Likert scale (never, rarely, sometimes, often).

Preference for online social interactions

(PFOSI) was measured with the 13-item Social Comfort subscale of the Online Cognition Scale [46]. Participants rated on a 8-point Likert scale (range 0 – 91) how much they agreed with statements describing their relationships with people who they know primarily through the Internet (e.g., chat rooms, message boards, online gaming communities). Higher scores indicate more preference for online social interactions (α = .92; Ω = 0.92).

Online political interactions

Participants were asked to indicate on a 6-point Likert scale (from “None/No time at all”, to “Several times a day”) how often their online interactions were oriented around these four statements: posted information about politics/current affairs on social media, discussed politics/current affairs with people you know, discussed politics/current affairs with people you do not know, had verbal conflicts with known people around information shared/posted online.

Depressive symptoms

Depressive symptoms were measured by using the 15-item subscale of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist-25 (HSCL-25) [47]. Items are rated on a Likert scale from 1 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) based on how much discomfort that problem has caused them during the past seven days, and a total score is obtained by computing the mean of all items. The clinical cut-off is set at 1.75 (score range from 1 to 4) and scores have been recoded as below (0) or above (1) this cut-off. The HSCL-25’s psychometric qualities have been well established [48] (α = .94; Ω = 0.94).

Socio-demographic variables

Participants provided information on their age, gender (man or woman), education (None/Less than high school, High school graduate, Apprenticeship, technical institute, trade or vocational school, College, CEGEP or other non-university certificate or diploma or University certificate, diploma or degree), Income ($19,999 or less, $20,000- $39,999, $40,000- $59,999, $60,000 - $79,999, $80,000- $99,999, $100,000 or more), employment (not employed, employed -essential, employed – non-essential), generational status (first-generation immigrant, second-generation immigrant, and third generation or more immigrant/non-immigrant), province (Alberta, Ontario, Quebec), religious beliefs (no religion, religion), and age.

Statistical analyses

Analyses were conducted using R software [49]. Missing data were imputed using the Random Forest method via the mice package [50, 51]. Sensitivity analysis suggested that missing data and multiple imputations did not alter the observed patterns of associations. First, we estimated the LPA model around variables related to digital media use via the tidyLPA package [52]. LPA is an analytic strategy that attempts to identify subgroups of people within a heterogeneous population who has a high degree of homogeneity in responses on a set of indicators. The appropriate number of latent profiles was selected based on the Akaike information criterion (AIC), the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), the Sample-size-adjusted BIC (SABIC), Bootstrap Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT), characteristics of the profiles (interpretability of response profiles or uniqueness) and a conservative profile sample size (>10%) [53,54,55,56]. Lower AIC, BIC and SABIC values and a statistically significant BLRT indicate a better model fit [53, 54]. Once the best LPA solution was identified, the level of entropy (acceptable if >.70) and Average Posterior Class Probability (AvePP; acceptable if >.70) were examined to determine the accuracy of classification [57].

Next, based on the predicted probabilities of profile membership made by the LPA, we assigned each participant to a specific profile. Analyses were then conducted on the univariable associations between sociodemographic characteristics and profile membership. Frequencies of profile membership by sociodemographic characteristics can be found in Table 3.

Lastly, we conducted linear regression analyses that estimated support for VR as a function of profile membership. A sequential model building approach was used to evaluate the associations between profiles and support for VR. Model 1 presents the unadjusted association between profile and support for VR; model 2 adjusts for sociodemographic characteristics, and model 3 adjusts for sociodemographic characteristics and depression.

