Skip to main content

Psychophysiological effects of walking in forests and urban built environments with disparate road traffic noise exposure: study protocol of a randomized controlled trial

Abstract

Background

Stress is a widespread phenomenon and reality of everyday life, entailing negative consequences for physical and psychological wellbeing. Previous studies have indicated that exposure to greenspaces and nature-based interventions are promising approaches to reducing stress and promoting restoration. However, an increasing percentage of the population lives in urban regions with limited opportunities to spend time in greenspaces. In addition, urban settings typically feature increased levels of noise, which represents a major environmental stressor. Although various studies have compared the effects of exposure to greenspaces versus urban built environments, evidence of the effects of noise in this context is very limited. Psychophysiological benefits of exposure to greenspaces compared to urban built environments reported in earlier studies might be less (or at least not only) due to features of the greenspaces than to additional stressors, such as road traffic noise in the urban built environment. Hence, differences in the effects attributed to greenness in previous studies may also be due to potentially detrimental noise effects in comparison settings. This paper reports the study protocol for a randomized, controlled intervention study comparing the effects of walking in forest versus urban built environments, taking road traffic noise exposure during walks in the respective settings into account.

Methods

The protocol envisages a field study employing a pretest–posttest design to compare the effects of 30-min walks in urban built environments and forests with different road traffic noise levels. Assessments will consist of self-reported measures, physiological data (salivary cortisol and skin conductance), an attention test, and noise, as well as greenness measurements. The outcomes will be restoration, stress, positive and negative affect, attention, rumination, and nature connectedness.

Discussion

The results will inform about the restorative effect of walking in general, of exposure to different types of environments, and to different noise levels in these sites. The study will provide insights into the benefits of walking and nature-based interventions, taking into account the potential detrimental effects of noise exposure. It will thus facilitate a better understanding of low-threshold interventions to prevent stress and foster wellbeing.

Trial registration

ISRCTN48943261; Registered 23.11.2023.

Background and rationale

Stress is a widespread phenomenon and part of our everyday life, with negative consequences for physical and psychological wellbeing. Data from the Swiss Health Survey 2017 revealed that in 2017, 21% of the respondents stated suffering from stress at their workplace “always” or “most of the time,” which represents an increase of 3% since 2012 [1]. Respondents who stated feeling stressed very often had a higher risk for burnout and emotional fatigue, compared with respondents reporting less stress [2]. Stress increases the risk of cardiovascular disease [3,4,5,6,7], gastrointestinal disorders [8], and blood pressure elevation [9]. Furthermore, stress is a risk factor for diabetes [10], obesity [11], and dementia [12] and is associated with hypertension [13]. Thus, stress constitutes an important public health issue, and research concerning stress recovery and approaches to prevent mental distress and improve psychophysiological wellbeing are needed.

In response to this urgency, some studies indicate that exposure to greenspaces can reduce stress symptoms and improve restoration and wellbeing [14,15,16,17,18,19]. Thus, exposure to greenspaces and nature-based interventions may be promising approaches to reduce stress and promote restoration. However, an increasing percentage of the population lives in urban regions with limited opportunities to spend time in greenspaces. In addition, such urban settings typically feature increased levels of transportation noise, representing a major environmental stressor and global challenge [20, 21]. Accordingly, the maintenance of environments suitable for promoting wellbeing, such as greenspaces, is suggested to foster mental health and support possibilities for coping with stress [22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31].

Much of the recent research on the benefits of engaging with greenspaces has been guided by theories concerned with psychophysiological stress reduction (stress reduction theory) [32, 33] or directed attention restoration (attention restoration theory) [34, 35]. Attention restoration theory states that nature allows restoration from a depleted capacity to direct attention, thus enabling more effective cognitive performance [35]; stress reduction theory proposes that natural environments promote recovery from stress and decrease arousal and negative thoughts through psychophysiological pathways [32, 33]. Correspondingly, there is an interest in measuring psychophysiological and attentional responses to greenspaces.

Although various earlier studies have compared the restorative effects of exposure to greenspaces versus urban built environments [18, 36,37,38,39], evidence of the effects of noise in this context is very limited. In their meta-analysis, Bowler and colleagues [36] compared the added benefits to health and well-being outcomes from activities such as walking and running in greenspaces versus synthetic (outdoor and indoor-built) environments. The results were mixed and revealed a paucity of good-quality studies. The authors concluded that well-designed studies are needed to further strengthen evidence and noted that differences between the benefits of greenspaces and urban built environments are less due to features of the greenspaces than to additional stressors in the urban built environment–one such stressor possibly being traffic noise. At the same time, Van Renterghem [31] discusses how vegetation can mitigate environmental noise perception, proposing a complex interplay between vegetation and noise factors for restoration.

In this context, we argue that although various earlier studies have compared the effects of exposure to greenspaces versus urban built environments, not enough attention has been given to potential confounding variables in previous studies. The greenspaces and urban built environments in earlier studies may have differed not only in terms of the extent of greenness of the compared environments, but also in terms of other variables. One particular variable in which environments might have differed is the extent of transportation noise (in particular road traffic noise) in different settings. Hence, differences in the effects attributed to greenness in previous studies may also be attributed to potentially detrimental (road traffic) noise effects in comparison settings, which potentially confounded previous results.

One previous study aimed at comparable noise levels in different environments, namely that by Gidlow and colleagues [40], who compared the effects of 30-min walks on stress, cognitive performance, restoration, and mood in a greenspace with water, a greenspace without water, and a pleasant residential urban environment. Differences between the different environments were found only for some of the outcome variables. The study showed no differences in the decrease in cortisol and improvements in mood between the different environments. A higher restorative experience in greenspaces, compared to the residential urban environment, and better cognitive performance 30 min after walking in greenspaces, compared to the residential urban environment, was shown. However, noise was only measured at one representative point on each route in each environment (urban residential 50.6 ± 4.3 dBA, green 47.5 ± 2.9 dBA, blue 45.6 ± 1.5 dBA) with two 15-min recordings in this study but was not assessed continuously during the walks. Furthermore, a difference of 5 dB between the conditions may result in a noticeable difference in noise levels between the different environments.

Research on noise exposure has shown associations between environmental noise, noise annoyance, and stress [41,42,43,44,45]. Studies on the effects of long-term noise exposure report an association between long-term aircraft noise exposure and a modified cortisol circadian rhythm [46] and decreased annoyance and increased wellbeing after a reduction of road traffic noise [47]. In contrast, various other studies could not establish such effects. Stockholm and colleagues [48] investigated the effects of long-term occupational noise on off-work cortisol levels. Their study showed no significant association between noise and cortisol levels. Other studies by Michaud et al. [49, 50] examined the effect of wind turbine noise levels up to 46 dBA on stress and found no effect. Studies on the effects of short-term noise exposure evidenced increased salivary cortisol levels for participants asked to solve a cognitive task in a louder (90 dBA) environment compared to a less noise-exposed environment (55–60 dBA, [51]; 45 dBA, [52]). Ellermeier et al. [44] found that skin conductance increased with the sound pressure level of vehicle sounds in a laboratory experiment. Overall, there is some evidence for stress as an effect of noise, suggesting that noise may also function as an acute impediment for stress reduction and restoration, but many results are based on cross-sectional data [41, 43, 53, 54] or laboratory studies [44, 51, 55,56,57]. Field experiments have shown weaker effects than laboratory research and have often relied on a very small sample size (e.g., Barbaresco et al. [58]). Good-quality field studies concerning the acute effects of noise on stress are lacking.

The planned study addresses this research gap. It will contribute to the existing literature with field experiments in ecologically valid settings, investigating the acute effects of walking in greenspaces versus urban built environments with different road traffic noise exposures, employing a longitudinal design. Following the need for robust experimental examination of psychophysiological responses to greenspaces expressed by Bowler et al. [36], the study presented in this protocol examines the effects of exposure to greenspaces versus urban built environments, with minimum confounding effects associated with road traffic noise. In addition, it provides a factorial design to study the individual contributions of environment and road traffic noise to the psychophysiological responses. It thus compares the effects of walking in greenspaces versus urban built environments with high versus low traffic noise exposure.

