Skip to main content

Insular cortex involvement in declarative memory deficits in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder

Abstract

Background

Neuroimaging studies have proved that hippocampus relate to the deficient of memory in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Many studies in healthy subjects also shown that insular cortex (IC) be involved in the declarative memory. This study was designed to investigate whether insular cortex is involved in declarative memory deficits in patients with PTSD.

Methods

Twelve subjects with PTSD and 12 subjects without PTSD victims underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging and magnetic resonance imaging. All subjects performed encoding and retrieval memory tasks during the fMRI session. Voxel-based morphometry method was used to analyze gray-matter volume, and the Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM2) was used to analyze activated brain areas when performing tasks.

Results

Grey matter volume was significantly reduced bilaterally in the insular cortex of PTSD subjects than non-PTSD. PTSD group also had lower level of activation in insular cortex when performing word encoding and retrieval tasks than non-PTSD group.

Conclusion

The study provides evidence on structural and function abnormalities of the insular cortex in patients with PTSD. Reduced grey-matter volume in insular cortex may be associated with declarative memory deficits in patients with PTSD.

Peer Review reports

Background

The insular cortex (IC) is a region located in the centre of the cerebral hemisphere. It processes sensory input in all modalities: gustatory, olfactory, auditory, visual and somatosensory [1, 2]. Although IC is considered primarily as a taste area and is involved in conditional taste aversion and taste recognition, some studies demonstrated the involvement of IC in face recognition, tactile recognition and working memory[3, 4]. Results of two animal studies also suggest that the IC is involved in declarative memory. For instance, Bermudez-Rattoni reported that IC is involved in consolidation of memory, and the study by Miranda suggested that cholinergic transmission in the IC is necessary for the acquisition and consolidation of contextual memory[5, 6].

Studies on PTSD suggest a specific association between the traumatic stress and changes in memory functions [7–9]. Patients with PTSD may suffer from long-term memory deficits. In Archibald and Tuddenham's follow-up study, many veterans of World War II still suffered from episodes of 'black-outs' or impairment of explicit memory[10]. Intrusive memories and impoverished memories are common complaints among patients with PTSD[7]. Intrusive memories are diagnostic symptoms in patients with PTSD; they were easily triggered by ordinary stimuli such as low-flying airplane or loud noise, or anything that relives any aspect of the traumatic event. These intrusive memories are accompanied by autonomic hyperarousal that may be experienced as reenactments of the original trauma (flashbacks)[11]. Impoverished memory includes deficits in declarative memory, fragmentation of memories, and trauma-related amnesia[7]. Declarative memory (explicit memory) refers to the ability to consciously remember and reproduce events and facts. Some studies demonstrated declarative memory deficits in PTSD [12–15].

IC may be involved in declarative memory deficits in patients with PTSD. Previous studies indicate that hippocampus plays an important role in the declarative memory deficits in PTSD, and most neuroimaging studies on patients with PTSD showed hippocampal atrophy[16]. Apart from hippocampal involvement, studies have shown involvement of other areas in memory processing. For instance, functional imaging studies on healthy subjects have shown that prefrontal cortex, medial temporal lobe (MTL) and cerebellum are active during the encoding process of episodic memory; while prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), MTL, occipital lobe are active during the retrieval process of declarative memory [17–21]. Recent studies using fMRI (functional Magnetic resonance imaging) and PET (Positron Emission Tomography) have shown that IC is involved in higher cognitive functions. Activities that involve social interactions (competition and cooperation) are associated with increased activation in the anterior IC (BA13) [22]. When implementing tasks of declarative memory such as encoding and retrieving verbal or picture materials, the IC of normal subjects shows high level of activation [23–27]. In patients with schizophrenia, the activation of IC was lower than healthy subjects when they implemented the declarative memory task [28]. Taken together, these studies suggest that IC might be involved in declarative memory.

While results from animal studies and human neuroimaging studies support the involvement of IC in the declarative memory in healthy subjects, there is paucity of evidence on the relationship between IC and declarative memory deficit in patients with PTSD. Using structural and functional MRI, we aimed to examine the structural and functional differences of IC in surviving victims of a fire disaster with and without PTSD. Based on previous findings, we hypothesize that structural and functional changes in IC of patients with PTSD may be associated with deficits in declarative memory. To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first study to report on the relationship between the IC and declarative memory deficits in patients with PTSD.

Methods

Participants

Twelve patients (8 females and 4 males) with PTSD, and twelve subjects (8 females and 4 males) without PTSD, were recruited. All of them were recruited from 157 victims surviving a fire disaster occurred in November 2003 in Hunan province in China. After the PTSD screening and diagnostic program, 21 patients with PTSD were found and 15 patients consented to be recruited in this study, during the neuroimaging test, 3 patients dropped out. So the final subjects were 12 in the PTSD patients group.