Results

Latent Profile Structure

LPA models 2 through 6 are presented in Table 2 along associated BIC and log-likelihood values. The four-class solution was selected as the best fit for sample size of profiles (>10%) and interpretability of findings, despite not having the lowest BIC value. Figure 1 presents profile membership item response probabilities for digital media use. Participants in all profiles had a high probability of reporting average levels of news literacy. Unique class characteristics emerged around time spent on social media and overall Internet use preference for online social interactions, online political interactions and trusting multiple sources of information. Profile 1, named Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust is characterized by individuals who demonstrated average Internet use yet infrequently used the Internet for interactions around politics/current affairs and showed undifferentiated trust towards information found on-line, regardless of the source. Participants in Profile 2, named Limited Internet use/Low Trust, infrequently used the Internet across the considered reasons and reported a low probability of trusting news and government sources compared to information from social media (e.g., peers, influencers, youtube). Profile 3, named Average Internet use/Institutional trust, is characterized by participants with average and undifferentiated internet use, who were more likely to report greater trust in institutional sources of online information (e.g., news, government) compared to other social media sources. Profile 4, named Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust, consists of individuals with a high probability of preferring online, as opposed to in person, social interactions and spending a large amount of time on social media on a daily basis. In addition, participants in Profile 4 had a high probability of using the Internet for discussing politics and other issues with both peers and strangers, actively posting on-line about politics, and were more likely to report conflicts online compared to all other profiles. Profile 4 participants had a lower probability of trusting news and government sources compared to other sources of information online (peers, influencers, youtube). Overall, Profile 1 and 3 included 46.8% and 27.9% of participants, respectively. Profile 2 was smaller and included 11.4% of participants, while Profile 4 included 13.9% of participants.

Table 2 Fit statistics for solutions with 2 to 6 profiles on multiple imputed dataset (N = 2324)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Four-Profile Solution with Standardized Mean by Item Responses (N = 2324)

Note. PFOSI Preference for online social interactions

Profile belonging, sociodemographic characteristics and depressive symptoms

Table 3 represents sociodemographic characteristics and depressive symptoms by profile for study participants. All variables were significantly associated (p < 0.05) with profile belonging at the univariable level. The Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profile included a higher representation of women, non-immigrant, employed and non-religious participants, as well as participants who reported high education and income. A total of 45% of participants in this profile scored above the clinical cut-off for depressive symptoms. Participants in the Limited Internet use/Low Trust profile had a higher probability of being less educated, reporting a lower income and more unemployement. Participants in this profile were more likely to live in Alberta. Profile 3, Average Internet use/Institutional trust included participants who were highly educated, had high income, without an immigration background (third generation or more) and without a religion. Participants in this profile reported also the lowest levels of depression (37.5% above clinical cut-off) and more of them lived in Quebec. Finally, the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profile had an overrepresentation of men, immigrants, participants with a religion and who lived mainly in Ontario. In addition, participants in this group were overall educated but reported low income and high unemployment. A total of 70% of participants in this profile scored above the clinical cut-off to our measure of depressive symptoms.

Table 3 Frequencies of Profile Membership by Sociodemographic Characteristics and Depressive Symptoms (N = 2,324)

Associations of profile membership with support for VR

Profile membership was associated with scores on the RIS (p < 0.001). Participants in the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profile were more likely to report higher levels of support for VR compared to the other profiles in both unadjusted and adjusted models. Specifically, belonging to this profile was associated with a 0.91 (SE = 0.06, p < 0.001) increase in support for VR compared to the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profile when controlling for sociodemographic variables and depressive symptoms (Table 4). Belonging to the Average Internet use/Institutional trust profile was associated with a -0.267 (SE = 0.046, p < 0.001) decrease in support for VR compared to the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profile when controlling for sociodemographic variables and depression (Table 4). Gender, generation, province, age, and depressive symptoms were also associated with support for VR (p < 0.05). Men, first generation immigrants, participants from Ontario, younger participants, and participants reporting more depressive symptoms were more likely to report higher support for VR. Religion, income and education were not significantly associated with support for VR (Table 4).