The overarching aim of this study is to assess the restorative effect of exposure to different types of environments (forests and urban built) and to different road traffic noise levels in these sites (high and low traffic noise), thus providing an improved empirical foundation for: (1) the practical application of walking and nature-based interventions as low-threshold approaches for stress reduction and the promotion of wellbeing; (2) the effects of additional environmental qualities of greenspaces and urban built areas like noise as an impediment to recover from stress; and (3) urban planning and the construction and preservation of restorative environments that support the wellbeing of inhabitants. In this paper, the study protocol is presented, and we used the SPIRIT checklist when writing our report [59].

The planned study is embedded in the research project RESTORE (Restorative potential of green spaces in noise-polluted environments). The purpose of the RESTORE project is to study green spaces as facilitators for restoration and stress recovery, as well as road traffic noise as an impediment to recovering from stress. The study presented here is the basis of a dissertation project examining the acute psychophysiological effects of walking in different environments and with disparate inner attitudes. In addition to investigating how much individuals restore, depending on where they go for a walk (research focus 1, RF1; focus of this study protocol), the study also investigates the effects of how individuals go for a walk and which inner attitude they adopt while walking. Specifically, this dissertation project examines whether helping people be more mindful while walking in forests may increase the potential positive effects of walking in forests on restoration (research focus 2, RF2). The current study protocol, however, focuses on describing the planned experiment for RF1). First, by means of inspecting the effects of 30-min walks, we will examine whether walking in forest settings, compared to walking in urban built settings, leads to a stronger increase in stress reduction, restoration, positive affect, attention, nature connectedness, and rumination as well as a stronger decrease in negative affect. Second, we will assess whether walking in an environment with less road traffic noise, compared to walking in an environment with more road traffic noise, increases the above effects.

Methods

Study design

We will conduct a randomized, controlled intervention field study employing a pretest–posttest design to compare the effects of 30-min walks in urban built environments and forests with different road traffic noise levels. Healthy adults will be asked to go for a guided group walk with a maximum of six participants. The participants will be randomly assigned to one of the following conditions: urban built environment with high road traffic noise, urban built environment with low road traffic noise, forest with high road traffic noise, and forest with low road traffic noise. A fifth condition will be a walk in a forest with low traffic noise, in which participants walk with a mindfulness intervention. In this study protocol, we focus on the examination of the objective environmental factors, as noted above, and the last condition is not discussed here (above RF2). Data will be assessed before (t1) and directly after (t2) the walk. After this, participants will be instructed to walk individually three times during the subsequent 10 days. Ten days after participating in the field experiment, participants will receive an invitation to respond to a third questionnaire (t3). The third measurement point is only used for RF2 and is not included here; as for answering the research questions included in this study protocol, we focus only on measurement timepoints t1 and t2.

Research questions and hypotheses

Research question RQ1

Compared to walking in an urban built environment, does walking in a forest lead to a stronger increase in stress reduction (variable 1), restoration (variable 2), positive affect (variable 3), attention (variable 4), nature connectedness (variable 5), and rumination (variable 6) as well as a stronger decrease in negative affect (variable 7)?

Hypothesis H1

Compared to walking in an urban built environment, taking a walk in a forest leads to a stronger increase in stress reduction (1), restoration (2), positive affect (3), attention (4), nature connectedness (5), and rumination (6) as well as a stronger decrease in negative affect (7).

RQ2

Compared to walking in an environment with high traffic noise, does walking in an environment with low traffic noise lead to a stronger increase in the outcome variables 1–6 as well as a stronger decrease in negative affect?

H2

Compared to walking in an environment with high road traffic noise, taking a walk in an environment with low road traffic noise leads to a stronger increase in the outcome variables 1–6 as well as a stronger decrease in negative affect.

RQ3

Compared to walking in a forest with high road traffic noise, does walking in a forest with low road traffic noise lead to a stronger increase in the outcome variables 1–6 as well as a stronger decrease in negative affect?

H3

Compared to walking in a forest with high road traffic noise, taking a walk in a forest with low road traffic noise leads to a stronger increase in the outcome variables 1–6 as well as a stronger decrease in negative affect.

RQ4

Compared to walking in an urban built environment with high road traffic noise, does walking in an urban built environment with low road traffic noise lead to a stronger increase in the outcome variables 1–6 as well as a decrease in negative affect?

H4

Compared to walking in an urban built environment with high road traffic noise, taking a walk in an urban built environment with low road traffic noise leads to a stronger increase in the outcome variables 1–6 as well as a stronger decrease in negative affect.

RQ5

What is the effect of 30-min walks in the forest on outcome variables 1–6 compared to walking in urban built settings, depending on the noise in the settings?

Procedures

The field experiment is based on a between-subject design, and assessments consist of self-report questionnaires, physiological measures, an attention test, and objective noise measurements. In preparation for the main data collection, a pretest was conducted in February and March 2022 to test the questionnaire and determine if changes in the experimental procedure were required. The questionnaire and experimental procedure were adjusted in line with the outcomes of the pretest.

Walking sessions will take place between 2:00 p.m. and 5:30 p.m., since participants’ salivary cortisol levels will be assessed, and salivary cortisol levels have been shown to fluctuate systematically during the day [60,61,62]. Additionally, stronger mental fatigue during the daytime is assumed [63] and hence, a higher need for restoration [64].

Before participation, individuals will be given a short introduction to the research topic, their task in the experiment, privacy policies, and asked to give their written informed consent. Before the date of participation, individuals will be asked to refrain from consuming caffeine and from performing exhausting physical activity 2 h before their walking appointment. Furthermore, participants will be requested to refrain from going for a walk on the day of their walking appointment before their participation and not to consume any drugs 24 h before taking the walk.

Upon arrival at the test site, the participants’ salivary cortisol levels will be assessed to obtain a baseline stress level. Next, participants will complete a series of questionnaires. After this, participants will be equipped with wearable wristbands to assess their skin conductance during the walk. Then, the participants’ attention will be assessed using the Necker Cube Pattern Control Test [65, 66]. Before starting the walk, a 2-min baseline value of participants’ skin conductance will be obtained while the individuals are seated, and a second salivary cortisol probe will be taken directly before the walk starts. One of the two experimenters present in the test setting will then lead the participants at a moderate pace of around 4,7 km/h along a predetermined route. The second experimenter will stay at the starting point of the route and look out for the assessment devices. Skin conductance will be measured continuously during the walk. Furthermore, during every walk, the experimenter will measure the sound while walking about 6 m ahead of the participants. A sound level meter (XL2 by NTI Audio, Schaan, Liechtenstein) with a free-field measurement microphone, fulfilling class 1 environmental requirements according to IEC 61672, will be used. The microphone will be fixated onto a recording stick and carried in a backpack during the walk by the experimenter, resulting in a microphone height of approximately 1.7 m above the ground (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Audio recording

Participants will be asked to walk in silence and not to outpace the experimenter. After the walk, a third cortisol probe will be taken, the attention test will be conducted again, and participants will be asked to respond to the post-questionnaire. For an illustration of the study procedure, see Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Study procedure

Data will be collected in three waves: May to October 2022, August to October 2023, and in a third wave in 2024. Any substantial protocol modifications will be communicated to BMC Psychology and approved by the ethics committee.

Outcomes

Primary outcomes

The primary outcomes are “perceived restoration”, measured with a self-report questionnaire and salivary cortisol as a physiological stress marker.

Perceived restoration will be assessed using a slightly modified version of the restoration outcome scale (ROS) [67]. The questionnaire was put in past tense, and participants will be asked to what extent the walk contributed to three items concerning relaxation and calmness (e.g., “I feel calmer after being here”), one item reflecting attention restoration (“my concentration and alertness clearly increased”), and two items indicating clearing one’s thoughts (e.g., “I was able to forget my everyday worries here”). The questionnaire has been translated into German, including a back translation into English by the internal translator of the Swiss Federal Research Institute WSL. Items are rated on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (completely). The scale has been used in several previous studies examining the benefits of exposure to nature [14, 68, 69] and has been shown to have good internal consistency with a Cronbach’s alpha of .92 [67].