We established PTSD diagnoses using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV[29] (First et al, 1995) and assessed the severity of PTSD using the Chinese version of the Distress Event Questionnaire (DEQ) [30, 31]. The two groups did not differ significantly in age (34.56 years ± 4.91 [PTSD] and 33.25 years ± 5.27 [non-PTSD]; t[22] = -0.49, p = 0.68). The PTSD group had higher scores on DEQ than did the non-PTSD group (43.12 ± 5.61 [PTSD] and 12.58 ± 4.92 [non-PTSD]; t[22] = 4.46, p = 0.000). The presence of other psychiatric disorders was also assessed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First et al 1995). No subject met the diagnostic criteria for major depression, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, alcohol and substance abuse. Subjects were excluded if they had any clinical significant abnormality of a clinical laboratory test, a history of psychiatric illness or neurological dysfunction, a history of alcohol and/or drug abuse (DSM-IV criteria) within 6 months prior to the study, or claustrophobia. None of the participants were taking psychotropic drugs at the time of the study.

The following comorbid DSM-IV Diagnoses were found in the PTSD group: dysthymia (n = 2), specific phobia (n = 1), and generalized anxiety disorder (n = 3). None of the subjects in the non-PTSD group had current diagnoses. In both groups, no subject (with or without PTSD) had ever received psychiatric treatment for PTSD caused by the fire disaster. Moreover, none of the subjects had received any psychotropic treatment.

Verbal informed consent was obtained from each participant before participation because most victims didn't want to sign any paperwork. The Institutional Review Board of Central South University Xiangya Medical School approved this study in writing, and accepted the switch from written informed consent to the verbal informed consent.

Tasks

Block design was used in the present study. Subjects were imaged during two functional runs while performing these encoding tasks. Each functional run lasted 300 sec and was comprised of 10 blocks, 5 of these were "task" blocks and 5 were "fixation" control blocks. During the fixation control blocks, a cross-hair (plus sign) was present on the screen for the duration of the block, and subjects were instructed to fixate the cross-hair. Task blocks (30 second duration) were interleaved with fixation blocks (30 second duration). 10 sets of Chinese words were presented during each task block (2000 msec stimulus duration, 1000 msec inter-stimulus interval). During the trial, subjects viewed a set of Chinese words (one pair of Chinese characters per trial) displayed simultaneously on a screen. One of the two characters in each pair was highlighted with a red arrow placed under the word. Subjects were instructed to remember words with a marker (target words) so that they would recognize it in the next task. Target words were randomly placed on the left or right screen. Subjects indicated their recognition of the target words using a keypad consisting of two horizontally arranged buttons, labeled as No. 1(left) and No. 2 (right). When the target word appeared on the left, subjects were instructed to press button No. 1.; vice-versa when the target word appeared on the right. The behavioral data (i.e., response times and percent of correct response) were recorded by the computer program.

Two seconds after the encoding scan, subjects were given an old-new recognition test. This trial consisted of 25 encoded words and 25 "new" words that were not presented during the encoding trial. There were also 5 "task" blocks and 5 "fixation" control blocks. The control block was presented in the same way as in the encoding trial. During the task block, one Chinese word was presented on the screen every time (2000 msec stimulus duration, 1000 msec inter-stimulus interval). 5 target words and 5 "new" words were presented in one task block. Each task block and control block lasted 30 seconds. Subjects indicated that an old word was presented by pressing button No. 1, or else, subjects pressed button No. 2,

Imaging acquisition

The tasks were run on a PC laptop using E-Prime presentation software (Psychology Software Tools) and displayed to the subjects using a color LCD projector (Epson, ELP-7000). Stimuli on the screen were visible to the subject via a mirror (1.5 in × 3 in) positioned approximately 15 cm above the subject's eyes. Functional MRI data were collected with a 1.5-T whole-body scanner (General Electric Medical Systems Signa, Milwaukee, WI) with a standard head coil. Cushions were used to minimize head movement. Anatomic images were acquired using a high-resolution 3-D spoiled gradient recovery sequence (SPGR, slice thickness 1 mm, TR = 25 msec, TE = 6 msec, flip angle = 258, matrix = 256 × 128, FOV 24 × 24 cm). Functional data were acquired using a gradient-echo EPI pulse sequence (GRE-EPI, TR = 3 s, TE = 60 ms, matrix = 64 × 64, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 24 × 24 cm, slice thickness = 5 mm, skip between slices = 1.5 mm).

fMRI and MRI Data Analyses

Data were analyzed with statistical parametric mapping (SPM2 software from the Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK), running under Matlab 6.0 (Mathworks, Sherbon, MA).

fMRI data

The fMRI data were realigned, spatially normalized to the standard brain space, and smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM). Low-frequency noise and global changes in activity were further removed. For each participant, task-specific effects were estimated using a general linear model (modeled as a box-car function convolved with the canonical homodynamic response function). For random effects analysis, a contrast image between tasks and control was generated for each participant and used for intersubject comparisons.