Table 4 Associations between profile membership and support for Violent Radicalization as dependent variable

Discussion

The current study investigated patterns of digital media use in a sample of young adults from three Canadian provinces. In addition, we examined whether these patterns were differentially associated with depressive symptoms and support for VR. Four profiles emerged from our LPA, confirming the pertinence of using a person-centered approach to shed light on the complex patterns of digital media use among young people. Overall, profiles differentiated participants mostly in terms of trust on specific sources of information and level and type of online engagement.

The two largest profiles (Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust and Average Internet use/Insitutional trust) differed primarily in their trust of online sources of information. Specifically, individuals in the Average Internet use/Insitutional trust profile reported to trust more frequently institutional sources of information (i.e., government and news) rather than social media (i.e., youtube, influencers, and peers), suggesting an overall acceptance of mainstream information and of the status quo. In contrast, the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust group showed average levels of trust to all sources of information alike. This group spent slightly more time online than the Average Internet use/Insitutional trust one, but overall these two groups did not differ much in their online social or political interactions. These two groups included 74.7% of participants, indicating a divide in the population mostly linked to what online sources to trust for information. The remaining participants were equally distributed between the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust and the Low Internet use/Low trust profiles. Participants in both of these profiles trusted more frequently alternative social media sources of information compared to institutional ones, but they differed in overall levels of trust, with the Limited Internet use/Low trust group reporting overall low levels of trust, especially for institutional sources of information. Participants in the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profile reported high levels of trust in alternative social media sources of information and were more actively and politically engaged online with both peers and especially with strangers. They spent more time online and preferred online social interactions more compared to the other profiles. Taken together, these findings suggest that patterns of digital media use echo the increasing polarization in our societies [58, 59] around issues of trust/distrust, engagement/disengagement as well as a variety of negative/positive online experiences. Indeed, the most important variables to differentiate the four profiles were related to the frequency of trusting different online sources of information as well as specific social and political interactions online, rather than reasons for Internet use or news literacy, which on the contrary did not seem to play a significant role in determining profile membership.

We suggest that the divide around trust in online information and engagement needs to be situated in the broader socio-political context, which can partly explain the socio-demographic differences we found across profiles.The Average Internet use/Insitutional trust and the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profiles consisted of more affluent and more educated participants, mostly employed and without an immigration background. Participants in these profiles may benefit from more privileges in society, which can favor their trust in mainstream institutional sources of information online [60,61,62] . Indeed, participants in the Average Internet use/Institutional trust group were more likely to report the highest levels of education and income as well as the lowest levels of depressive symptoms followed by the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profile. The difference in levels of depression between these two profiles can also be associated with the presence of younger participants and more women in the Average Internet use/Undifferentiated trust profile compared to the Average internet use/Institutional trust one. The Low Internet use/Low trust and Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profiles included more participants reporting lower income. Participants in the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust group included a higher percentage of men, participants with an immigration background and professing a religion – although participants in this profile reported an education level similar to the two larger profiles. This profile reported concerning levels of depression (70.2% above clinical cut-off). Relying on the internet for relational and political purposes combined with more frequent trust in alternative social media sources of information and less privileges in society can jeopardize young people’s mental health. Within a socio-ecological perspective, the fact that this profile is made up of primarily educated men with an immigrant background may represent a form of double-bind in which some groups may feel alienated because official discourses and stances about equity in Canada are contradicted by daily life experiences. This group’s pattern of digital media use may be related to the hardships, grievances and social deprivation experienced by minorities both online and offline. The combination of negative life experiences with high emotional distress may lead to experience overall negative and conflictual online social and political exchanges, subsequently legitimazing violence as an ultimate solution [16, 17, 63]. Besides reporting low income similarly to the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust group, the Limited Internet use/Low trust profile included less educated and more unemployed participants compared to all other profiles, mostly without an immigration background. Participants in this group may not be content with their socio-political reality, and disengage from social and political issues, at least online. Noteworthy, our profiles suggest that digital media use is closely intertwined with social experiences offline. Interventions should consider this complex interaction and adopt a socio-ecological approach to both research and intervention, tailored not only to the different groups in society but also addressing the gap between them to mend the social fabric.