The physiological stress marker salivary cortisol is considered a reliable indicator for assessing hypothalamic pituitary adrenocortical (HPA) axis activity [62, 70]. Saliva samples will be collected using synthetic swabs (Sarstedt, Germany). For saliva collection, participants will place the swab in their mouth for 2 min before returning it to the salivette collection tube. The salivettes will be put in a portable cooler immediately after saliva collection and stored there until the end of the walk. When returning from the field, saliva samples are frozen and stored at − 20 °C until analysis. The samples will be analyzed by the laboratory of Professor Dr. Kirschbaum at the Technical University of Dresden, Germany, to determine cortisol levels. After thawing, the samples will be centrifuged at 3,000 rpm for 5 min to obtain a clear supernatant of low viscosity. Salivary concentrations are measured using a commercially available chemiluminescence immunoassay with high sensitivity (Tecan – IBL International, Hamburg, Germany; catalog number R62111).

Secondary outcomes

Self-report questionnaires

Affect

Positive and negative affect will be assessed using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule [71]. It consists of 20 items, providing separate scores for a positive affect scale (10 items, e.g., “active” and “enthusiastic”) and a negative affect scale (10 items, e.g., “nervous” and “distressed”). Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). Participants will be asked to indicate the extent to which they currently experience each of the feelings or emotions. The scale has been widely used in previous studies examining the benefits of exposure to nature and has been shown to be sensitive to change in this context [14, 15, 72,73,74]. A good internal consistency of the scale, with a Cronbach’s alpha of between .86 and .90 for positive affect and between .84 and .87 for negative affect, has been reported [71].

Rumination

Rumination will be assessed using the German version of the Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ; [75]). The PTQ measures content-independent repetitive negative thinking (RNT), which is referred to as rumination in research [76]. For 15 statements related to their process of thinking, the participants will rate how often they apply to them using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (almost always). Item examples of RNT are: “The same thoughts keep going through my mind again and again”, or “Thoughts come to my mind without me wanting them to.” The internal consistency and convergent validity of the PTQ have been shown to be good [75]. The PTQ was evaluated in nonclinical and clinical samples and shown to have a high internal consistency of Cronbach’s alpha = .94 for the nonclinical sample [75].

Nature relatedness

Nature relatedness will be measured with the 6-item short form of the Nature Relatedness Scale (NR-6; [77]). The construct of nature relatedness [76] is assumed to capture an individual’s interest in nature, fascination with nature, and desired contact with nature. Nisbet and Zelenski [77] understand nature relatedness as encompassing an awareness and understanding of all aspects of the natural world, transcending aesthetically appealing aspects. In the present study, we will apply the German version [78] of the NR-6 (e.g., “I always think about how my actions affect the environment,” “My relationship to nature is an important part of who I am,” and “My ideal vacation spot would be a remote, wilderness area”). The scale encompasses the affective and cognitive elements of the relationship with nature. Participants will be asked to respond to the items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The NR-6 scale has been shown to have good internal consistency, with a Cronbach’s alpha between .83 and .86 [77].

Connection to nature

Connection to nature will be assessed using the Love and Care for Nature Scale (LCS; [79]). The LCS measures the emotional components of the connection between humans and nature with reference to the two terms love and care for nature. The initial version of the LCS was developed by Perkins [79]. Perkins defines the construct of love and care for nature as “deep love and caring for nature which includes a clear recognition of nature’s intrinsic value as well as a personal sense of responsibility to protect it from harm” [79]. The LCS was chosen due to its affective focus on the relationship with nature. In the present study, we will use the 5-item version of the scale. Items (e.g., “I feel a deep love for nature”) are rated on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The scale has been shown to have good internal consistency, with a Cronbach’s alpha of .93 [79].

Satisfaction with life

Life satisfaction will be measured with the German General Life Satisfaction Short Scale (L-1; [80]); for the English version see Nießen an colleagues [81]). This constitutes a single item (“How satisfied are you at present, all in all, with your life?”), rated on an 11-point scale (0 = not satisfied at all to 10 = completely satisfied). A good test–retest reliability as well as high convergent validity with other multi-item scales measuring satisfaction with life have been shown for the L-1 [80].

Perceived restorative qualities of environments

The extent to which the different environments examined in this study have restorative qualities will be assessed using the Perceived Restorativeness Scale-11 (PRS-11) [82]. Pasini et al. [82] established an 11-item version of the scale, favoring a four-factor model with the factors “being away,” “fascination,” “coherence,” and “scope.” Results have shown the PRS-11 to discriminate among different environmental categories, with hills and lakes exhibiting the highest PRS scores and industrial zones holding the lowest scores [82]. The questionnaire has been translated into German, including a back translation into English by the internal translator of the Swiss Federal Research Institute WSL. Subjects rate the extent to which the items reflect their experience of the environment in which they have been walking with higher sum scores, indicating stronger perceived restorativeness. Information on the internal consistency for this scale is not available.

Stress recovery

In an additional question, using a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (completely), participants indicated to what extent the environment they walked through contributed to them being able to recover from stress.

Noise annoyance

Noise annoyance will be assessed with the 11-point International Commission on Biological Effects of Noise (ICBEN) [83], also recommended in Standard ISO/TS 15666 [84], assessing per noise source how much the participants felt bothered, disturbed, or annoyed by noise from the corresponding sources during the walk (road traffic noise, public transport, railway noise, aircraft noise, construction work noise, or noise from restaurants, bars, or leisure activities). Items are rated on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 10 (extremely). The scale is a standardized noise annoyance scale widely used in noise annoyance research, yielding internationally comparable measures of annoyance reactions in noise studies [83].

Acoustic impression of the soundscapes

The participants’ individual experience of soundscapes will be assessed in accordance with the International Organization for Standardization data collection and reporting requirements ISO/TS 12913–2 [85]. Participants will rate the extent to which the soundscape during the walk was perceived as pleasant, chaotic, vibrant, uneventful, calm, annoying, eventful, and monotonous on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (completely). Additionally, we added “loud” as a ninth attribute to rate on the same scale. Following ISO/TS 12913–2 [85], participants will further be asked to rate to what extent they heard the four following types of sounds during the walk: “traffic noise (e.g., cars, buses, trains, and airplanes),” “other noise (e.g., sirens, construction, industry, and loading of goods),” “sounds from human beings (e.g., conversation, laughter, children at play, and footsteps),” and “natural sounds (e.g., singing birds, flowing water, and wind in vegetation)”. Items will be rated on a 5-item Likert scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (dominates completely). In addition, participants will be asked to rate the overall sound environment during the walk on a 10-point Likert scale, ranging from very pleasant to very unpleasant.

Demographics

We will assess age, gender, highest education, main occupation, income, frequency of exposure to nature, previous mindfulness experience, and noise sensitivity. Noise sensitivity will be evaluated with one item, asking participants how strongly they agree with the statement “I am noise sensitive”. The item is rated on a 5-point rating scale ranging from 1 (do not agree at all) to 5 (very much agree). The approach to assess noise sensitivity with a single question was taken from Brink et al. [86], who introduced this concept. Furthermore, we will ask the participants to describe their emotions through the sounds they heard during the walk. Finally, they will be asked to what extent going for a walk helps them think through personal problems and how their thoughts change when they go for a walk.

Physiological outcome measure

Skin conductance

Skin conductance level (SCL) will be measured continuously throughout the experiment with Shimmer3 GSR + sensor wristbands [87]. Electrodes will be placed on the volar phalanges of two fingers of the non-dominant hand. In previous research, SCL was shown to be an effective indicator of physiological stress, and SCL variations reflect changes in arousal [88].

Cognitive test

Attention

Attention will be assessed with the Necker Cube Pattern Control Test [65, 66], subsequently referred to as the Necker Cube Test. Stevenson et al. [89] and Ohly et al. [90] identified cognitive domains sensitive to restorative processes through exposure to greenspaces. According to Stevenson et al. [89] measures of attention control might exhibit the most direct relation to effortful directed attention. Here, different tests assessing attention control were explored, and the Necker Cube Test was selected [65]. The Necker Cube Test is a three-dimensional wire frame drawing of a cube without depth cues, which can be seen from two different perspectives [65]. When observed for more than a few seconds, the cube spontaneously switches perspectives due to reversals of the fore- and background [65]. It is assumed that holding one perspective and avoiding reversals requires directed attentional capacities [65]. After having been familiarized with the functionality of the paper version of the test, the participants are instructed to hold one perspective as long as they can and indicate every time the pattern reverses. Changes in perspective occurring despite attempting to focus on one pattern are believed to rely on attentional fatigue [35]. The number of reversals is thus used as a measure for directed attention, with fewer shifts indicating better attention.