2.6 MRI data

Automated voxel-based morphometry (VBM) method was used in order to minimize operational biases when comparing the Neuroanatomical differences between patients with PTSD and the control subjects, [32, 33]. VBM was used recently in structural MRI studies of various neuropsychiatric disorders [34]. This method was used by Yamasue (2003)[35] in another study on PTSD. The MRI data were first spatially normalized into the standard space of Talairach and Tournoux [36]. Second, normalized images were segmented into gray matter, white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, and skull scalp compartments by using an automated process. Third, the spatially normalized segments of the gray and white matters were smoothed with a 12-mm full-width, half-maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel to accommodate individual variability. Then, the partial-volume effect was used to create a spectrum of gray or white matter intensities by smoothing the data, Gray or white matter density is equivalent to the weighted average of the gray or white-matter voxel located in the volume defined by the smoothing kernel, and according to previous studies[33, 34, 37], the regional gray or white matter density can be considered to represent the local amount of gray or white matter.

Statistical Analysis

For fMRI and MRI data, using an analysis of Two-sample t-test model running in SPM2, the significance level was set at corrected p < 0.05, performance between PTSD and controls was statistically compared. Furthermore, to rule out potential confounding factors that may affect fMRI and VBM findings, Simple Regression Analysis (in SPM 2) was also performed taking into account symptom measures and demographic data (age, gender, and years of education.) in victims with and without PTSD separately. Statistical significance was defined at corrected P < 0.05.

Results

Performance

Performance data are summarized in Additional file 1. Behavioral measures of accuracy (retrieval task) and reaction time (encoding and retrieval tasks) were acquired for all subjects. In encoding task, there were no differences in reaction time between PTSD patients and comparison subjects (Z = 0.52, p > 0.05) (see Additional file 1). In the retrieval task, patients tend to have lower accuracy in recognizing targets and longer response time. For instance, there were significant differences in reaction time (Z = 4.21, P < 0.001) and in response bias (X = 16.98, P < 0.001).

Imaging results

Encoding task

Additional file 2 presents local maxima for encoding task. As in previous studies [38–40], the comparison subjects had extensive frontal activation, including bilateral Broca's area (Brodmann's area 6), right frontal pole (Brodmann's area 10), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann's area 46) and bilateral inferior frontal Gyrus (Brodmann's area 47). The comparison subjects also had activation in the right cingulate Gyrus (Brodmann's area 31), bilateral anterior cingulate (Brodmann's area 24, 25), bilateral parahippocampal (Brodmann's area 30), left hippocampus and bilateral insular cortex (Brodmann's area 13). Like the comparison subjects, the patients activated the bilateral Broca's area (Brodmann's area 6), left dorsolateral cortex (Brodmann's area 46), left hippocampus, left insular (Brodmann's area 13) and bilateral parahippocampal (Brodmann's area 34, 35). However, the patients did not activate the right cingulate Gyrus (Brodmann's area 31), bilateral anterior cingulate (Brodmann's area 24, 25), and the right insular cortex (Brodmann's area 13).

Between-group contrasts (Additional file 2, Figure 1) revealed that the comparison subjects had greater frontal activation in the right superior frontal Gyrus (Brodmann's area 8), bilateral middle frontal (Brodmann's area 6, 8) and bilateral inferior frontal Gyrus (Brodmann's area9, 45, 46). The comparison subjects also had great activation in the left hippocampus, bilateral parahippocampal (Brodmann's area 30), right cingulate Gyrus (Brodmann's area 31), bilateral anterior cingulate Gyrus (Brodmann's area 24, 25) and left insular cortex (Brodmann's area 13)

Figure 1
figure 1

Insular cortex activation showed great difference between PTSD and controls. In encoding task, activation of left insular in controls was greater than that in patients with PTSD (left). In retrieval task, activation of bilateral insulars in comparison subjects was greater than that in patients with PTSD (right).

