Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Bias and imprecision in posture percentile variables estimated from short exposure samples

Abstract

Background

Upper arm postures are believed to be an important risk determinant for musculoskeletal disorder development in the neck and shoulders. The 10th and 90th percentiles of the angular elevation distribution have been reported in many studies as measures of neutral and extreme postural exposures, and variation has been quantified by the 10th-90th percentile range. Further, the 50th percentile is commonly reported as a measure of "average" exposure. These four variables have been estimated using samples of observed or directly measured postures, typically using sampling durations between 5 and 120 min.

Methods

The present study examined the statistical properties of estimated full-shift values of the 10th, 50th and 90th percentile and the 10th-90th percentile range of right upper arm elevation obtained from samples of seven different durations, ranging from 5 to 240 min. The sampling strategies were realized by simulation, using a parent data set of 73 full-shift, continuous inclinometer recordings among hairdressers. For each shift, sampling duration and exposure variable, the mean, standard deviation and sample dispersion limits (2.5% and 97.5%) of all possible sample estimates obtained at one minute intervals were calculated and compared to the true full-shift exposure value.

Results

Estimates of the 10th percentile proved to be upward biased with limited sampling, and those of the 90th percentile and the percentile range, downward biased. The 50th percentile was also slightly upwards biased. For all variables, bias was more severe with shorter sampling durations, and it correlated significantly with the true full-shift value for the 10th and 90th percentiles and the percentile range. As expected, shorter samples led to decreased precision of the estimate; sample standard deviations correlated strongly with true full-shift exposure values.

Conclusions

The documented risk of pronounced bias and low precision of percentile estimates obtained from short posture samples presents a concern in ergonomics research and practice, and suggests that alternative, unbiased exposure variables should be considered if data collection resources are restricted.

Peer Review reports

Background

Upper arm postures and musculoskeletal disorder risk

In ergonomics intervention and epidemiology studies, upper arm posture is often assessed and viewed as an important measure of biomechanical exposure [1–4]. This is justified by evidence that working with elevated arms is associated with increased risk of musculoskeletal disorders (MSD) in the shoulders [5, 6]. The extent of 'rest' during occupational work has also been proposed to be an important determinant of risk [7, 8], which suggests that the occurrence of neutral postures is of interest as well [9–12]. In occupational studies, percentiles of the cumulative posture distribution have been commonly used to assess the occurrence of neutral and extreme postures. Most studies have employed the 10th and 90th percentiles for this purpose [12–27]. The prominent standing of these variables stems from two seminal papers, published approximately 30 years ago, in which Jonsson suggested that an exposure distribution should be described not only by its median, but also by the 10th and 90th percentiles to reflect its 'static' and 'peak' properties, respectively [28, 29]. In these papers, Jonsson also proposed guidelines for acceptable 10th, 50th and 90th percentiles of muscle activity during occupational work. While originally proposed as a data reduction method for electromyographic recordings from relevant muscles, and extensively used for this purpose in the ergonomics literature (examples in [30]), posture percentiles ad modum Jonsson were introduced by Aarås [31], and have - as shown by the references above - been commonly accepted as descriptive metrics in occupational studies of biomechanical exposures. Percentiles have even been used to describe the distribution of other occupational and environmental exposures, for instance noise [32].

In addition to the effects of neutral and extreme postures in their own right, lack of posture variation is also commonly accepted to imply an increased risk for disorders, especially in the shoulders and neck [33, 34]. The concept of 'variation' has been proposed to include several aspects of change in exposure over time [35], one of which - the amplitude of exposure changes - can be assessed using a percentile range for postural distribution. In studies of the upper arm, the 10th -90th [9, 12, 36, 37] and 5th-95th [13, 22, 38] percentile ranges have been used for this purpose.

Posture sampling strategy, precision and bias

Occupational posture assessments by observation and direct technical recording have typically employed a continuous sampling approach of a duration ranging from some minutes up to approximately two hours [13, 14, 16, 17, 25, 26, 31, 39]. In a few rare cases, a whole shift has been monitored [12, 19, 27, 40, 41], or even a whole day including both work and leisure [37]. These sampling strategies have been applied even in studies pursuing exposures over an extended period of time, for example, a month or a year. Since postures vary across time within individuals, limited duration samples do, in these cases, return uncertain estimates. The variability of posture variables between individuals, and within and between days within individuals has been addressed in several occupational settings [12, 23, 40, 42, 43], and the influence of sample size [30, 40, 44–51], sample allocation [46, 48, 50, 52], and data processing method [53, 54] on the precision of the resulting exposure estimate has been discussed for postures and other biomechanical exposures. All of these studies have addressed the size and effects of random error, assuming that the investigated sampling strategies produce unbiased exposure estimates, i.e. values free of systematic error. However, the latter conjecture is not trivial, particularly for variables that are not necessarily well estimated over an extended period of time by averaging across embedded sub-periods, for example, variances [55] and, as we propose, percentiles. Data on lumbar muscle activity in different occupations reported by Trask et al. [56] indicate that job exposure percentiles may, indeed, be biased if based on short samples. Bias and precision when assessing posture percentiles have not to our knowledge, been previously investigated, nor have the effects of the selected sampling strategy on the size and structure of these errors been quantified.

The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of different data sampling strategies, ranging in duration from 5 to 240 min, on the bias and precision of estimated full-shift values for the 10th, 50th and 90th upper arm elevation percentiles, and the 10th-90th percentile range. We based the study on 73 full-shift inclinometry recordings from 20 hairdressers.