Test site selection and characterization

Selection of the test sites

First, we aimed to identify areas featuring a high variation in traffic noise at different times of the day to compare the benefits of walking in the same area with high and low traffic noise exposure. For this purpose, we analyzed traffic data from the city of Zurich, Switzerland, on the number of motorized individual vehicles as a proxy for traffic noise in the corresponding area [91]. The city of Zurich provides daily updated data on motorized individual vehicles in Zurich, measured at 97 measurement points of the traffic department (for the exact locations of the measurement points, see City of Zurich traffic department [92]). We randomly chose the working days of one week in 2021 (05.31.2021–06.04.2021) and calculated the mean number of vehicles per hour of the working days of this week from 8:00 a.m. to 8:00 p.m. We then checked the variances to identify the places with the highest hourly variation. Using this approach, we did not find settings with a stable time and a sufficiently high traffic noise variation of at least 10 dB between 8:00 a.m and 8:00 p.m. Thus, we decided to instead compare urban built and forest settings with widely constant noise conditions during the afternoon, featuring either high or low road traffic noise, which are as similar as possible between locations with respect to other characteristics besides noise (e.g., amount of vegetation, density of buildings, inclination, and presence of water).

To minimize the risk of systematically confounding loud and quiet settings with other variables that might potentially vary between settings with different noise levels (e.g., inclination in the walking route and flowing water in the surroundings), we selected three settings per condition in Zurich (three loud and three quiet urban built settings as well as three loud and three quiet forest settings). The test settings were selected stepwise. We incorporated geographic information system (GIS) data and GIS methods to select potential test sites and considered two important factors: road traffic noise and vegetation.

The road traffic noise data were obtained from the Swiss-wide noise database sonBASE for the year 2015 [93], and we used the mean exposure values for the daytime period between 6:00 a.m. and 10:00 p.m. SonBASE provides so-called rating sound levels (Lr) according to the Swiss Noise Abatement Ordiance (NAO) [94], with a spatial resolution of 10 × 10 m. The Lr corresponds to the A-weighted equivalent continuous sound pressure level (LAeq) on a yearly average for road traffic, railway and aircraft noise, potentially with level corrections depending on the noise source and traffic density (for details see NAO [94]). To choose potential settings with similar noise characteristics, road traffic noise was divided into three noise classes (N1: Lr of daytime < 35 dBA, N2: Lr of daytime at 35–45 dBA, N3: Lr of daytime > 45 dBA). Areas with railway and aircraft noise Lr > 30 dB were excluded. Vegetation was characterized using its height (vegetation height model, VHM, [95]) as well as its abundance expressed by the normalized difference vegetation index (NDVI) [96], with NDVI values ranging from − 1 to 1 (NDVI > 0 shows vegetation), and VHM values given in meters. We calculated the median and standard deviation of the NDVI from all available cloud-free Sentinel-2 satellite images available between April and October for the years 2016–2019. Three classes of vegetation types were distinguished. To build the classes, we aimed to distinguish areas of similar characteristics in terms of NDVI and VHM based on unsupervised classification, using the Iso Cluster [97] and Maximum Likelihood Classification tools [98]. The classes were as follows: G1: hardly green spaces (mostly artificial surfaces); NDVI median = 0.07–0.24, VHM median = 0 m; G2: green spaces with low vegetation (mostly grass, with possible single shrubs and very small trees); NDVI median = 0.55–0.66, VHM median = 0.25–0.43 m; G3: green spaces with abundant and high vegetation (composed of trees, shrubs, and grass); NDVI median = 0.67–0.70, VHM median = 17.71–23.06 m.

The combinations of the three noise-based and the three vegetation-based categories indicated the potential test settings to be explored. In the next step, potential test sites were explored further via satellite images, inspecting the size of the areas (large enough for 30-min walks), vegetation, the existence of continuous sidewalks in the urban built areas, density, and height of buildings. Areas with water bodies or bridges were excluded, and we avoided steep road or path sections to minimize differences in exercise intensity. Finally, the remaining settings were inspected on-site. To avoid differences in social interaction between the settings, places with construction work were excluded as were places likely to feature large numbers of people. Potential walking routes were developed for suitable settings, and the sound pressure levels during the entire 30-min routes of the potential test settings were recorded during various walks at different times of the afternoon to inspect if the settings differed sufficiently in terms of noise. We aimed at a difference of 10 dB between loud and quiet settings. All of these criteria were used only for identifying potential and selecting the final test sites. In the final analysis, the actual measured noise levels during the walks will be taken as inputs for the statistical analysis.

Characterization of the final test sites

Figure 3 shows the locations of the starting points of the walking routes in the final test sites in the city of Zurich. For the exact coordinates of the starting points, see the file on “Locations of the starting points of the walking routes in the test settings” in the Supporting Information. The GPS files of the walking routes can also be found in the supporting information.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Locations of the final test settings. 1) Wydlerweg, 8047 Zurich. 2) Albisriederstr., 8047 Zurich. 3) Schweighofstr. 8055 Zurich. 4) Rieterplatz, 8002 Zurich. 5) Saumackerstr., 8048 Zurich. 6) Langgrütstr. 137, 8047 Zurich. 7) Krähbühlweg, 8044 Zurich. 8) Bruderholzweg, 8053 Zurich. 9) Alte Regensdorferstr., 8049 Zurich. 10) Hermann-Trüb-Weg, 8044 Dübendorf. 11) Forsthausweg, 8044 Zurich. 12) Hungerbergstr., 8049 Zurich. Basemap: swisstopo. 2020; Orthophoto (SWISSIMAGE 10) [99]

After selecting the final test settings, we performed a post-hoc classification of their vegetation and sound characteristics in the final test settings. For predicting vegetation, we calculated NDVI and VHM along each walking route with 50-m wide buffer zones on both sides of the lines. Table 1 shows a visual impression of the test settings and gives information on the vegetation characteristics of the respective walking routes. The locations for the two pictures on each route were chosen to be as characteristic as possible in terms of the visual and acoustical impressions of the walk.

Table 1 Characterization of vegetation in the final test sites

Table 2 provides the acoustical and soundscape characteristics of the final settings. The first column shows the LAeq recorded on the walking routes before starting with the data collection. For an additional post hoc classification of the sound environment in the settings, the acoustic impression was evaluated for the two locations at each test site in the afternoons of the same week in 2023 by a civil servant not familiar with the research questions of the present study. The sound environment was evaluated three subsequent times for the last 5 min at every location. When analyzing the results of the sound evaluation, the mean and standard deviation of all six sound evaluations (2 locations × 3 replicate evaluations) in each setting were computed. Table 2 shows the results of the major items for evaluating the sound environment. An additional result of the post hoc classification of the sound environment was that, in urban built and forest settings with high road traffic noise, road traffic noise was judged the most dominant sound source in all settings. In urban built settings with low road traffic noise, road traffic noise or sounds from humans were judged as the most dominant sound source. In forest settings with little road traffic noise, natural sounds and sounds from other than the three previously mentioned sound sources were judged as most dominant. As intended, urban built settings with high road traffic noise showed a higher LAeq and were judged to feature a higher extent of traffic noise than urban built settings with low road traffic noise. Equally, forest settings with high road traffic noise showed a higher LAeq and were judged to feature a higher extent of traffic noise than forest settings with low road traffic noise. Generally, LAeq was higher in urban built settings, compared to forest settings and the extent of traffic noise was similar in urban built and forest settings. For urban built sites with (very) little vegetation and low noise, we could not find suitable environments with the dBA thresholds that we chose at the beginning. Thus, the thresholds had to be adapted to the environments present in the area of Zurich. This is because we strived for high external validity. Loud and quiet are thus defined depending on the environment. As expected, the extent of natural sounds was generally higher in forest settings, compared to urban built settings.