Retrieval task

Additional file 3 presents local maxima for retrieval task. The comparison subjects had extensive prefrontal cortex activation, including activation in the right superior pole (Brodmann's area 10), and bilateral Broca's area (Brodmann's area 6, 9). The comparison subjects also showed bilateral activation of cingulate Gyrus (Brodmann's area 24, 31), left activation of parahippocampal Gyrus (Brodmann's area 36), bilateral activation of hippocampus and ICs. Patients with PTSD also had activation in the right superior frontal Gyrus (Brodmann's area 6), bilateral activation of middle frontal Gyrus (Brodmann's area 9, 10), left inferior frontal Gyrus (Brodmann's area 44) and right parahippocampal Gyrus (Brodmann's area 30). However, in the patients group, there were no significant activations in bilateral cingulate Gyrus, bilateral hippocampus and bilateral ICs.

Between-group contrasts (Additional file 3, Figure 2) showed greater activation in frontal regions (Brodmann's area 6, 10, 11, 38), limbic cortex (bilateral hippocampus, bilateral anterior cingulate cortex), and bilateral ICs (Brodmann's area 13) of the comparison subjects.

Figure 2
figure 2

Regional differences between PTSD group and control group. A showed that left Medial Frontal Gyrus (Brodmann's area 9) with significantly reduced gray-matter densities in PTSD group compared with controls. B showed that bilateral insulars (Brodmann's area 13) with significantly reduced gray-matter densities in PTSD compared with controls. Images were rendered onto orthogonal slices of the normal template magnetic resonance images.

Morphological comparison

MRI images were analyzed by VBM method so as to compare whether there was morphological difference between PTSD and controls or not. Results revealed that regions with less gray-matter density in PTSD group compared with control group included left Medial Frontal Gyrus (Brodmann's 9) {peak coordinate (Talairach) [x = -1, y = 41, z = 21], T score = 5.05}, and bilateral ICs (Brodmann's 13) {the left IC, peak coordinate (Talairach) [x = -36, y = 2, z = 0], T score = 4.64; the right IC, peak coordinate (Talairach) [x = 34, y = 4, z = 6], T score = 4.44;}. The intensities in other gray-matter regions and any of the white-matter regions did not show any significantly differences between two groups. These results indicated left Medical Frontal Gyrus and bilateral volume had significantly reduction in patients with PTSD than in the controls.

Discussion

The IC is located along the rhinal sulcus, rostra to the peripheral cortex. It is involved in the processing of visceral sensory, visceral motor, vestibular, attention, pain, emotion, verbal, motor information, inputs related to music and eating, in addition to gustatory, olfactory, visual, auditory, and tactile data. Recent neuroimaging data, including voxel based morphometry, PET and fMRI, revealed that IC was involved in various neuropsychiatric diseases such as mood disorders, panic disorders, PTSD, obsessive-compulsive disorders, eating disorders, and schizophrenia. Investigations of functions and connections of the IC suggest that sensory information including gustatory, olfactory, visual, auditory, and tactile inputs converge on IC, and that these multimodal sensory information may be integrated there.

The goal of the current study is to examine whether the IC (Brodmann's area 13) may be involved in declarative memory deficits in PTSD. Based on results of fMRI and MRI analysis, the IC may be involved in declarative memory deficits in PTSD. In encoding and retrieval tasks (tasks of declarative memory), the activation of the IC in patients with PTSD was lower than that in comparison subjects. Furthermore, gray-matter volume of bilateral ICs in patients with PTSD had greatly decreased.

Connections between the IC and other parts of cortex were extensive [41, 42]. These include connections with the prefrontal cortex (orbital cortex, medical prefrontal cortex), the limbic system (anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala), and the temporal pole had been documented [41]. IC has also been implicated as a visceral sensory area, visceral motor area, motor association area, area, and language area. It thus plays important roles in somatosensory integration, pain perception [41, 43], and the experience of some emotional states, especially disgust [44–46]. Until now, increasing evidence has been showing the involvement of IC in emotional processing[46]. Human subjects reported fearful emotion when their IC cortex was stimulated by electricity[47], Anterior IC of patients with phobia were activated when their symptoms were provoked [48]. Activation of IC was also found in processing taste and recalling negative emotion (such as sadness, fear, disgust), [49–53].

Recent studies indicated that the IC was involved in cognitive processing. IC was involved in performing cognitively demanding emotional tasks [54]. In a meta-analysis of 43 PET and 12 fMRI activation studies that used the emotional activation paradigm, Phan suggested that anterior cingulate cortex and the IC were involved in emotional induction with cognitive demand[54]. Reiman found that emotional recall, but not emotional film viewing, engaged the IC[55]. When implementing two cognitive tasks (competition and cooperation), the anterior IC (Brodmann's area 13) activation increased [22]. The IC was activated when processing the task of suppressing all conscious thoughts[56]. Study of Chee indicated the left IC was a marker for language attainment in bilinguals[57]. A magnetic resonance imaging found that hippocampus, parietal cortex, and the IC had significantly more atrophy in patients with early Alzheimer's disease than in healthy controls[58]. Their data suggest that the IC may be involved early in Alzheimer's disease and that atrophy of the IC may contribute to the cognitive deficits typical of early Alzheimer's disease.