Methods

Subjects and posture recordings

Full-shift, right upper arm elevation angles were collected from a convenience sample of 20 female hairdressers in 13 salons in Umeå, Sweden; mean age 31 (range 19-60) years, mean height 167 (150-176) cm, mean seniority 11 (0.25-42) years. Only hairdressers working a minimum of 30 h per week were considered for inclusion. These data were collected as part of a larger study which included 28 hairdressers [12], however, only data from hairdressers who fully completed a study diary were included in the parent data set for the present data analyses. Participants were recorded over four full shifts within the same week, using triaxial accelerometers [57] mounted above the insertion of the right deltoid muscle, aligned with the long axis of the humerus. These accelerometers have previously been shown to provide elevation angles deviating, on average, less than 2° from true values [57]. The study was approved by the local ethical committee at Umeå University.

Procedures for calibration and basic data processing have previously been described in detail [12]. Following the exclusion of shifts with technical shortcomings and/or less than 300 min of viable data, 73 acceptable shifts remained: 14 subjects with 4 shifts, 5 subjects with 3 shifts, and 1 subject with 2 shifts. The mean full-shift recording duration was 486 min (range 300 to 595 min).

Data processing and simulated sampling strategies

For all 73 shifts, the true 10th, 50th and 90th percentiles, and the 10th-90th percentile range were determined from the cumulative distribution of the full-shift parent data set recording. The same four variables were also estimated for seven simulated sampling strategies of durations: 5, 10, 20, 40, 60, 120 and 240 min. For each sampling strategy, sampling windows of the appropriate duration were formed from the parent data set at one-minute intervals across the full-shift data. For example, employing the 10-minute sampling strategy over a 480 min shift, 471 ten-minute sampling windows would be obtained, the first window spanning minutes #1-10, the second minutes #2-11, and so on until the last window, spanning minutes #471-480. For each of the 471 sampling windows in this example, the four posture variables would then be calculated as estimates of their true full-shift value.

For each sampling strategy, shift and posture variable, four statistical performance variables were calculated on the basis of these estimates:

Sample bias; B = μ y -M

Sample standard deviation; s y

Lower sample dispersion limit; D 2.5 = P 2.5 -M

Upper sample dispersion limit; D 97.5 = P 97.5 -M

where μ y and s y are the mean and standard deviation of the sample estimates (n = 471 in the example above); M is the true exposure value for the full-shift; and P 2.5 and P 97.5 are the empirical 2.5th and 97.5th percentile values of the cumulative distribution of the sample estimates. Thus, D 2.5 and D 97.5 state the distances in either direction from the true exposure value that a single sample will exceed with a 2.5% probability, and can be viewed as measures of the combined effect of bias and imprecision. D 2.5 and D 97.5 give the same information as the limits of agreement in a standard Bland&Altman plot [58], however, since they are based on the empirical structure of data, they represent a more versatile approach which allows for a non-normal distribution of the difference between sample estimates and true exposure values.

For each sampling strategy and posture variable, cumulative probability distribution plots across the 73 shifts were produced for each of the four performance variables, B, s y , D 2.5 and D 97.5 , as a basis for comparing statistical performance.

To further examine the nature of the sampling error, Spearman's rank correlation coefficients (with 95% confidence intervals) were determined according to standard procedures [59] between M and B, and between M and s y for each posture variable and sampling strategy across the 73 measured shifts.

Results

True posture values

The true mean 10th, 50th and 90th percentile elevation angles across all 73 full-shifts in the parent data set were 8.2°, 21.9° and 50.3°, respectively, and the mean 10th-90th percentile range was 42.0°. As expected [12], the cumulative probability distribution plots for all four posture percentile variables as well as the mean inclination angle showed a considerable dispersion across shifts (Figure 1). This exposure variability contains contributions from both between-subject and within-subject (between-shifts) sources.

Figure 1
figure 1

Cumulative probability distributions across the 73 investigated full work shifts for the 10 th , 50 th and 90 th upper arm elevation percentiles, the mean elevation (m), and the 10 th -90 th percentile range, as marked by the colour codes and text above the figure.

Sampling bias

Estimates of the 10th and 50th percentile were, in general, 'upward' biased (shifted to the right), while those for the 90th percentile and the 10th-90th percentile range were 'downward' biased (shifted to the left) for all sampling strategies; the bias was more severe with shorter sample durations (Figure 2). For a particular sampling duration, the size of the bias differed considerably among the 73 shifts (as seen in the width of the distribution) and, for long sample durations, some shifts even showed a bias in the opposite direction of the rest of the data; for example, negative values were seen for the 10th and 50th percentiles for sampling durations 60 min or longer, while conversely, some examples of positive bias were seen for the 90th percentile and the 10th-90th percentile range.

Figure 2
figure 2

Cumulative probability distributions across the 73 investigated work shifts for the bias, B , of the four assessed posture variables (10 th , 50 th , 90 th percentiles, 10 th -90 th percentile range) for each of the seven sampling durations as marked by colour codes and numbers in each figure.

For the 10th and 90th percentiles and the 10th-90th percentile range, bias correlated significantly with the true exposure value, M, in particular for short samples (Table 1). Thus, for the 10th percentile variable, a larger positive bias was found for full-shifts with larger true mean exposures, while for the 90th percentile and the 10th-90th percentile range, larger true M values were associated with a larger negative bias. The bias of the 50th percentile was positively correlated with the true exposure, M, value for long samples, but since bias was small in this case, correlations may not be particularly informative.

Table 1 Spearman's rank correlation coefficients [95% confidence intervals] between the true full-shift exposure value M and the bias B, and between M and the sample standard deviation s y , for each of the seven sampling durations (5, 10, 20, 40, 60, 120, 240 min) and each of the four posture variables (10th, 50th, 90th percentiles, 10th-90th percentile range)

Sampling precision

As anticipated, the precision of the sample exposure estimate increased, i.e. s y decreased, with longer sampling durations for all three percentile variables (Figure 3), and for the percentile range. However, s y decreased at a slower rate than that expected for randomly distributed data, for which the standard deviation should decrease in inverse proportion to the square root of the sampling duration [35]. For all four posture variables, s y was significantly correlated with the true exposure value M (Table 1), and the association was stronger at short sampling durations.