Table 2 Sound characterization of the final test sites

For including noise as a predictor in the final statistical model, we will calculate the percentage of time in which traffic noise is the dominating sound source during each walk as well as the LAeq during the time in which traffic noise is the dominating sound source after having completed data collection.

Participants and recruitment

Participants will be recruited by various means. A random sample of 7,000 people will be drawn from the residents’ registration office in the city of Zurich. The sample will be stratified over four age groups, 18–35, 35–50, 50–65, and > 65, to reach people of all ages. These participants will be invited to take part in the study by letter. In addition, students from the universities of Zurich (University of Zurich and Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich) will be recruited via e-mail and by lecturers, who will forward the invitation to their students. The invitation to participate will also be distributed via local newspapers, local web portals, as well as at some of the authors' institution Empa. Furthermore, participants will be recruited through advertising with invitation flyers in public places in Zurich and via a study webpage. Finally, participants will be invited via the snowball sampling method, in which the researchers reach out to their social networks to distribute the invitation in Zurich. Participants will receive a remuneration of 50 CHF. Additionally, two massage vouchers amounting to 120 CHF each will be raffled anonymously among all participants who have completed the t1, t2, and t3 questionnaires. Individuals will be eligible for participation if they: (a) are 18 years and older; (b) are physically able to walk for half an hour at a moderate pace; (c) have no diagnosed hearing problems; (d) do not take cortisone for medical reasons; and (e) have a BMI < 35. The study has been approved by the Ethics Committee of the Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich (Ref No. EK 2021-N-211 of 27.01.2022). The study results will be released to the participants and the public. All data will be identified with an identification number to maintain participant confidentiality. Access to the study data will be restricted to the study team, and all forms related to the study data will be kept in locked cabinets.

Sample size calculation

A statistical power calculation was conducted for the restoration outcome scale as the primary outcome. A previous study, employing a similar design to the current study, was conducted by Tyrväinen et al. [14], who compared the restorative effects of exposure to a forest, a park, or an urban built environment using the restoration outcome scale. Participants sat in the respective environment for 15 min, followed by a 30-min walk. Based on the mean scores and standard deviations for the restorative outcome scale before and after the walk in the urban built environment and in the forest, reported by Tyrväinen et al. [14] (mean ROS urban built before = 4.56 (SD = 0.89), mean ROS urban built after = 4.2 (SD = 0.97), mean ROS forest before = 4.6 (SD = 0.84), mean ROS forest after = 5.16 (SD = 0.82)), the power for the current study was calculated using a simulation-based approach. For the simulation, a linear mixed effects model was used, taking group effects and participant effects into account by including a random intercept for participant groups as well as for individual participants. The required number of groups was estimated for a desired statistical power of ≥ 80%, calculating the number of simulations with a significant interaction effect of environment × time using R [100]. Power calculation showed that 19 groups with 1–6 participants per group are required to identify statistical differences between the conditions, assuming a power of ≥ 80% and an alpha level of 0.05. Thus, to accomplish an equal number of groups in urban built environments and forests, we are aiming for a minimum of 10 groups in urban built environments and 10 in forests.

Randomization

Individuals eligible for participation can register for the study via a link to an online questionnaire using SoSci Survey [101]. Here, participants must confirm that they meet the inclusion criteria for participating in the study. Then, they will be randomly allocated to one of the study conditions (urban built with high road traffic noise, urban built with low road traffic noise, forest with high road traffic noise, forest with low road traffic noise, and forest with low traffic noise with a mindfulness intervention [the latter is not treated here]). Randomization will be conducted by the random generator within SoSci Survey, showing participants a link to an online scheduling tool [102] with free timeslots for the corresponding condition. Alternatively, participants can book a slot via e-mail or telephone. In this case, randomization is conducted by the study team staff clicking on the random generator within SoSci Survey and proposing the participant-free slots shown by the online scheduling tool in the respective condition.

Data processing and statistical analysis

We will analyze research questions 1–5 using linear mixed-effects models. We will conduct prespecified models comparing the change from t1 to t2 in the outcome measures between participants in the different conditions. Prior to the analyses, the distributions of all measures will be examined, and data will be checked for influential outliers. The model diagnostics of linear mixed-effects models will be examined to assure that model assumptions are met. Due to the large number of outcome variables and the distinct theoretical background of the outcome variables regarding exposure to nature, the results will be analyzed and published in separate papers. One paper will focus on restoration, stress, attention, and affect. A second paper will focus on rumination.

To include road traffic noise as a predictor in the statistical model, the audio files recorded during every walk will be checked by listening to every recoding and categorizing each one-second time period in which road traffic noise is the dominant sound source. We will then calculate the LAeq (equivalent continuous sound level) for the cumulative time periods in which traffic noise is the dominant sound source as well as the cumulative duration in seconds in which traffic noise is dominant. Next, we will calculate the proportion of time in which road traffic noise is not the dominant sound source in relation to the total time of the recording. Then, we will compute the LAE (sound exposure level) for the cumulative time periods in which traffic noise is dominant. Further, we will calculate the LAE for the time periods when traffic noise is not dominant, using 30 dB for times when traffic noise is not dominant. This will result in a LAE value including LAE when traffic noise is dominant and LAE for time periods when traffic noise is not dominant, as well as a corresponding LAeq value including times when traffic noise is dominant and not dominant. Noise will be included in the model as a combined predictor of LAE including times when traffic noise is dominant and not dominant and the proportion of time, during which traffic noise is not dominant or by using the LAeq including times when traffic noise is dominant and not dominant. We plan to conduct a first model with vegetation and noise as categorial predictors (forest-urban built; high-low traffic noise) and a second model including noise as a continuous and vegetation as a categorial predictor.

In both models, we will specify fixed effects for conditions (vegetation and noise) and time (before and after the walk). Further, we will include an interaction term between vegetation and noise, as well as a random intercept term accounting for individual differences (one per subject). Additionally, in both models, the group constituting the experimental unit in this study will be entered as a second random intercept (one per group) to account for the hierarchical structure of the data due to the organization of participants into different groups. Since we will be conducting group walks, individuals will be subsamples of the walking groups, and the treatment will be applied to groups, not to individual participants. Therefore, the following model will be used: \(outcome \sim vegetation\times noise (1 |\mathrm{ group}/\mathrm{ subject})\). If suitable, further exploratory models will be computed. For all analyses, we will set the significance level at .05 and analyses will be conducted with R [100] and R package lme4 [103].

Discussion

This article describes the development and design of a protocol for a randomized, controlled longitudinal intervention study, comparing the effects of walking in forests and urban built environments on restoration, considering road traffic noise exposure during the walks in the respective settings.

Our planned study has several methodological strengths, including a longitudinal design, a multi-method outcome assessment including physiological measures (e.g., salivary cortisol), an attention test, and self-report data. This study extends upon previous studies comparing the effects of walking in greenspaces versus urban built environments by controlling for the potential detrimental effects of different road traffic noise levels in the environments. Since previous studies on the effects of noise on stress have often been based on laboratory [44, 51, 55,56,57] or cross-sectional data [41, 43, 53, 54], this study contributes to emerging research in providing data from a longitudinal field study. Unlike previous studies, which only assessed noise at one representative point on each route in each environment [40], noise will be measured continuously throughout every walk to assess the effect of the effective noise in the actual moment of the walk.

Further, rather than focusing on restoration from a stressed or depleted state, we will explore the instorative effects of walking in different environments with participants who have not undergone a prior stress intervention [104, 105]. Many previous studies have examined restoration after stressing participants [68, 74, 106, 107], which makes it easier to find greater restoration effects because of a higher baseline level of stress and attentional fatigue [89, 104]. However, the aim of this study is to examine the potential of walking in different environments for the psychophysiological wellbeing of individuals with an ecologically valid amount of stress and the need for restoration in their everyday life, without an artificial induction of stress or fatigue. This is because we focus on exposure to greenspaces as a low-threshold approach in the general public, not merely focusing on reducing stress and restoring depleted attentional resources but also on fostering wellbeing.