Moreover, the IC is involved in declarative memory. Two fMRI studies found BOLD signal of bilateral ICs increased when implementing the task of word recognition,[23, 24]. Also, a fMRI study by Opitz (2000) found that bilateral ICs of normal subjects were activated during the word encoding and retrieval task[27]. Similar results were also reported in other neuroimaging studies on memory. The activation of IC was great when encoding and retrieving materials in episodic memory, and encoding memory tasks with pictures material [25, 26].

The hippocampus is plays key role in declarative memory [59–63]. Several structural MRI studies have found smaller hippocampus in patients with PTSD. In a recent positron emission tomography (PET) and MRI study in women with PTSD related to childhood sexual abuse, Bremner (2003) found decreased hippocampal blood flow in patients with PTSD compared to controls during paragraph encoding. In another paradigm, women with PTSD showed greater decreases in blood flow in frontal cortex and left hippocampus, and increases in visual association and motor cortex during recall of emotionally valenced word pairs[64]. Another PET study of word-stem completion also revealed an abnormal rCBF response in the hippocampus in firefighters with PTSD[52]. Squire (1996) considered that the hippocampal formation consists of two components: the hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex[65, 66]. The entorhinal cortex is the major source of cortical projections to the hippocampus region. It also receives other direct inputs from the olfactory bulb, orbital frontal cortex, the IC cortex, cingulate cortex and superior temporal Gyrus. That means the IC may be involved in declarative memory via hippocampus. In our study, the PTSD group with deficits in declarative memory had less activation in orbital frontal cortex (Brodmann's area 9, 10), hippocampus, cingulate and the IC than controls (Additional file 1, Additional file 2), when implementing encoding and retrieval tasks. These results are consistent with recent findings from study of Regland[28]in which patients with schizophrenia who had significant cognitive deficits showed less bilateral ICs activation (the left IC when encoding, the right IC with recognition, Brodmann's area 13) than healthy comparison subjects when processing word encoding and retrieval tasks.

Our study also suggests that the IC may be involved when patients experience symptoms of PTSD. Critchley (2001) proposed that the IC is involved in representing states of awareness related to external threat as well as in representing internal states of arousal[67]. Cortical regions involved in subjective awareness and states of bodily arousal during fear conditioning also include the anterior IC and adjacent orbit frontal cortices[68]. Autonomic hyperarousal symptom is one of the core symptoms of PTSD. The IC was shown to play an important role in suppressing conscious thoughts[56], which means that deficits of IC may be responsible for the recurring and intrusive nature of traumatic memories.

There are several further limitations of the present study. One of the limitations is that the sample size of the groups was relatively small; this did not allow application of alternative statistical models. Investigations with larger sample sizes are currently in progress. Another limitation is that the connection between IC and the declarative memory deficits in PTSD need more strong evidences not just from the function activation analysis but also further functional connectivity analyses.

Conclusion

To conclude, VBM analysis in our study showed bilateral reduction in IC gray-matter volume in subjects with PTSD. Furthermore, the fMRI data showed that there was less IC activation in people with PTSD than the comparison subjects when performing word encoding and retrieval tasks. These findings suggest that IC may be involved in declarative memory deficits in PTSD, which may be implicated in the symptom generation of PTSD.

References

  1. Kobayakawa T, Ogawa H, Kaneda H, yabe-Kanamura S, Endo H, Saito S: Spatio-temporal analysis of cortical activity evoked by gustatory stimulation in humans. Chem Senses. 1999, 24: 201-209. 10.1093/chemse/24.2.201.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Pritchard TC, Hamilton RB, Norgren R: Projections of the parabrachial nucleus in the old world monkey. Exp Neurol. 2000, 165: 101-117. 10.1006/exnr.2000.7450.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Paller KA, Ranganath C, Gonsalves B, LaBar KS, Parrish TB, Gitelman DR, et al: Neural correlates of person recognition. Learn Mem. 2003, 10: 253-260. 10.1101/lm.57403.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Reed JM, Means LW: Human implicit memory for irrelevant dimension values is similar to rats' incidental memory in simultaneous discrimination tasks. Behav Processes. 2004, 67: 383-393. 10.1016/j.beproc.2004.06.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bermudez-Rattoni F, Okuda S, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL: Insular cortex is involved in consolidation of object recognition memory. Learn Mem. 2005, 12: 447-449. 10.1101/lm.97605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Miranda MI, Bermudez-Rattoni F: Cholinergic activity in the insular cortex is necessary for acquisition and consolidation of contextual memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007, 87: 343-351. 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.09.010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. American Psychiatry Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 1994, Washington DC: Author, 4