Figure 3
figure 3

Cumulative probability distributions across the 73 investigated work shifts for the sample standard deviation, s y , of the four assessed posture variables (10 th , 50 th , 90 th percentiles, 10 th -90 th percentile range) for each of the seven sampling durations as marked by colour codes and numbers in each figure.

Sampling dispersion

As expected from the precision results, smaller dispersions resulted for all postural variables with longer sampling durations, but estimates were still skewed due to the bias. This pattern is reflected in the dispersion limits D 2.5 (Figure 4) and D 97.5 (Figure 5) which both approach zero with increasing sample duration, while not being of equal size. Ninety-five percent of all samples within a full-shift will lie in the interval between the two dispersion limits. For example, for the 20 min sampling strategy, an estimate of the 10th-90th percentile range could, in median, with a 5% probability be either more than 22.7° smaller (marked in Figure 4) or more than 25.4° larger (marked in Figure 5) than the true full-shift percentile range.

Figure 4
figure 4

Cumulative probability distributions across the 73 investigated work shifts for the lower dispersion limit, D 2.5 , of the four assessed posture variables (10 th , 50 th , 90 th percentiles, 10 th -90 th percentile range) for each of the seven sampling durations as marked by colour codes and numbers in each figure. The square marks an example described in the running text.

Figure 5
figure 5

Cumulative probability distributions across the 73 investigated work shifts for the upper dispersion limit, D 97.5 , of the four assessed posture variables (10 th , 50 th , 90 th percentiles, 10 th -90 th percentile range) for each of the seven sampling durations as marked by colour codes and numbers in each figure. The square marks an example described in the running text.

Discussion

The present study showed that estimates of the 10th and 90th upper arm elevation percentiles and the 10th-90th percentile range can be severely biased if they are estimated from posture samples collected over short periods of time. Since short samples were also shown to have a larger variability, i.e. be less precise, great caution should be exercised when using such sampling strategies to estimate overall exposures intended to represent extended periods of time. The findings also imply that previous studies reporting posture percentiles should be compared cautiously and only with due consideration to the fact that estimates may have been based on samples of different durations. What would appear to be a difference between groups or conditions may - in whole or part - be the result of a statistical flaw.

Hairdressers as a representative working population

As reported by Wahlström and co-workers [12], the exposure of the present hairdressers was similar to those in a sample of Norwegian hairdressers [25], when considering that the Norwegian data were collected only when the hairdressers had customers, as compared to the current study which measured full working shifts, irrespective of task. The mean 10th percentile value across the hairdressers in the present study population was similar to previously reported values from a wide range of occupational groups, including: sewing machine assembly workers [17], car disassembly workers [9], and hospital cleaners [19], while it was lower than that reported for CAD operators [60], air traffic controllers [13], floor sheet handlers [18], and dentists [21]. The 90th percentile was comparable to that of cleaners [19] and dentists [21]. The 10th-90th percentile range was somewhat smaller than that reported for car disassembly workers [9]; this variable was nor reported in any of the other cited studies. In summary, the exposures seen in the present population seem comparable to those experienced by several other occupational groups, and we therefore believe that the performance of percentile measurement strategies as reported here is valid in a wide range of occupations. The observed dependence of both the bias, B, and the sample standard deviation, s y, , on the true exposure level suggests that in occupational settings with more extreme percentiles than the present ones, errors associated with limited sampling would be larger than those reported here. Further, these suppositions would apply even to jobs characterized by cyclic operations, such as in industrial assembly. While it is often claimed that such cyclic jobs can be adequately monitored by collecting data from just a few work cycles, several studies suggest that the magnitude of cycle-to-cycle posture variability can, indeed, be considerable [51, 61, 62]. In a real occupational setting, the full-shift exposure variability will, in addition to this within-day cycle-to-cycle variability, include contributions from non-cyclic and irregular parts of the job such as production stops, breaks, occasional tasks, and meetings. Thus, the statistical performance of limited duration sampling strategies in cyclic work may not deviate as much as expected from that seen in non-routinized work [63].

Bias, imprecision and sampling duration

Bias (Figure 2) and lack of precision, i.e. a large s y (Figure 3), was very pronounced with sampling durations of 5-10 min - durations which are not unusual among ergonomics practitioners, e.g. in occupational health practices [48]. In research studies, samples of this short a duration are rare, but the present study showed that the magnitude of both bias and imprecision was considerable even for samples of 20-120 min, as has been previously used [15–18, 24]. Thus, the example marked by squared in Figures 4 and 5 illustrates that for an 'average' (median) shift, using a 20 min sampling strategy to determine the 10th-90th percentile range would result in 5% of all estimates deviating more than 22.7° downwards or 25.4° upwards from the true full-shift value. For half of the investigated shifts, the 20 min sampling strategy performed even worse.

Short samples may occur if a 'long' job sampling period is subdivided into tasks included in the job [9, 12, 21, 40, 64]; and also in cases where a sample is subdivided in shorter sequences, for instance for illustrative purposes (see e.g. Figure 3 in [13]). In these cases, it is important to comprehend that an overall job exposure estimate obtained as a weighted average of such constituent parts [40, 52] will be biased to the same extent as if sampling were conducted using windows that short.

We believe that the finding of limited sampling leading to upward biased 10th percentiles and downward biased 90th percentiles and 10th-90th percentile ranges is a general result, inherent to these percentile variables, irrespective of the exposure domain. In addition to the empirical findings in the present study, we also base this contention on similar results obtained by simulating limited sampling strategies from artificial, random data sets, and on the observation by Trask et al. of biased percentiles in samples of electromyography from the lower back [56]. We also believe, on the basis of our observation that the estimation bias of the 10th and 90th percentiles was directed 'inwards' towards the overall central exposure value, that more extreme percentiles will be even more biased with limited sampling. Thus, we recommend great caution when assessing, comparing and interpreting extreme percentiles, for example, the 1st and 99th posture percentiles as reported in a recent compilation of studies from a large range of occupations [24], or 5th-95th posture percentile ranges as used in a number of studies [13, 22, 38].