This study also has some methodological limitations. First, while we expect the settings in noisy conditions to display higher mean noise values compared to relatively quiet conditions, mean noise levels in urban built settings are expected to be systematically louder than in forest settings. When beginning the process of selecting the test sites, we aimed to apply the same noise thresholds in terms of dBA for loud and quiet urban built and forest settings for the GIS analysis, as we wanted to compare these. During the selection process of the test settings, however, it became clear that defining loud and quiet depends on the context of the site, with mean noise levels in urban built environments being, on average, higher than in forest settings (see Table 2). This systematic difference between urban built environments and forests, however, if not explicitly accounted for in the statistical model, might result in a potential systematic confounding of noise level and setting category (urban built or forest). We therefore plan to account for this by including noise as a continuous predictor in the second model, as discussed above. However, this must be considered in the data analysis and interpretation of the study results.

Second, because noise features high situational variance, finding situations with widely constant noise conditions was a major challenge. Thus, the noise variance within each setting will have to be considered and discussed along with the final data analysis. The same also applies to seasonal differences in the vegetation and thus in VHM and NDVI.

Third, it is important to note that without further information on the type of sound source, dB values possess only limited potential to predict noise annoyance and consequently stress. The assessed noise levels during the walks will partly reflect sound from individual motorized traffic but also other sound sources, such as sounds from people, animals, wind, and moving leaves from trees. The research team, with expertise in both environmental and health psychology as well as acoustics research, carefully discussed these limitations during the study design process; however, there is no viable way to fully avoid them in a field study. Therefore, we plan to account for this by extracting the percentage of time, during which road traffic noise is the dominant sound source in the sound measurements of every walk, as well as by including the LAeq during the time when road traffic noise is the dominating sound source as predictors for the statistical model. Fourth, especially when measuring sound during the walks in the forest settings, the sound of the experimenter walking partly masks other, quieter, or more distant sounds. Thus, the LAeq from such sounds (e.g., natural sounds) during the walks cannot be extracted from the recordings. However, because traffic noise is usually louder than the walking sound, this will only be a minor source of uncertainty regarding the recorded road traffic LAeq during the walks.

Finally, for individuals working full time, who could be hypothesized to be the ones with the highest stress levels, it might be difficult to find a convenient, appropriate, or suitable time slot, since walking sessions only take place in the afternoon.

The results of this study will inform us about the restorative effects of exposure to different types of landscape environments and to different road traffic noise levels at these sites. Through the methodology of this study, this research not only adds to the body of literature on the topic, but also provides an improved foundation for practical applications. The study will add information about the benefits of walking and nature-based interventions as well as their potential to reduce stress and promote wellbeing, thus facilitating a better understanding of low-threshold interventions to prevent stress and foster wellbeing. In addition, the results will have the potential to inform noise legislation and the implementation of spatial planning acts.

Availability of data and materials

The data from the setting selection process is publicly available on the Open Science Framework under https://osf.io/2bfgv/. The final dataset of the study will be made publicly available in the results publications.

References

  1. Swiss Federal Statistical Office. Arbeitsbedingungen und Gesundheitszustand, 2012–2017: Schweizerische Gesundheitsbefragung (SGB) [Work conditions and health status, 2012–2017: Swiss Health Survey]. 2019. https://www.bfs.admin.ch/bfs/en/home/statistics/catalogues-databases.assetdetail.9366230.html. Accessed 15 Dec 2022.

  2. Swiss Federal Statistical Office. Stress und psychosoziale Risiken am Arbeitsplatz haben 2017 zugenommen [Stress and psychosocial risks increased in working life in 2017]. 2019. https://www.bfs.admin.ch/bfs/en/home/statistics/catalogues-databases.assetdetail.9366231.html. Accessed 5 Jan 2023.

  3. Osborne MT, Shin LM, Mehta NN, Pitman RK, Fayad ZA, Tawakol A. Disentangling the links between psychosocial stress and cardiovascular disease. Circ Cardiovasc Imaging. 2020;13:e010931.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Santosa A, Rosengren A, Ramasundarahettige C, Rangarajan S, Gulec S, Chifamba J, et al. Psychosocial risk factors and cardiovascular disease and death in a population-based cohort from 21 low-, middle-, and high-income countries. JAMA Netw Open. 2021;4:e2138920.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Steptoe A, Kivimäki M. Stress and cardiovascular disease. Nat Rev Cardiol. 2012;9:360–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Vancheri F, Longo G, Vancheri E, Henein MY. Mental stress and cardiovascular health—part I. J Clin Med. 2022;11:3353.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Kivimäki M, Steptoe A. Effects of stress on the development and progression of cardiovascular disease. Nat Rev Cardiol. 2018;15:215–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Black J, Sweeney L, Yuan Y, Singh H, Norton C, Czuber-Dochan W. Systematic review: the role of psychological stress in inflammatory bowel disease. Aliment Pharmacol Ther. 2022;56:1235–49.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Landsbergis PA, Dobson M, Koutsouras G, Schnall P. Job strain and ambulatory blood pressure: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Am J Public Health. 2013;103:e61–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Hackett RA, Steptoe A. Type 2 diabetes mellitus and psychological stress - a modifiable risk factor. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2017;13:547–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Bergmann N, Gyntelberg F, Faber J. The appraisal of chronic stress and the development of the metabolic syndrome: a systematic review of prospective cohort studies. Endocr Connect. 2014;3:R55–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Franks KH, Bransby L, Saling MM, Pase MP. Association of stress with risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis JAD. 2021;82:1573–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Spruill TM. Chronic psychosocial stress and hypertension. Curr Hypertens Rep. 2010;12:10–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Tyrväinen L, Ojala A, Korpela K, Lanki T, Tsunetsugu Y, Kagawa T. The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment. J Environ Psychol. 2014;38:1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Mayer FS, Frantz CM, Bruehlman-Senecal E, Dolliver K. Why is nature beneficial?: The role of connectedness to nature. Environ Behav. 2009;41:607–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Martens D, Gutscher H, Bauer N. Walking in “wild” and “tended” urban forests: The impact on psychological well-being. J Environ Psychol. 2011;31:36–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM. Underestimating nearby nature: affective forecasting errors obscure the happy path to sustainability. Psychol Sci. 2011;22:1101–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Bratman GN, Gretchen CD, Benjamin JL, James JG. The benefits of nature experience: Improved affect and cognition. Landsc Urban Plan. 2015;138:41–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Elsadek M, Liu B, Lian Z, Xie J. The influence of urban roadside trees and their physical environment on stress relief measures: A field experiment in Shanghai. Urban For Urban Green. 2019;42:51–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. WHO Regional Office for Europe. Burden of disease from environmental noise: quantification of healthy life years lost in Europe. Copenhagen: World Health Organization. Regional Office for Europe; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  21. European Environment Agency. Environmental noise in Europe, 2020. LU: Publications Office; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Twohig-Bennett C, Jones A. The health benefits of the great outdoors: A systematic review and meta-analysis of greenspace exposure and health outcomes. Environ Res. 2018;166:628–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Maas J, Verheij RA, Groenewegen PP, de Vries S, Spreeuwenberg P. Green space, urbanity, and health: how strong is the relation? J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60:587–92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. McCormick R. Does access to green space impact the mental well-being of children: A systematic review. J Pediatr Nurs. 2017;37:3–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Roe J, Thompson C, Aspinall P, Brewer M, Duff E, Miller D, et al. Green space and stress: evidence from cortisol measures in deprived urban communities. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2013;10:4086–103.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Wood L, Hooper P, Foster S, Bull F. Public green spaces and positive mental health – investigating the relationship between access, quantity and types of parks and mental wellbeing. Health Place. 2017;48:63–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Beyer KMM, Kaltenbach A, Szabo A, Bogar S, Nieto FJ, Malecki KM. Exposure to neighborhood green space and mental health: evidence from the survey of the health of Wisconsin. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2014;11:3453–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Sugiyama T, Leslie E, Giles-Corti B, Owen N. Associations of neighbourhood greenness with physical and mental health: do walking, social coherence and local social interaction explain the relationships? J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008;62:e9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Vienneau D, de Hoogh K, Faeh D, Kaufmann M, Wunderli JM, Röösli M, et al. More than clean air and tranquillity: Residential green is independently associated with decreasing mortality. Environ Int. 2017;108:176–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Schäffer B, Brink M, Schlatter F, Vienneau D, Wunderli JM. Residential green is associated with reduced annoyance to road traffic and railway noise but increased annoyance to aircraft noise exposure. Environ Int. 2020;143:105885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Van Renterghem T. Towards explaining the positive effect of vegetation on the perception of environmental noise. Urban For Urban Green. 2019;40:133–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Ulrich RS. Aesthetic and Affective Response to Natural Environment. In: Altman I, Wohlwill JF, editors. Behavior and the Natural Environment. Boston, MA: Springer, US; 1983. p. 85–125.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  33. Ulrich RS, Simons RF, Losito BD, Fiorito E, Miles MA, Zelson M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J Environ Psychol. 1991;11:201–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Kaplan R, Kaplan S. The experience of nature: A psychological perspective. Cambridge; 1989.