With regards to depressive symptoms and support for VR, the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust reported the highest levels of depression and support for VR, followed by the Limited Internet use/Low trust profile. The fact that the two groups that reported less trust in institutional sources of information compared to alternative social media showed more depressive symptoms and support for VR indicates that issues of trust are important to address with young people in prevention and intervention efforts. Given that individuals in these groups had overall a lower status in society, compared to the other two profiles, it is possible that they may have been experiencing more social deprivation and grievances during the pandemic and have been more sensitive to the anti-system rhetoric which provided meaning to this perceived injustice [60]. This divide aligns with the emergence of polarized social movements in the whole of Canada (e.g., pro- and anti-vaxx groups during the pandemic). Promoting a sense of agency and belonging as well as ensuring that young people can express their opinions and have a purpose in life may help decrease depressive symptoms and reduce overall socio-political distrust and disengement both online and offline, which can in turn contribute to reduce the legitimation of violence. However, such interventions need to consider the social adversity and deprivation experienced by young people and be tailored to the specific needs and challenges that they face. Multi-level systemic interventions that target online and offline socio-political macro-determinants of mental health and injustices in our societies are needed above and beyond individual intervention programs.

The association between membership to the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profile and support for VR aligns with prior studies pointing to an association between active online political engagement and interactions and support for VR [8, 23, 35]. Noteworthy, this was a characteristic that clearly distinguished the Online active political engagement/Social media trust profile from all other profiles. Online relational and political engagement should be addressed in prevention and intervention, while also addressing possible isolation and injustices experienced offline. The association between membership to the Limited Internet use/Low trust and support for VR can be related to an overall distrust in society and especially in government and official institutions, which has been found to represent a risk factor for VR [32].

As expected, the group that was at higher risk of supporting VR was also the one that reported the highest level of depressive symptoms, which were significantly and positively associated with support for VR, confirming prior evidence [38, 64,65,66,67]. Depressive symptoms do not necessarily lead to greater risk of VR [68]. Yet, multiple studies indicate a positive association between depressive symptoms and support for VR [38, 64,65,66,67]. Although directionality of associations remain to be established, available evidence suggests that youth who interact more with strangers online [17, 40], who prefer online social interactions [69,70,71] and who experience more social adversity [14, 67] are at higher risk of depression, which can partly explain the higher scores of depressive symptoms found among the Online relational and political engagement/Social media trust profiles. Identifying as a man and being younger were also risk factors for support for VR, in line with prior studies [7, 15], underlining the pertinence for future studies to focus on young people and to consider specificities by gender in VR studies [14, 29, 32, 45].

Limitations

This study has several limitations. Most importantly, the cross-sectional design prevents us from drawing any conclusions about causality. Longitudinal studies are needed to shed light on the trajectories of associations between patterns of digital media use, depressive symptoms and support for VR. Second, our study is based on a convenience sample with a relatively high socio-economic level and education. This means that our results may not be generalizable to a larger, general population of young adults. Nonetheless, our online method of recruitment is appropriate given the sensitivity of the topic and the challenges of conducting research during a pandemic. Third, all data are based on young people’s self-reports and social desirability biases cannot be excluded. Fourth, our measures of digital media use were limited and not comprehensive of the broad range of possible online experiences. Given the rapidly evolving and dynamic aspects of the Internet, the availability of validated measures for different facets of Internet use remains a challenge for future studies. Last, our data were collected during the COVID-19 pandemic in three Canadian provinces, and results cannot be easily generalized to other provinces or countries, nor to a non-pandemic context.