The present study investigated the performance of limited sampling within shifts because true exposures were available for comparison at this level. Since, however, posture distributions vary between shifts for a particular individual [12, 23, 40, 42], true posture percentiles for single full-shifts may, themselves, be biased estimates of posture percentiles across multiple shifts, analogous to within-shift samples being biased estimates of that shift's true exposure. Thus, the magnitude of bias reported here may, in fact, underestimate the bias of using limited within-day samples as representations of the overall occupational exposure of an individual across an extended period of time, which is standard practice in epidemiologic exposure assessment. Examination of the present data set, which contained subsets of 2-4 shifts from different days belonging to the same subject, indicated that the true percentiles of individual shifts (Figure 1) were, indeed, biased when compared to the corresponding variables measured across all available shifts within the same subject; the size of the bias was, however, small compared to the sample bias within a shift. The most pronounced example of bias across shifts was seen for the 10th percentile, where 16 of the 20 observed hairdressers had upwards biased shift estimates (as expected), with an average bias of 0.4°. Due to the limited number of shifts per subject, however, the extent and structure of this across-shifts bias could not be determined in detail.

Effects of exposure bias and imprecision in epidemiologic studies

Bias and imprecision of exposure estimates have an obvious influence on the quality of studies documenting or comparing exposures in individuals or groups. Bias shifts the central result of a study and imprecision reduces its informative value and decreases power [30, 44]. The combined effect of bias and imprecision can be assessed by simple metrics, such as the lower and upper dispersion limits (cf. Figures 4 and 5).

The effects of exposure bias and imprecision are less trivial in an epidemiologic investigation of the relationship(s) between exposure and outcome, for example whether the occurrence of neutral or extreme postures, or the size of posture variation, has an effect on the likelihood of developing an MSD. The general effect of within-subject imprecision is to attenuate the true exposure-outcome relationship, i.e. to modify its slope towards zero, and add uncertainty to the slope estimate [65–68]. This attenuation effect is directly dependent on the so-called variance ratio, i.e. the ratio of within- to between- subject variability; a proportionally larger within-subject variability leading to more pronounced attenuation [65, 66]. For a consistently increasing exposure-outcome relationship, an upward biased exposure estimate will lead to an estimated exposure-outcome relationship that predicts the outcome at a certain exposure level to be smaller than what it actually is, but the shape of this attenuation depends on whether the bias is constant across exposure levels or proportional to the exposure, as suggested in the present data set. Thus, both bias and imprecision in 10th percentile estimates lead to attenuation of an increasing exposure-outcome relationship, for instance, when investigating whether decreased occurrence of neutral postures (i.e. a larger 10th percentile value) leads to a larger risk for MSD.

In the case of "extreme" postures measured through the 90th percentile, an expectedly increasing relationship with an MSD outcome would be attenuated by exposure imprecision but amplified by the downward bias. Thus, bias and imprecision would have opposite effects on the estimated exposure-outcome relationship. The net trade-off must be addressed in each particular case, but in the present setting, noting the between-subjects and between-days percentile variabilities for the 90th arm elevation percentiles reported by Wahlström et al. [12], the attenuating effect of imprecision seems to be more potent than the amplifying effect of bias. A similar opposite effect of bias and imprecision on the exposure-outcome relationship will appear if the exposure value is downward biased and the relationship is decreasing. This is the expected scenario if the influence of posture variation on MSD risk is addressed using the 10th-90th percentile range as the exposure metric.

While the consequences of exposure variability for exposure-outcome relationships can often be assessed and adjusted for, provided that the sources of this variability are known [66, 68], adjustment for bias requires access to specific and sufficient information on the structure and properties of the bias to permit a translation of estimated exposures to the expected true values [69–71].

Data collection strategies for posture percentile variables

In the present study, bias was correlated with true exposure; thus it might be possible to derive a useful expression to translate an estimate to the expected true value, conditional on the sampling strategy. As a more feasible correction approach, which can be used even if true values are not available for calibration, the overall median (or mean) bias calculated for a similar occupational setting using the same sampling duration (cf. Figure 2) could be used to adjust the percentile estimate.

If the residual error after bias correction does not have the desirable random and normal distribution properties, limited sampling strategies may not perform as expected from analytical theory [50, 52]. For instance, exposure may be correlated within days, as when a work shift is composed of a sequence of tasks with different exposures [52], between days, as in seasonal work [72] or between subjects, as in team work [73]. In these cases, depending on the structure of the correlation, effects of increasing the sample duration can be larger or smaller than anticipated by theory [50], and consecutive sampling may perform better or worse than a more dispersed allocation of the same total sample duration [52]. For a particular setting, this behavior can be difficult to predict even if a negative effect on sampling performance may be the more common case [50].

For the present data set, visual inspection of the time-patterns of upper arm elevation angle over individual shifts clearly indicated that a non-random data structure was common: extended periods with a large upper arm elevation, when handling customers, could alternate with long periods in an almost neutral posture when no customers were present. This autocorrelation is a probable explanation to the less-than-expected decrease in s y which was observed with increased sampling duration (Figure 3). The attenuated effect of increasing the sample duration implies that the precision of a percentile estimate does not improve as much as expected from theory when longer samples are employed. In this case, sampling performance may improve if a particular sample duration is obtained with dispersed sampling rather than in a consecutive approach, as simulated in the present study [50, 52]. Alternative sample distributions across time were not investigated here, and consecutive sampling has been the dominant strategy in previous studies of working postures, particularly in studies employing on-site or video observations [39, 74–77]. Consecutive observation may have originally been chosen by virtue of the logistic simplicity, while the drawbacks, in terms of decreased efficiency, may not have been realized. In the case of postures assessed by inclinometry, sampling in separate time blocks within a day is not an attractive option. Efforts associated with equipment set up on the subject are substantial, and continuous monitoring with the inclinometers in place appears to be a cost-efficient choice. In this case, however, recordings should be continued for sufficiently long periods for the bias and precision to reach satisfying levels; i.e. at least 120 min when using metrics based on the 10th and 90th percentiles in occupational settings comparable to the one studied here.