  35. Kaplan S. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. J Environ Psychol. 1995;15:169–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Bowler DE, Buyung-Ali LM, Knight TM, Pullin AS. A systematic review of evidence for the added benefits to health of exposure to natural environments. BMC Public Health. 2010;10:456.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Hartig T, Evans GW, Jamner LD, Davis DS, Gärling T. Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J Environ Psychol. 2003;23:109–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Ulrich RS, Miles MA, Simons RF. Effects of environmental simulations and television on blood donor stress. J Archit Plan Res. 2003;20:38–47.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Lee J, Park B-J, Tsunetsugu Y, Kagawa T, Miyazaki Y. Restorative effects of viewing real forest landscapes, based on a comparison with urban landscapes. Scand J For Res. 2009;24:227–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Gidlow CJ, Jones MV, Hurst G, Masterson D, Clark-Carter D, Tarvainen MP, et al. Where to put your best foot forward: Psycho-physiological responses to walking in natural and urban environments. J Environ Psychol. 2016;45:22–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Meister EA, Donatelle RJ. The impact of commercial-aircraft noise on human health: A neighborhood study in Metropolitan Minnesota. J Environ Health. 2000;63:9–15.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Monteiro R, Tomé D, Neves P, Silva D, Rodrigues M. The interactive effect of occupational noise on attention and short-term memory: A pilot study. Noise Health. 2018;20:190–8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Jensen HAR, Rasmussen B, Ekholm O. Neighbour and traffic noise annoyance: a nationwide study of associated mental health and perceived stress. Eur J Public Health. 2018;28:1050–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Ellermeier W, Kattner F, Klippenstein E, Kreis M, Marquis-Favre C. Short-term noise annoyance and electrodermal response as a function of sound-pressure level, cognitive task load, and noise sensitivity. Noise Health. 2020;22:46–55.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Babisch W, Pershagen G, Selander J, Houthuijs D, Breugelmans O, Cadum E, et al. Noise annoyance–a modifier of the association between noise level and cardiovascular health? Sci Total Environ. 2013;452–453:50–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Lefèvre M, Carlier M-C, Champelovier P, Lambert J, Laumon B, Evrard A-S. Effects of aircraft noise exposure on saliva cortisol near airports in France. Occup Environ Med. 2017;74:612–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Ohrström E. Longitudinal surveys on effects of changes in road traffic noise-annoyance, activity disturbances, and psycho-social well-being. J Acoust Soc Am. 2004;115:719–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Stokholm ZA, Hansen ÅM, Grynderup MB, Bonde JP, Christensen KL, Frederiksen TW, et al. Recent and long-term occupational noise exposure and salivary cortisol level. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;39:21–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Michaud DS, Feder K, Keith SE, Voicescu SA, Marro L, Than J, et al. Self-reported and measured stress related responses associated with exposure to wind turbine noise. J Acoust Soc Am. 2016;139:1467–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Michaud DS, Keith SE, Feder K, Voicescu SA, Marro L, Than J, et al. Personal and situational variables associated with wind turbine noise annoyance. J Acoust Soc Am. 2016;139:1455–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Miki K, Kawamorita K, Araga Y, Musha T, Sudo A. Urinary and salivary stress hormone levels while performing arithmetic calculation in a noisy environment. Ind Health. 1998;36:66–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Tafalla RJ, Evans GW. Noise, physiology, and human performance: The potential role of effort. J Occup Health Psychol. 1997;2:148–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Gidlöf-Gunnarsson A, Öhrström E. Noise and well-being in urban residential environments: The potential role of perceived availability to nearby green areas. Landsc Urban Plan. 2007;83:115–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Öhrström E, Skånberg A, Svensson H, Gidlöf-Gunnarsson A. Effects of road traffic noise and the benefit of access to quietness. J Sound Vib. 2006;295:40–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Alvarsson JJ, Wiens S, Nilsson ME. Stress recovery during exposure to nature sound and environmental noise. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2010;7:1036–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Annerstedt M, Jönsson P, Wallergård M, Johansson G, Karlson B, Grahn P, et al. Inducing physiological stress recovery with sounds of nature in a virtual reality forest–results from a pilot study. Physiol Behav. 2013;118:240–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Ellermeier W, Kattner F, Klippenstein E, Kreis M, Marquis-Favre C. Subjective and electrodermal responses to annoying vehicle sounds: Role of task load and noise sensitivity. In: Proceedings of the 23rd International Congress on Acoustics. Aachen; 2019;9–13 . 

  58. Barbaresco GQ, Reis AVP, Lopes GDR, Boaventura LP, Castro AF, Vilanova TCF, et al. Effects of environmental noise pollution on perceived stress and cortisol levels in street vendors. J Toxicol Environ Health A. 2019;82:331–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Chan A-W, Tetzlaff JM, Gøtzsche PC, Altman DG, Mann H, Berlin JA, et al. SPIRIT 2013 explanation and elaboration: guidance for protocols of clinical trials. BMJ. 2013;346:e7586.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Ljubijankić N, Popović-Javorić R, Šćeta S, Šapčanin A, Tahirović I, Sofić E. Daily fluctuation of cortisol in the saliva and serum of healthy persons. Bosn J Basic Med Sci. 2008;8:110–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:355–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Von Dawans B, Kirschbaum C, Heinrichs M. The trier social stress test for groups (TSST-G): A new research tool for controlled simultaneous social stress exposure in a group format. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:514–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Meier-Koll A. Biological rhythms in the anthropological context. Regensburg: Roderer; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Hartig T, Staats H. The need for psychological restoration as a determinant of environmental preferences. J Environ Psychol. 2006;26:215–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. De Young R. Necker Cube Test. 2022. https://seas.umich.edu/eplab/demos/nt0/neckerintro.html. Accessed 7 Jul 2021.

  66. Cimprich B. Development of an intervention to restore attention in cancer patients. Cancer Nurs. 1993;16:83–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Korpela KM, Ylén M, Tyrväinen L, Silvennoinen H. Determinants of restorative experiences in everyday favorite places. Health Place. 2008;14:636–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Pasanen T, Johnson K, Lee K, Korpela K. Can nature walks with psychological tasks improve mood, self-reported restoration, and sustained attention? Results from two experimental field studies. Front Psychol. 2018;9:2057.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Ojala A, Korpela K, Tyrväinen L, Tiittanen P, Lanki T. Restorative effects of urban green environments and the role of urban-nature orientedness and noise sensitivity: A field experiment. Health Place. 2019;55:59–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Noise and stress-salivary cortisol as a non-invasive measure of allostatic load. Noise Health. 1999;1:57.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54:1063–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Berman MG, Jonides J, Kaplan S. The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol Sci. 2008;19:1207–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Berman MG, Kross E, Krpan KM, Askren MK, Burson A, Deldin PJ, et al. Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. J Affect Disord. 2012;140:300–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Olafsdottir G, Cloke P, Schulz A, Van Dyck Z, Eysteinsson T, Thorleifsdottir B, et al. Health benefits of walking in nature: A randomized controlled study under conditions of real-life stress. Environ Behav. 2020;52:248–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Ehring T, Zetsche U, Weidacker K, Wahl K, Ehlers A. The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): Validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2011;42:225–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM, Murphy SA. The nature relatedness scale: linking individuals’ connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environ Behav. 2009;41:715–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM. The NR-6: a new brief measure of nature relatedness. Front Psychol. 2013;4:813.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Dornhoff M, Sothmann J-N, Fiebelkorn F, Menzel S. Nature relatedness and environmental concern of young people in Ecuador and Germany. Front Psychol. 2019;10:453.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Perkins HE. Measuring love and care for nature. J Environ Psychol. 2010;30:455–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Beierlein C, Kovaleva A, László Z, Kemper CJ, Rammstedt B. Kurzskala zur Erfassung der Allgemeinen Lebenszufriedenheit (L-1). Zusammenstellung Sozialwissenschaftlicher Items Skalen ZIS. 2015. https://doi.org/10.6102/ZIS229.