Conclusion

Despite these limitations, our findings suggest that digital media use, psychological distress and their interaction play a role in processes of VR among young people and need to be situated and understood within a socio-ecological and social justice perspective. Specifically, trust in different sources of information and social and political experiences online are as relevant as the emotional and relational experiences of young people. The dynamic associations among these key elements have to be considered simultanously when reflecting on VR prevention and digital media use among young people. Prevention efforts should be adapted to the needs of specific populations and consider the diversity of their online/offline experiences. Indeed, our results suggests that online experiences are intertwined with offline experiences in society, in particular with grievances, and that an attention to the rapidly evolving socio-political scenario is warranted when designing intervention programs to prevent processes of VR among young people targeting their digital media use. The fact that self-reported news literacy did not differ across profiles questions the pertinence of VR prevention programs that target mainly news literacy skills among youth. Our findings support preliminary results that showed that media literacy did not protect youth from exposure to extremist content online [35] or risks of VR [25]. It has been argued that programs aimed to foster digital literacy may be associated with improved technical competence but leave participants “critically naïve” [72], failing to situate digital competence within the broader socio-political context. Although digital literacies may still be relevant skills to promote among young people, our findings suggest that, when it comes to the prevention of VR processes, critical thinking skills, supportive environments and a social justice approach to intervention may be equally important.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

References

  1. Venkatesh V, Rousseau C, Morin D, Hassan G. Violence as collateral damage of the COVID-19 pandemic. The Conversation. 2021. https://theconversation.com/collateral-damage-of-covid-19-rising-rates-of-domestic-and-social-violence-143345.

  2. Harpviken AN. Psychological vulnerabilities and extremism among western youth: A literature review. Adolescent Research Review. 2020;5(1):1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Steinberg LD. Age of opportunity: Lessons from the new science of adolescence. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt; 2014.

  4. McGilloway A, Ghosh P, Bhui K. A systematic review of pathways to and processes associated with radicalization and extremism amongst Muslims in Western societies. International review of psychiatry. 2015;27(1):39–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Schmid AP. Radicalisation, de-radicalisation, counter-radicalisation: A conceptual discussion and literature review. International Centre for Counter-terrorism (ICCT). Research Paper. 2013. https://www.icct.nl/sites/default/files/import/publication/ICCT-Schmid-Radicalisation-De-Radicalisation-Counter-Radicalisation-March-2013_2.pdf.

  6. Oppetit A, Campelo N, Bouzar L, Pellerin H, Hefez S, Bronsard G, et al. Do radicalized minors have different social and psychological profiles from radicalized adults? Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2019;10:644.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Amit S, Kafy AA. A systematic literature review on preventing violent extremism. J Adolesc. 2022;94(8):1068–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Hassan G, Brouillette-Alarie S, Alava S, Frau-Meigs D, Lavoie L, Fetiu A, et al. Exposure to extremist online content could lead to violent radicalization: A systematic review of empirical evidence. International Journal of Developmental Science. 2018(Preprint):1-18.

  9. Eisenman DP, Flavahan L. Canaries in the coal mine: Interpersonal violence, gang violence, and violent extremism through a public health prevention lens. International review of psychiatry. 2017;29(4):341–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Weine S, Eisenman DP, Kinsler J, Glik DC, Polutnik C. Addressing violent extremism as public health policy and practice. Behavioral Sciences of Terrorism and Political Aggression. 2017;9(3):208–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Adam-Troian J, Tecmen A, Kaya A. Youth extremism as a response to global threats? European Psychologist. 2021;26(1):15–28.

  12. Knapstad M, Sivertsen B, Knudsen AK, Smith ORF, Aarø LE, Lønning KJ, et al. Trends in self-reported psychological distress among college and university students from 2010 to 2018. Psychological medicine. 2021;51(3):470–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Lipson SK, Phillips MV, Winquist N, Eisenberg D, Lattie EG. Mental health conditions among community college students: A national study of prevalence and use of treatment services. Psychiatric services. 2021:appi. ps. 202000437.

  14. Miconi D, Levinsson A, Frounfelker RL, Li ZY, Oulhote Y, Rousseau C. Cumulative and independent effects of experiences of social adversity on support for violent radicalization during the COVID-19 pandemic: the mediating role of depression. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2022;57:1–13.