Autocorrelation may also help explain the occasional overestimate of the 90th percentile, and hence the 10th-90th percentile range, particularly for long sampling durations (Figure 2). With randomly distributed data, overestimation of the 90th percentile and the 10th-90th percentile - and the corresponding incident of an underestimated 10th percentile - would probably occur only in rare cases, if ever.

Posture metrics - alternatives to percentiles

The 10th and 90th posture percentiles are intended to measure the occurrence of neutral and extreme postures, respectively, in a posture recording. A number of studies have applied alternative metrics for these purposes, based on the proportion of time spent in certain posture sectors [78]. The operational definition of neutral upper arm postures have included angles less than 20° [9, 12, 79], 30° [14, 80] and 45° [81], and extreme or "severe" postures have been expressed as angles larger than 60° [9, 12, 14, 23, 80] or 90° [6, 81]. Similar posture metrics based on time spent in specific angle sectors have also been presented for other body regions, including the neck [9, 14, 23, 80–82], trunk [9, 23, 27, 43, 51, 80, 82] and wrist [9, 82]. A few studies have documented the statistical performance of arm posture metrics based both on angle sectors and percentiles in the same population [12, 23], suggesting posture variabilities between and within subjects to be of similar sizes for comparable variables. Thus, in this respect, angle sector metrics are a viable alternative to percentiles. Furthermore, and most important, angle sector metrics are not inherently biased. For instance, with a randomly distributed arm elevation across a shift, the mean value of all 5-minute samples for percent time with arms above 90° will be the correct value for that shift, while the corresponding 90th percentile estimates will, on average, be too small (cf. Figure 2). This suggests that summary metrics based on time proportions are preferable to metrics based on percentiles, in particular if the sampling duration is short. With longer samples, the percentile bias vanishes (Figure 2), and the choice among relevant posture metrics can be based predominantly on their statistical performance in terms of precision. Thus, as a tentative rule of thumb, samples longer than 120 min allow for satisfying estimates of both 10th and 90th percentiles and angle sector metrics, while the percentiles should not be utilised on shorter samples.

For the 10th-90th percentile range, alternative metrics are not as easy to identify. Other metrics assessing the "how much" aspect of variation have been proposed [35], such as, the standard deviation between mean angles in posture recording chunks throughout a shift [44], and the standard deviation of the cell values in an Exposure Variation Analysis of postures [36, 37], but neither of these variables have attractive or even documented statistical properties. Thus, a viable unbiased alternative to the 10th-90th percentile range with known statistical performance still needs to be developed.

Conclusions

This study demonstrated the risk of encountering both substantial bias and pronounced lack of precision when estimating full-shift upper arm posture percentiles on the basis of samples of limited duration. We believe this to be an inherent property of percentiles as a class of exposure metrics, even for other measures of biomechanical exposure, including postures of other body parts and muscle activity levels measured by electromyography. This disadvantage should be noted when deciding which posture variables to use both in research studies and, in particular, in ergonomics practice, where short sampling durations may be dictated by resource limitations [48]. Alternatives to variables based on percentiles may be preferable, such as the proportion of time spent in pre-defined angle sectors [9, 12, 23, 27, 79, 80]; informed decisions based on the statistical properties of such alternatives are paramount [6, 12, 50, 51, 78].