    Google Scholar 

  8. Saigh PABJD: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Comprehensive Text. 1999, New York: Allyn and Bacon

    Google Scholar 

  9. Pitman RK: Post-traumatic stress disorder, hormones, and memory. Biol Psychiatry. 1989, 26: 221-223. 10.1016/0006-3223(89)90033-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Rchibald HC, Tuddenham RD: Persistent stress reaction after combat: a 20-year follow-up. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1965, 12: 475-481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. van Oyen WC: Traumatic intrusive imagery as an emotional memory phenomenon: a review of research and explanatory information processing theories. Clin Psychol Rev. 1997, 17: 509-536. 10.1016/S0272-7358(97)00025-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Barrett DH, Green ML, Morris R, Giles WH, Croft JB: Cognitive functioning and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1996, 153: 1492-1494.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Gilbertson MW, Gurvits TV, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK: Multivariate assessment of explicit memory function in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress. 2001, 14: 413-432. 10.1023/A:1011181305501.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Roca V, Freeman TW: Complaints of impaired memory in veterans with PTSD. Am J Psychiatry. 2001, 158: 1738-1739. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.10.1738-a.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Vasterling JJ, Duke LM, Brailey K, Constans JI, Allain AN, Sutker PB: Attention, learning, and memory performances and intellectual resources in Vietnam veterans: PTSD and no disorder comparisons. Neuropsychology. 2002, 16: 5-14. 10.1037/0894-4105.16.1.5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Nutt DJ, Malizia AL: Structural and functional brain changes in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004, 65 (Suppl 1): 11-17.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Cabeza R, Mangels J, Nyberg L, Habib R, Houle S, McIntosh AR, et al: Brain regions differentially involved in remembering what and when: a PET study. Neuron. 1997, 19: 863-870. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80967-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Buckner RL, Koutstaal W: Functional neuroimaging studies of encoding, priming, and explicit memory retrieval. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998, 95: 891-898. 10.1073/pnas.95.3.891.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Nolde SF, Johnson MK, D'Esposito M: Left prefrontal activation during episodic remembering: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroreport. 1998, 9: 3509-3514. 10.1097/00001756-199810260-00032.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Fletcher PC, Shallice T, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS, Dolan RJ: The functional roles of prefrontal cortex in episodic memory. II. Retrieval. Brain. 1998, 121 (Pt 7): 1249-1256. 10.1093/brain/121.7.1249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Fletcher PC, Shallice T, Dolan RJ: The functional roles of prefrontal cortex in episodic memory. I. Encoding. Brain. 1998, 121 (Pt 7): 1239-1248. 10.1093/brain/121.7.1239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Decety J, Jackson PL, Sommerville JA, Chaminade T, Meltzoff AN: The neural bases of cooperation and competition: an fMRI investigation. Neuroimage. 2004, 23: 744-751. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.025.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Buckner RL, Raichle ME, Miezin FM, Petersen SE: Functional anatomic studies of memory retrieval for auditory words and visual pictures. J Neurosci. 1996, 16: 6219-6235.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Buckner RL, Koutstaal W, Schacter DL, Wagner AD, Rosen BR: Functional-anatomic study of episodic retrieval using fMRI. I. Retrieval effort versus retrieval success. Neuroimage. 1998, 7: 151-162. 10.1006/nimg.1998.0327.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Reber PJ, Wong EC, Buxton RB: Encoding activity in the medial temporal lobe examined with anatomically constrained fMRI analysis. Hippocampus. 2002, 12: 363-376. 10.1002/hipo.10018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Iidaka T, Anderson ND, Kapur S, Cabeza R, Craik FI: The effect of divided attention on encoding and retrieval in episodic memory revealed by positron emission tomography. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000, 12: 267-280. 10.1162/089892900562093.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Opitz B, Mecklinger A, Friederici AD: Functional asymmetry of human prefrontal cortex: encoding and retrieval of verbally and nonverbally coded information. Learn Mem. 2000, 7: 85-96. 10.1101/lm.7.2.85.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Ragland JD, Gur RC, Valdez J, Turetsky BI, Elliott M, Kohler C, et al: Event-related fMRI of frontotemporal activity during word encoding and recognition in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2004, 161: 1004-1015. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.6.1004.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. First MBSRLGMWJBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV. 1994, New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research Department