  15. Rousseau C, Miconi D, Frounfelker RL, Hassan G, Oulhote Y. A repeated cross-sectional study of sympathy for violent radicalization in Canadian college students. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2020;90(4):406–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Baker DA, Algorta GP. The relationship between online social networking and depression: A systematic review of quantitative studies. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2016;19(11):638–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Ybarra ML, Alexander C, Mitchell KJ. Depressive symptomatology, youth Internet use, and online interactions: A national survey. Journal of adolescent health. 2005;36(1):9–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Stavropoulos V, Motti-Stefanidi F, Griffiths MD. Risks and opportunities for youth in the digital era: A cyber-developmental approach to mental health. European Psychologist. 2022;27(2):86–101.

  19. Subrahmanyam K, Šmahel D. Connecting online behavior to adolescent development: A theoretical framework. Digital youth: Springer; 2011. p. 27–39.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Hamilton L, Gross B. How Has the Pandemic Affected Students’ Social-Emotional Well-Being? A Review of the Evidence to Date: Center on Reinventing Public Education; 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Kaakinen M, Keipi T, Räsänen P, Oksanen A. Cybercrime victimization and subjective well-being: An examination of the buffering effect hypothesis among adolescents and young adults. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2018;21(2):129–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Turner N, Holt TJ, Brewer R, Cale J, Goldsmith A. Exploring the relationship between opportunity and self-control in youth exposure to and sharing of online hate content. Terrorism and Political Violence. 2022;35(7):1–16.

  23. Costello M, Barrett-Fox R, Bernatzky C, Hawdon J, Mendes K. Predictors of viewing online extremism among America’s youth. Youth & Society. 2020;52(5):710–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Morris E. Children: extremism and online radicalization. Journal of Children and Media. 2016;10(4):508–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Wolfowicz M, Hasisi B, Weisburd D. What are the effects of different elements of media on radicalization outcomes? A systematic review. Campbell Systematic Reviews. 2022;18(2):e1244.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. V Shah NK, R. Lance Holbert, Dhavan. Connecting and disconnecting with civic life: Patterns of Internet use and the production of social capital. Political Commun. 2001;18(2):141-62.

  27. Lee N-J, Shah DV, McLeod JM. Processes of political socialization: A communication mediation approach to youth civic engagement. Communication Research. 2013;40(5):669–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Ekström M, Olsson T, Shehata A. Spaces for public orientation? Longitudinal effects of Internet use in adolescence. Information, Communication & Society. 2014;17(2):168–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Conway M. Determining the role of the internet in violent extremism and terrorism: Six suggestions for progressing research. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism. 2017;40(1):77–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Anderson M, Jiang J. Teens, Social Media, and Technology 2018. Pew Research Center; 2018.

  31. Ohme J. Updating citizenship? The effects of digital media use on citizenship understanding and political participation. Information, Communication & Society. 2019;22(13):1903–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Levinsson A, Miconi D, Li Z, Frounfelker RL, Rousseau C. Conspiracy theories, psychological distress, and sympathy for violent radicalization in young adults during the CoViD-19 pandemic: a cross-sectional study. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2021;18(15):7846.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Vv Mulukom, Pummerer LJ, Alper S, Bai H, Čavojová V, Farias J, et al.  Antecedents and consequences of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs A systematic review. Soc Sci Med. 2022;301:114912.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Henschke A, Reed A. Toward an ethical framework for countering extremist propaganda online. Studies in Conflict and Terrorism. 2021:1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/1057610X.2020.1866744.