References

  1. Li G, Buckle P: Current techniques for assessing physical exposure to work-related musculoskeletal risks, with emphasis on posture-based methods. Ergonomics. 1999, 42: 674-695. 10.1080/001401399185388.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. van der Beek AJ, Frings-Dresen MHW: Assessment of mechanical exposure in ergonomic epidemiology. Occup Environ Med. 1998, 55: 291-299. 10.1136/oem.55.5.291.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. David CG: Ergonomic methods for assessing exposure to risk factors for work-related musculoskeletal disorders. Occup Med (Lond). 2005, 55: 190-199. 10.1093/occmed/kqi082.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Takala EP, Pehkonen I, Forsman M, Hansson G-Å, Mathiassen SE, Neumann WP, Sjøgaard G, Veiersted KB, Westgaard RH, Winkel J: Systematic evaluation of observational methods assessing biomechanical exposures at work. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2010, 36: 3-24. 10.5271/sjweh.2876.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Punnett L, Fine LJ, Monroe Keyserling W, Herrin GD, Chaffin DB, Punnett L, Fine LJ, Monroe Keyserling W, Herrin GD, Chaffin DB: Shoulder disorders and postural stress in automobile assembly work. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2000, 26: 283-291. 10.5271/sjweh.544.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Svendsen SW, Bonde JP, Mathiassen SE, Steengaard-Pedersen K, Frich LH: Work-related shoulder disorders: quantitative exposure-response relationships with reference to arm posture. Occup Environ Med. 2004, 61: 844-853. 10.1136/oem.2003.010637.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Jensen C, Nilsen K, Hansen K, Westgaard R: Trapezius muscle load as a risk indicator for occupational shoulder-neck complaints. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 1993, 64: 415-423. 10.1007/BF00517947.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Veiersted KB, Westgaard RH, Andersen P: Electromyographic evaluation of muscular work pattern as a predictor of trapezius myalgia. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1993, 19: 284-290. 10.5271/sjweh.1472.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Kazmierczak K, Mathiassen SE, Forsman M, Winkel J: An integrated analysis of ergonomics and time consumption in Swedish 'craft-type' car disassembly. Appl Ergon. 2005, 36: 263-273. 10.1016/j.apergo.2005.01.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Aarås A, Fostervold KI, Ro O, Thoresen M, Larsen S: Postural load during VDU work: a comparison between various work postures. Ergonomics. 1997, 40: 1255-1268. 10.1080/001401397187496.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Jensen C, Finsen L, Hansen K, Christensen H: Upper trapezius muscle activity patterns during repetitive manual material handling and work with a computer mouse. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 1999, 9: 317-325. 10.1016/S1050-6411(99)00007-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Wahlström J, Mathiassen SE, Liv P, Hedlund P, Forsman M, Ahlgren C: Upper arm postures and movements in female hairdressers across four full working days. Ann Occup Hyg. 2010, 54: 584-594. 10.1093/annhyg/meq028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Arvidsson I, Hansson G-Ã…, Mathiassen SE, Skerfving S: Changes in physical workload with implementation of mouse-based information technology in air traffic control. Int J Ind Ergon. 2006, 36: 613-622. 10.1016/j.ergon.2006.03.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Juul-Kristensen B, Hansson G-Ã…, Fallentin N, Andersen JH, Ekdahl C: Assessment of work postures and movements using a video-based observation method and direct technical measurements. Appl Ergon. 2001, 32: 517-524. 10.1016/S0003-6870(01)00017-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Delisle A, Larivière C, Plamondon A, Imbeau D: Comparison of three computer office workstations offering forearm support: impact on upper limb posture and muscle activation. Ergonomics. 2006, 49: 139-160. 10.1080/10610270500450739.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Vasseljen O, Westgaard RH: Arm and trunk posture during work in relation to shoulder and neck pain and trapezius activity. Clin Biomech. 1997, 12: 22-31. 10.1016/S0268-0033(97)00048-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Bao S, Mathiassen SE, Winkel J: Ergonomic effects of a management-based rationalization in assembly work - a case study. Appl Ergon. 1996, 27: 89-99. 10.1016/0003-6870(95)00063-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Balogh I, Ohlsson K, Hansson G-Å, Engström T, Skerfving S: Increasing the degree of automation in a production system: consequences for the physical workload. Int J Ind Ergon. 2006, 36: 353-365. 10.1016/j.ergon.2004.09.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Unge J, Ohlsson K, Nordander C, Hansson G-Ã…, Skerfving S, Balogh I: Differences in physical workload, psychosocial factors and musculoskeletal disorders between two groups of female hospital cleaners with two diverse organizational models. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 2007, 81: 209-220. 10.1007/s00420-007-0208-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Forsman M, Hansson G-Å, Medbo L, Asterland P, Engström T: A method for evaluation of manual work using synchronised video recordings and physiological measurements. Appl Ergon. 2002, 33: 533-540. 10.1016/S0003-6870(02)00070-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Jonker D, Rolander B, Balogh I: Relation between perceived and measured workload obtained by long-term inclinometry among dentists. Appl Ergon. 2009, 40: 309-315. 10.1016/j.apergo.2008.12.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Ã…kesson I, Hansson G-Ã…, Balogh I, Moritz U, Skerfving S: Quantifying work load in neck, shoulders and wrists in female dentists. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 1997, 69: 461-474. 10.1007/s004200050175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Hansson G-Ã…, Arvidsson I, Ohlsson K, Nordander C, Mathiassen SE, Skerfving S, Balogh I: Precision of measurements of physical workload during standardised manual handling. Part II: Inclinometry of head, upper back, neck and upper arms. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2006, 16: 125-136. 10.1016/j.jelekin.2005.06.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Hansson G-Å, Balogh I, Ohlsson K, Granqvist L, Nordander C, Arvidsson I, Åkesson I, Unge J, Rittner R, Strömberg U, et al: Physical workload in various types of work: Part II. Neck, shoulder and upper arm. Int J Ind Ergon. 2010, 40: 267-281. 10.1016/j.ergon.2009.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Veiersted KB, Gould KS, Osterås N, Hansson G-Å: Effect of an intervention addressing working technique on the biomechanical load of the neck and shoulders among hairdressers. Appl Ergon. 2008, 39: 183-190. 10.1016/j.apergo.2007.05.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Christmansson M, Medbo L, Hansson G-Å, Ohlsson K, Unge Byström J, Möller T, Forsman M: A case study of a principally new way of materials kitting - an evaluation of time consumption and physical workload. Int J Ind Ergon. 2002, 30: 49-65. 10.1016/S0169-8141(02)00077-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Fethke NB, Gant LC, Gerr F: Comparison of biomechanical loading during use of conventional stud welding equipment and an alternate system. Appl Ergon. 2011, 42: 725-734. 10.1016/j.apergo.2010.11.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Jonsson B: Measurement and evalution of local muscular strain in the shoulder during constrained work. J Hum Ergol. 1982, 11: 73-88. 10.1016/S0047-2484(82)80033-X.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Jonsson B: Kinesiology. With special reference to electromyographic kinesiology. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl. 1978, 34: 417-428.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Mathiassen SE, Burdorf A, van der Beek AJ: Statistical power and measurement allocation in ergonomic intervention studies assessing upper trapezius EMG amplitude. A case study of assembly work. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2002, 12: 27-39. 10.1016/S1050-6411(01)00031-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Aarås A, Westgaard RH, Stranden E: Postural angles as an indicator of postural load and muscular injury in occupational work situations. Ergonomics. 1988, 31: 915-933. 10.1080/00140138808966731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. To WM, Ip RC, Lam GC, Yau CT: A multiple regression model for urban traffic noise in Hong Kong. J Acoust Soc Am. 2002, 112: 551-556. 10.1121/1.1494803.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. National Research Council: Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Workplace. 2001, Washington DC: National Academies Press