    Google Scholar 

  30. Kubany ES, Leisen MB, Kaplan AS, Kelly MP: Validation of a brief measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: the Distressing Event Questionnaire (DEQ). Psychol Assess. 2000, 12: 197-209. 10.1037/1040-3590.12.2.197.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Shulin Chen, Lingjiang Li: Reliability and Validity of the PTSD symptoms self-rating scale. Chinese Mental Health Journal. 2005, 19: 308-311.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Ashburner J, Friston KJ: Voxel-based morphometry – the methods. Neuroimage. 2000, 11: 805-821. 10.1006/nimg.2000.0582.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Ashburner J, Friston KJ: Why voxel-based morphometry should be used. Neuroimage. 2001, 14: 1238-1243. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0961.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Wright IC, McGuire PK, Poline JB, Travere JM, Murray RM, Frith CD, et al: A voxel-based method for the statistical analysis of gray and white matter density applied to schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 1995, 2: 244-252. 10.1006/nimg.1995.1032.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Yamasue H, Kasai K, Iwanami A, Ohtani T, Yamada H, Abe O, et al: Voxel-based analysis of MRI reveals anterior cingulate gray-matter volume reduction in posttraumatic stress disorder due to terrorism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003, 100: 9039-9043. 10.1073/pnas.1530467100.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Talairach JTP: Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain, Georg. 1988, New York: Thieme Verlag Stuttgart

    Google Scholar 

  37. Richardson MP, Friston KJ, Sisodiya SM, Koepp MJ, Ashburner J, Free SL, et al: Cortical grey matter and benzodiazepine receptors in malformations of cortical development. A voxel-based comparison of structural and functional imaging data. Brain. 1997, 120 (Pt 11): 1961-1973. 10.1093/brain/120.11.1961.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Ragland JD, Gur RC, Valdez JN, Loughead J, Elliott M, Kohler C, et al: Levels-of-processing effect on frontotemporal function in schizophrenia during word encoding and recognition. Am J Psychiatry. 2005, 162: 1840-1848. 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1840.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Ragland JD, Gur RC, Valdez J, Turetsky BI, Elliott M, Kohler C, et al: Event-related fMRI of frontotemporal activity during word encoding and recognition in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2004, 161: 1004-1015. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.6.1004.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Ragland JD, Gur RC, Lazarev MG, Smith RJ, Schroeder L, Raz J, et al: Hemispheric activation of anterior and inferior prefrontal cortex during verbal encoding and recognition: a PET study of healthy volunteers. Neuroimage. 2000, 11: 624-633. 10.1006/nimg.2000.0577.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Augustine JR: Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1996, 22: 229-244. 10.1016/S0165-0173(96)00011-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Phelps EA, O'Connor KJ, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Grillon C, Davis M: Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nat Neurosci. 2001, 4: 437-441. 10.1038/86110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Peyron R, Laurent B, Garcia-Larrea L: Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000). Neurophysiol Clin. 2000, 30: 263-288. 10.1016/S0987-7053(00)00227-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Mayberg HS, Liotti M, Brannan SK, McGinnis S, Mahurin RK, Jerabek PA, et al: Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness. Am J Psychiatry. 1999, 156: 675-682.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Phillips ML, Young AW, Senior C, Brammer M, Andrew C, Calder AJ, et al: A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature. 1997, 389: 495-498. 10.1038/39051.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Dalgleish T: The emotional brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004, 5: 583-589. 10.1038/nrn1432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Mullan S, Penfield W: Illusions of comparative interpretation and emotion; production by epileptic discharge and by electrical stimulation in the temporal cortex. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1959, 81: 269-284.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Rauch SL, Kolk van der BA, Fisler RE, Alpert NM, Orr SP, Savage CR, et al: A symptom provocation study of posttraumatic stress disorder using positron emission tomography and script-driven imagery. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996, 53: 380-387.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Damasio AR, Grabowski TJ, Bechara A, Damasio H, Ponto LL, Parvizi J, et al: Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat Neurosci. 2000, 3: 1049-1056. 10.1038/79871.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Zald DH, Lee JT, Fluegel KW, Pardo JV: Aversive gustatory stimulation activates limbic circuits in humans. Brain. 1998, 121 (Pt 6): 1143-1154. 10.1093/brain/121.6.1143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Kosslyn SM, Shin LM, Thompson WL, McNally RJ, Rauch SL, Pitman RK, et al: Neural effects of visualizing and perceiving aversive stimuli: a PET investigation. Neuroreport. 1996, 7: 1569-1576. 10.1097/00001756-199607080-00007.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Shin LM, McNally RJ, Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Rauch SL, Alpert NM, et al: Regional cerebral blood flow during script-driven imagery in childhood sexual abuse-related PTSD: A PET investigation. Am J Psychiatry. 1999, 156: 575-584.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. George MS, Ketter TA, Parekh PI, Herscovitch P, Post RM: Gender differences in regional cerebral blood flow during transient self-induced sadness or happiness. Biol Psychiatry. 1996, 40: 859-871. 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00572-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I: Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage. 2002, 16: 331-348. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1087.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Reiman EM, Lane RD, Ahern GL, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ, Friston KJ, et al: Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion. Am J Psychiatry. 1997, 154: 918-925.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Wyland CL, Kelley WM, Macrae CN, Gordon HL, Heatherton TF: Neural correlates of thought suppression. Neuropsychologia. 2003, 41: 1863-1867. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.08.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Chee MW, Soon CS, Lee HL, Pallier C: Left insula activation: a marker for language attainment in bilinguals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004, 101: 15265-15270. 10.1073/pnas.0403703101.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Foundas AL, Leonard CM, Mahoney SM, Agee OF, Heilman KM: Atrophy of the hippocampus, parietal cortex, and insula in Alzheimer's disease: a volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1997, 10: 81-89.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Bremner JD: Neuroimaging studies in post-traumatic stress disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2002, 4: 254-263. 10.1007/s11920-996-0044-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Bremner JD, Randall P, Scott TM, Bronen RA, Seibyl JP, Southwick SM, et al: MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in patients with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1995, 152: 973-981.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Bremner JD, Randall P, Vermetten E, Staib L, Bronen RA, Mazure C, et al: Magnetic resonance imaging-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood physical and sexual abuse – a preliminary report. Biol Psychiatry. 1997, 41: 23-32. 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00162-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Hull AM: Neuroimaging findings in post-traumatic stress disorder. Systematic review. Br J Psychiatry. 2002, 181: 102-110. 10.1192/bjp.181.5.433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Shin LM, Shin PS, Heckers S, Krangel TS, Macklin ML, Orr SP, et al: Hippocampal function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Hippocampus. 2004, 14: 292-300. 10.1002/hipo.10183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, Southwick SM, McGlashan T, Nazeer A, et al: MRI and PET study of deficits in hippocampal structure and function in women with childhood sexual abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2003, 160: 924-932. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.5.924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Squire LR, Zola SM: Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996, 93: 13515-13522. 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13515.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Squire LR, Kandel ER, Kosslyn SM: Cognitive neuroscience. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1996, 6: 153-157. 10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80067-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Critchley HD, Melmed RN, Featherstone E, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ: Brain activity during biofeedback relaxation: a functional neuroimaging investigation. Brain. 2001, 124: 1003-1012. 10.1093/brain/124.5.1003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ: Fear conditioning in humans: the influence of awareness and autonomic arousal on functional neuroanatomy. Neuron. 2002, 33: 653-663. 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00588-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledgement support from grant from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (30470621, 30670751 to Lingjiang Li and Shulin Chen), the National Science and Technology Program of China (2007BAI17B02 to Lingjiang Li and Shulin Chen), the National 973 Program of China (2006CB5000800 to Lingjiang Li), the Science and Technology Bureau of Hangzhou under grant 2006533Q16 to Shulin Chen.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shulin Chen.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