  35. Schmuck D, Fawzi N, Reinemann C, Riesmeyer C. Social media use and political cynicism among German youth: the role of information-orientation, exposure to extremist content, and online media literacy. Journal of Children and Media. 2022;16(3):313–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Jones-Jang SM, Mortensen T, Liu J. Does media literacy help identification of fake news? Information literacy helps, but other literacies don’t. American Behavioral Scientist. 2021;65(2):371–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Ellis H, Miller E, Sideridis G, Frounfelker RL, Miconi D, Abdi S, et al. Risk and protective factors associated with attitudes in support of violent radicalization: variations by geographic location. Int J Public Health. 2021;66:617053.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Miconi D, Geenen G, Frounfelker RL, Levinsson A, Rousseau C. Meaning in life, future orientation and support for violent radicalization among Canadian college students during the CoViD-19 pandemic. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13:765908.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Caplan SE. Problematic Internet use and psychosocial well-being: development of a theory-based cognitive–behavioral measurement instrument. Computers in human behavior. 2002;18(5):553–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Gross EF, Juvonen J, Gable SL. Internet use and well-being in adolescence. Journal of social issues. 2002;58(1):75–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Miconi D, Rousseau C. Another way out: A positive youth development (PYD) approach to the study of violent radicalizaton in Quebec (Canada). In: Dimitrova R, Wiium N, editors. Handbook of positive youth development: Advancing research, policy, and practice in global contexts. New York: Springer; 2021. p. 415–29.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  42. Gøtzsche-Astrup O, Van den Bos K, Hogg MA. Radicalization and violent extremism: Perspectives from research on group processes and intergroup relations. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2020;23(8):1127–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Leger 360. Leger Marketing 2022 [Available from: https://leger360.com/.

  44. Moskalenko S, McCauley C. Measuring political mobilization: The distinction between activism and radicalism. Terrorism and political violence. 2009;21(2):239–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Frounfelker RL, Frissen T, Miconi D, Lawson J, Brennan RT, d’Haenens L, et al. Transnational evaluation of the Sympathy for Violent Radicalization Scale: Measuring population attitudes toward violent radicalization in two countries. Transcult Psychiatry. 2021;58(5):669–82.

  46. Davis RA, Flett GL, Besser A. Validation of a new scale for measuring problematic Internet use: Implications for pre-employment screening. Cyberpsychology & behavior. 2002;5(4):331–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Derogatis LR, Lipman RS, Rickels K, Uhlenhuth EH, Covi L. The Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL): A self-report symptom inventory. Systems Research and Behavioral Science. 1974;19(1):1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Mollica RF, Caspi-Yavin Y, Bollini P, Truong T, Tor S, Lavelle J. The Harvard Trauma Questionnaire: validating a cross-cultural instrument for measuring torture, trauma, and posttraumatic stress disorder in Indochinese refugees. Journal of nervous and mental disease. 1992;180(2):111–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2017.

  50. van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. mice: Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations in R. J Stat Softw. 2011;45(3):1–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Sterne JA, White IR, Carlin JB, Spratt M, Royston P, Kenward MG, et al. Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. BMJ. 2009;338:b2393.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Rosenberg JM, Beymer PN, Anderson DJ, Van Lissa C, Schmidt JA. tidyLPA: An R package to easily carry out latent profile analysis (LPA) using open-source or commercial software. J Open Source Software. 2019;3(30):978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Ferguson SL, G. Moore EW, Hull DM Finding latent groups in observed data A primer on latent profile analysis in Mplus for applied researchers. Int J Behav Devel. 2020;44(5):458-68.

  54. Tein J-Y, Coxe S, Cham H. Statistical power to detect the correct number of classes in latent profile analysis. Struct Equation Model: a multidisciplinary J. 2013;20(4):640–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Lubke G, Neale MC. Distinguishing between latent classes and continuous factors: Resolution by maximum likelihood? Multivariate Behav Res. 2006;41(4):499–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Spurk D, Hirschi A, Wang M, Valero D, Kauffeld S. Latent profile analysis: A review and “how to” guide of its application within vocational behavior research. J Vocational Behav. 2020;120:103445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Masyn KE. Latent class analysis and finite mixture modeling. In Todd D. Little (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Quantitative Methods in Psychology vol. 2. Oxford Library of Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2013. p. 551–611.