    Google Scholar 

  34. Sluiter JK, Rest KM, Frings-Dresen MHW: Criteria document for evaluating the work-relatedness of upper-extremity musculoskeletal disorders. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2001, 27 (Suppl 1): 1-102.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Mathiassen SE: Diversity and variation in biomechanical exposure: what is it, and why would we like to know?. Appl Ergon. 2006, 37: 419-427. 10.1016/j.apergo.2006.04.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Straker L, Coleman J, Skoss R, Maslen BA, Burgess-Limerick R, Pollock C: A comparison of posture and muscle activity during tablet computer, desktop computer and paper use by young children. Ergonomics. 2008, 51: 540-555. 10.1080/00140130701711000.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Ciccarelli M, Straker L, Mathiassen SE, Pollock C: ITkids Part II: variation of postures and muscle activity in children using different information and communication technologies. Work. 2011, 38: 413-427.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Bernmark E, Forsman M, Wiktorin C: Head movements during two computer tasks assessed by accelerometry. Appl Ergon. 2011, 42: 309-313. 10.1016/j.apergo.2010.07.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Dahlberg R, Karlqvist L, Bildt C, Nykvist K: Do work technique and musculoskeletal symptoms differ between men and women performing the same type of work tasks?. Appl Ergon. 2004, 35: 521-529. 10.1016/j.apergo.2004.06.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Svendsen SW, Mathiassen SE, Bonde JP: Task-based exposure assessment in ergonomic epidemiology - a study of upper arm elevation in the jobs of machinists, car mechanics, and house painters. Occup Environ Med. 2005, 62: 18-26. 10.1136/oem.2004.015966.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Leijon O, Bernmark E, Karlqvist L, Härenstam A: Awkward work postures: association with occupational gender segregation. Am J Ind Med. 2005, 47: 381-393. 10.1002/ajim.20166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Burdorf A, Verburgh A, Elders L: Time-dependent variation in back load of workers in a dairy factory. Occup Hyg. 1994, 1: 199-206.

    Google Scholar 

  43. van der Beek AJ, Kuiper JI, Dawson M, Burdorf A, Bongers PM, Frings-Dresen MHW: Sources of variance in exposure to nonneutral trunk postures in varying work situations. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1995, 21: 215-222. 10.5271/sjweh.30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Mathiassen SE, Möller T, Forsman M: Variability in mechanical exposure within and between individuals performing a highly constrained industrial work task. Ergonomics. 2003, 46: 800-824. 10.1080/0014013031000090125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Allread WG, Marras WS, Burr DL: Measuring trunk motions in industry: variability due to task factors, individual differences, and the amount of data collected. Ergonomics. 2000, 43: 691-701. 10.1080/001401300404670.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Hoozemans MJM, Burdorf A, van der Beek AJ, Frings-Dresen MHW, Mathiassen SE: Group-based measurement strategies in exposure assessment explored by bootstrapping. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2001, 27: 125-132. 10.5271/sjweh.599.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Paquet V, Punnett L, Woskie S, Buchholz B: Reliable exposure assessment strategies for physical ergonomics stressors in construction and other non-routinized work. Ergonomics. 2005, 48: 1200-1219. 10.1080/00140130500197302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Paquet V, Mathiassen SE, Dempsey PG: Video-based ergonomic job analysis. A practitioner's guide. Prof Saf. 2006, 11: 27-35.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Fethke NB, Anton D, Cavanaugh JE, Gerr F, Cook TM: Bootstrap exploration of the duration of surface electromyography sampling in relation to the precision of exposure estimation. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2007, 33: 358-367. 10.5271/sjweh.1155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Liv P, Mathiassen SE, Svendsen SW: Theoretical and empirical efficiency of sampling strategies for estimating upper arm elevation. Ann Occup Hyg. 2011, 55: 436-449. 10.1093/annhyg/meq095.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Mathiassen SE, Paquet V: The ability of limited exposure sampling to detect effects of interventions that reduce the occurrence of pronounced trunk inclination. Appl Ergon. 2010, 41: 295-304. 10.1016/j.apergo.2009.08.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Mathiassen SE, Burdorf A, van der Beek AJ, Hansson G-Ã…: Efficient one-day sampling of mechanical job exposure data - a study based on upper trapezius activity in cleaners and office workers. Am Ind Hyg Assoc J. 2003, 64: 196-211. 10.1080/15428110308984809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Kazmierczak K, Mathiassen SE, Neumann WP, Winkel J: Observer reliability of industrial activity analysis based on video recordings. Int J Ind Ergon. 2006, 36: 275-282. 10.1016/j.ergon.2005.12.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Jackson JA, Mathiassen SE, Dempsey PG: Methodological variance associated with normalization of occupational upper trapezius EMG using submaximal reference contractions. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2009, 19: 416-427. 10.1016/j.jelekin.2007.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Liv P, Mathiassen SE, Svendsen SW: Accuracy and precision of variance components in occupational posture recordings: a simulation study of different data collection strategies. BMC Med Res Methodol. submitted

  56. Trask C, Teschke K, Morrison J, Johnson PW, Village J, Koehoorn M: How long is long enough? Evaluating sampling durations for low back EMG assessment. J Occup Environ Hyg. 2008, 5: 664-670. 10.1080/15459620802313840.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Hansson G-Ã…, Asterland P, Holmer N-G, Skerfving S: Validity and reliability of triaxial accelerometers for inclinometry in posture analysis. Med Biol Eng Comput. 2001, 39: 405-413. 10.1007/BF02345361.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Bland JM, Altman DG: Measuring agreement in method comparison studies. Stat Methods Med Res. 1999, 8: 135-160. 10.1191/096228099673819272.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Statistics with Confidence: Confidence intervals and statistical guidelines. Edited by: Altman DG, Machin D, Bryant T, Gardner S. 2000, Hoboken: Wiley/BMJ Books, 2