  81. Nießen D, Groskurth K, Rammstedt B, Lechner CM. General Life Satisfaction Short Scale (L-1). ZIS - Collect Items Scales Soc Sci. 2020. https://doi.org/10.6102/ZIS284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Pasini M, Berto R, Brondino M, Hall R, Ortner C. How to measure the restorative quality of environments: The PRS-11. Procedia - Soc Behav Sci. 2014;159:293–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Fields JM, De Jong RG, Gjestland T, Flindell IH, Job RFS, Kurra S, et al. Standardized general-purpose noise reaction questions for community noise surveys: research and a recommendation. J Sound Vib. 2001;242:641–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. International Organization for Standardization. ISO/TS 15666. Technical specification: Acoustics – assessment of noise annoyance by means of social and socio-acoustic surveys. 2nd ed. Geneva: International Organization for Standardization; 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  85. International Organization for Standardization. ISO/TS 12913–2:2018 Acoustics - Soundscape - Part 2: Date collection and reporting requirements. Geneva: International Organization for Standardization; 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  86. Brink M, Schäffer B, Vienneau D, Foraster M, Pieren R, Eze IC, et al. A survey on exposure-response relationships for road, rail, and aircraft noise annoyance: Differences between continuous and intermittent noise. Environ Int. 2019;125:277–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Shimmer Wearable Sensor Technology. 2022.

  88. Braithwaite JJ, Watson DG, Jones R, Rowe M. A guide for analysing electrodermal activity (EDA) & skin conductance responses (SCRs) for psychological experiments. Tecnical report. Birmingham: Selective Attention & Awareness Laboratory Behavioural Brain Seciences Centre, University of Birmingham; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  89. Stevenson MP, Schilhab T, Bentsen P. Attention Restoration Theory II: a systematic review to clarify attention processes affected by exposure to natural environments. J Toxicol Environ Health Part B. 2018;21:227–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Ohly H, White MP, Wheeler BW, Bethel A, Ukoumunne OC, Nikolaou V, et al. Attention Restoration Theory: A systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. J Toxicol Environ Health Part B. 2016;19:305–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. City of Zürich. Daten der Verkehrszählung zum motorisierten Individualverkehr (Stundenwerte) [data on hourly number of motorized individual vehicles]. 2021. https://data.stadt-zuerich.ch/dataset/sid_dav_verkehrszaehlung_miv_od2031/download/sid_dav_verkehrszaehlung_miv_OD2031_2021.csv. Accessed 16 Jun 2021.

  92. City of Zürich traffic department. Zählstellenplan [plan of measurement points]. 2020. https://data.stadt-zuerich.ch/dataset/sid_dav_verkehrszaehlung_miv_od2031/download/Zaehlstellenplan.pdf. Accessed 5 Apr 2021.

  93. Federal Office for the Environment FOEN. sonBASE GIS noise database. 2020. https://www.bafu.admin.ch/bafu/en/home/topics/noise/state/gis-laermdatenbank-sonbase.html. Accessed 21 Sept 2022.

  94. Noise Abatement Ordinance (NAO). Lärmschutz-Verordnung (LSV) vom 15. Dezember 1986 (Stand am 1. November 2023) [Noise Abatement Ordinance (NAO) of 15 December 1986 (Status as of 1 November 2023)]. SR 814.41. 2023. https://www.fedlex.admin.ch/eli/cc/1987/338_338_338/. Accessed 21 Sept 2023.

  95. Ginzler C, Hobi ML. Countrywide stereo-image matching for updating digital surface models in the framework of the Swiss National Forest Inventory. Remote Sens Basel Switz. 2015;7:4343–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Drusch M. Sentinel-2: ESA’s optical high-resolution mission for GMES Operational Services. Remote Sens Environ. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rse.2011.11.026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Esri ArcGis. How Iso Cluster works. https://pro.arcgis.com/en/pro-app/latest/tool-reference/spatial-analyst/how-iso-cluster-works.htm. Accessed 21 Feb 2022.

  98. Esri ArcGis. How Maximum Likelihood Classification works. https://pro.arcgis.com/en/pro-app/latest/tool-reference/spatial-analyst/how-maximum-likelihood-classification-works.htm. Accessed 5 Mar 2022.

  99. Federal Office of Topography swisstopo. SWISSIMAGE. Orthophoto (SWISSIMAGE 10) © 2020 swisstopo. 2020. https://www.swisstopo.admin.ch/en/geodata/images/ortho/swissimage10.html. Accessed 20 Dec 2023.

  100. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version R 4.0.2). 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  101. Leiner D. SoSci survey. 2023.

    Google Scholar 

  102. eTermin GmbH - Online-Terminplaner und Online-Terminbuchung. (n.d.). https://www.etermin.net. Accessed 7 Sept 2023.

  103. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67:1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Hartig T, Böök A, Garvill J, Olsson T, Gärling T. Environmental influences on psychological restoration. Scand J Psychol. 1996;37:379–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Korpela KM, Ratcliffe E. Which is primary: Preference or perceived instoration? J Environ Psychol. 2021;75:101617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Gatersleben B, Andrews M. When walking in nature is not restorative—The role of prospect and refuge. Health Place. 2013;20:91–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Hartig T, Mang M, Evans GW. Restorative effects of natural environment experiences. Environ Behav. 1991;23:3–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank the study participants of the pretest (February and March 2022) for their willingness to take part in this research.

Funding

This research, undertaken as part of the RESTORE project, is funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNF Research Grant Number CRSII5_193847). Contact: Swiss National Science Foundation, Wildhainweg 3, Postfach 8232, CH-3001 Bern. Interdisciplinary research funding. Phone: + 41 31 308 22 22. Email: sinergia@snf.ch. Additionally, Karin Hediger is funded through an Eccellenza grant (PCEFP1_194591) from the Swiss National Science Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JS and NB designed and conceptualized the study, described in this protocol. JS developed the methodology, collects and analyzes the data, and conducts the investigation. NB substantially supervised the whole process; KH gave additional supervising input. JS wrote the original draft of this manuscript. NK provided the GIS maps on which the search for potential test settings was initially based and provided further GIS-related support. NB and ST acquired funding. BS and J-MW provided substantial input on the noise-related methodology and noise assessment in the study. Review and editing: BS, J-MW, NB, KH, ST, and NK. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Julia Schaupp.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Swiss Federal Institute of Technology in Zurich (ETH), Ref No. EK 2021-N-211 (27.01.2022). Participation in this study is voluntary; all participants are provided with information about the study and have an opportunity to ask questions about the study. Participants sign a informed consent document and are provided with information about the potential risks of participation. Participants are allowed to withdraw from the study at any time and for any reason.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Locations of the starting points of the walking routes in the test settings.

Additional file 2.

Data on LAeq from noise recordings of walking routes.

Additional file 3.

Data on the evaluation of acoustic impression of the test settings.

Additional file 4.

Participant information sheet.

Additional file 5.

Participant consent form.

Additional file 6.

Completed SPIRIT checklist.

Additional file 7.

GPS tracks of walking routes.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schaupp, J., Hediger, K., Wunderli, JM. et al. Psychophysiological effects of walking in forests and urban built environments with disparate road traffic noise exposure: study protocol of a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychol 12, 250 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01720-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01720-x

Keywords