  58. Pickup M, Stecula D, Van Der Linden C. Novel coronavirus, old partisanship: COVID-19 attitudes and behaviours in the United States and Canada. Can J Pol Sci/Revue canadienne de science politique. 2020;53(2):357–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Boxell L, Gentzkow M, Shapiro JM. Cross-country trends in affective polarization. Rev Econ Stat. 2024;106(2):557–65.

  60. Gibson B, Schneider J, Talamonti D, Forshaw M. The impact of inequality on mental health outcomes during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review. Can Psychol/Psychologie Canadienne. 2021;62(1):101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Bornand T, Klein O. Political Trust by Individuals of low Socioeconomic Status: The Key Role of Anomie. Soc Psychol Bull. 2022;17:1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Goubin S, Hooghe M. The Effect of Inequality on the Relation Between Socioeconomic Stratification and Political Trust in Europe. Soc Justice Res. 2020;33(2):219–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Vannucci A, Simpson EG, Gagnon S, Ohannessian CM. Social media use and risky behaviors in adolescents: A meta-analysis. J Adolesc. 2020;79:258–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Bhui K, Warfa N, Jones E. Is violent radicalisation associated with poverty, migration, poor self-reported health and common mental disorders? PloS one. 2014;9(3):e90718.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Bhui K, Silva MJ, Topciu RA, Jones E. Pathways to sympathies for violent protest and terrorism. Br J Psychiatry J Mental Sci. 2016;209(6):483–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Bhui K, Otis M, Silva MJ, Halvorsrud K, Freestone M, Jones E. Extremism and common mental illness: Cross-sectional community survey of White British and Pakistani men and women living in England. Br J Psychiatry. 2020;217(4):547–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Rousseau C, Oulhote Y, Lecompte V, Mekki-Berrada A, Hassan G, El Hage H. Collective identity, social adversity and college student sympathy for violent radicalization. Transcult Psychiatry. 2019;58(5):654–68.

  68. Misiak B, Samochowiec J, Bhui K, Schouler-Ocak M, Demunter H, Kuey L, et al. A systematic review on the relationship between mental health, radicalization and mass violence. Eur Psychiatry. 2019;56:51–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Gámez-Guadix M. Depressive symptoms and problematic Internet use among adolescents: Analysis of the longitudinal relationships from the cognitive–behavioral model. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2014;17(11):714–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Seabrook EM, Kern ML, Rickard NS. Social networking sites, depression, and anxiety: a systematic review. JMIR Mental Health. 2016;3(4): e5842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Vannucci A, McCauley Ohannessian C. Social media use subgroups differentially predict psychosocial well-being during early adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2019;48(8):1469–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Hinrichsen J, Coombs A. The five resources of critical digital literacy: a framework for curriculum integration. Res Lear Technol. 2014;21(0):1–16.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Our work is funded by a Digital Citizen Contribution Program grant awarded to DM by Canadian Heritage and by a project grant awarded to CR by the Canadian Institute of Health Research (CIHR).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Author DM contributed to conception and design of the study, interpretation of study findings, and writing the manuscript. Authors TS, RLF and ANM contributed to data analysis and drafting parts of the manuscript. Author CR contributed to the interpretation of study findings and provided feedback on several versions of the manuscript. The authors listed in the byline have agreed to the byline order and to submission of the manuscript in this form. All authors agreed to act as guarantor of the work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Diana Miconi.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. Informed consent to participate was obtained from all of the participants in the study. All procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Educational and Psychological Institutional Review Board of the University of Montreal.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Miconi, D., Santavicca, T., Frounfelker, R.L. et al. Digital media use, depressive symptoms and support for violent radicalization among young Canadians: a latent profile analysis. BMC Psychol 12, 260 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01739-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01739-0

Keywords