    Google Scholar 

  60. Unge Byström J, Hansson G-Å, Rylander L, Ohlsson K, Källrot G, Skerfving S: Physical workload on neck and upper limb using two CAD applications. Appl Ergon. 2002, 33: 63-74. 10.1016/S0003-6870(01)00044-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Möller T, Mathiassen SE, Franzon H, Kihlberg S: Job enlargement and mechanical exposure variability in cyclic assembly work. Ergonomics. 2004, 47: 19-40. 10.1080/0014013032000121651.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Gaudart C: Conditions for maintaining ageing operators at work - a case study conducted at an automobile manufacturing plant. Appl Ergon. 2000, 31: 453-462. 10.1016/S0003-6870(00)00024-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Gold JE, Park JS, Punnett L: Work routinization and implications for ergonomic exposure assessment. Ergonomics. 2006, 49: 12-27. 10.1080/00140130500356643.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Hye-Knudsen CT, Schibye B, Hjortskov N, Fallentin N: Trunk motion characteristics during different patient handling tasks. Int J Ind Ergon. 2004, 33: 327-337. 10.1016/j.ergon.2003.09.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Burdorf A: Reducing random measurement error in assessing postural load on the back in epidemiological surveys. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1995, 21: 15-23. 10.5271/sjweh.3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Tielemans E, Kupper LL, Kromhout H, Heederik D, Houba R: Individual-based and group-based occupational exposure assessment: some equations to evaluate different strategies. Ann Occup Hyg. 1998, 42: 115-119.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Seixas NS, Sheppard L: Maximizing accuracy and precision using individual and grouped exposure assessments. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1996, 22: 94-101. 10.5271/sjweh.116.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Reeves GK, Cox DR, Darby SC, Whitley E: Some aspects of measurement error in explanatory variables for continuous and binary regression models. Stat Med. 1998, 17: 2157-2177. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0258(19981015)17:19<2157::AID-SIM916>3.0.CO;2-F.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Spiegelman D, McDermott A, Rosner B: Regression calibration method for correcting measurement-error bias in nutritional epidemiology. Am J Clin Nutr. 1997, 65 (4 suppl): 1179S-1186S.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Stram DO, Hankin JH, Wilkens LR, Pike MC, Monroe KR, Park S, Henderson BE, Nomura AMY, Earle ME, Nagamine FS, et al: Calibration of the dietary questionnaire for a multiethnic cohort in Hawaii and Los Angeles. Am J Epidemiol. 2000, 151: 358-370.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Rosner B, Gore R: Measurement error correction in nutritional epidemiology based on individual foods, with application to the relation of diet to breast cancer. Am J Epidemiol. 2001, 154: 827-835. 10.1093/aje/154.9.827.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Symanski E, Rappaport SM: An investigation of the dependence of exposure variability on the interval between measurements. Ann Occup Hyg. 1994, 38: 361-372. 10.1093/annhyg/38.4.361.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Lampa EG, Nilsson L, Liljelind IE, Bergdahl IA: Optimizing occupational exposure measurement strategies when estimating the log-scale arithmetic mean value - an example from the reinforced plastics industry. Ann Occup Hyg. 2006, 50: 371-377. 10.1093/annhyg/mei078.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Fransson-Hall C, Gloria R, Kilbom Ã…, Winkel J, Karlqvist L, Wiktorin C, Stockholm Music I Study Group: A portable ergonomic observation method (PEO) for computerized on-line recording of postures and manual handling. Appl Ergon. 1995, 26: 93-100. 10.1016/0003-6870(95)00003-U.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Kilbom Ã…, Persson J: Work technique and its consequences for musculoskeletal disorders. Ergonomics. 1987, 30: 273-279. 10.1080/00140138708969706.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Punnett L, Fine L, Keyserling WM, Herrin GD, Chaffin D: Back disorders and non-neutral trunk postures of automobile assembly workers. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1991, 17: 337-346. 10.5271/sjweh.1700.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Häkkänen M, Viikari-Juntura E, Takala E-P: Effects of changes in work methods on musculoskeletal load. An intervention study in the trailer assembly. Appl Ergon. 1997, 28: 99-108. 10.1016/S0003-6870(96)00040-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Denis D, Lortie M, Rossignol M: Observation procedures characterizing occupational physical activities: critical review. Int J Occup Safety Ergon. 2000, 6: 463-491.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  79. Bao S, Howard N, Spielholz P, Silverstein B, Polissar N: Interrater reliability of posture observations. Hum Factors. 2009, 51: 292-309. 10.1177/0018720809340273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Hooftman WE, van der Beek AJ, van de Wal BG, Knol DL, Bongers PM, Burdorf A, van Mechelen W: Equal task, equal exposure? Are men and women with the same tasks equally exposed to awkward working postures?. Ergonomics. 2009, 52: 1079-1086. 10.1080/00140130902915921.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Keyserling M: Postural analysis of the trunk and shoulders in simulated real time. Ergonomics. 1986, 29: 569-583. 10.1080/00140138608968292.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Bao S, Howard N, Spielholz P, Silverstein B: Two posture analysis approaches and their application in a modified rapid upper limb assessment evaluation. Ergonomics. 2009, 50: 2118-2136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was financially supported by the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research (FAS Dnr. 2005-0183 and Dnr. 2009-1761). Jennie Jackson is gratefully acknowledged for valuable viewpoints on the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Svend Erik Mathiassen.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

SEM conceived the study concept, designed the simulation procedures, processed the results, and drafted the manuscript. JW was responsible for the full-shift parent data set collection, took part in designing the data analyses, and contributed significantly to the manuscript. MF took part in designing the data analyses, performed the numerical simulations, and contributed significantly to the manuscript. All three authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mathiassen, S.E., Wahlström, J. & Forsman, M. Bias and imprecision in posture percentile variables estimated from short exposure samples. BMC Med Res Methodol 12, 36 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-12-36

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-12-36

Keywords