SC carried out the study, participated in the sequence alignment and drafted the manuscript. LL participated in the design of the study, conceived of the study. BX and JL participated in the sequence alignment. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

12888_2008_587_MOESM1_ESM.doc

Additional file 1: Table 1. Performance during Encoding and Retrieval Tasks for Patients with PTSD and Comparison subjects. (DOC 26 KB)

12888_2008_587_MOESM2_ESM.doc

Additional file 2: Table 2. Local Maxima of Blood-Oxygen-Level-Dependent fMRI Signal Change during Encoding in Comparison Subjects and Patients with PTSD. Notes in Table 2 and Table 3. Bold means interesting areas. a Peak activation in a cluster of at least ten voxel in which the difference in signal change exceeded an extent and threshold corrected p value of 0.05. b Coordinates from the stereotaxic atlas of Talairach and Tournoux. (DOC 34 KB)

12888_2008_587_MOESM3_ESM.doc

Additional file 3: Table 3. Local Maxima of Blood-Oxygen-Level-Dependent fMRI Signal Change during Retrieval in Healthy Comparison Subjects and Patients with PTSD. Notes in Table 2 and Table 3. Bold means interesting areas. a Peak activation in a cluster of at least ten voxel in which the difference in signal change exceeded an extent and threshold corrected p value of 0.05. b Coordinates from the stereotaxic atlas of Talairach and Tournoux. (DOC 30 KB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chen, S., Li, L., Xu, B. et al. Insular cortex involvement in declarative memory deficits in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder. BMC Psychiatry 9, 39 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-9-39

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-9-39

Keywords