Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Understanding complex clinical reasoning in infectious diseases for improving clinical decision support design

Abstract

Background

Clinical experts’ cognitive mechanisms for managing complexity have implications for the design of future innovative healthcare systems. The purpose of the study is to examine the constituents of decision complexity and explore the cognitive strategies clinicians use to control and adapt to their information environment.

Methods

We used Cognitive Task Analysis (CTA) methods to interview 10 Infectious Disease (ID) experts at the University of Utah and Salt Lake City Veterans Administration Medical Center. Participants were asked to recall a complex, critical and vivid antibiotic-prescribing incident using the Critical Decision Method (CDM), a type of Cognitive Task Analysis (CTA). Using the four iterations of the Critical Decision Method, questions were posed to fully explore the incident, focusing in depth on the clinical components underlying the complexity. Probes were included to assess cognitive and decision strategies used by participants.

Results

The following three themes emerged as the constituents of decision complexity experienced by the Infectious Diseases experts: 1) the overall clinical picture does not match the pattern, 2) a lack of comprehension of the situation and 3) dealing with social and emotional pressures such as fear and anxiety. All these factors contribute to decision complexity. These factors almost always occurred together, creating unexpected events and uncertainty in clinical reasoning. Five themes emerged in the analyses of how experts deal with the complexity. Expert clinicians frequently used 1) watchful waiting instead of over- prescribing antibiotics, engaged in 2) theory of mind to project and simulate other practitioners’ perspectives, reduced very complex cases into simple 3) heuristics, employed 4) anticipatory thinking to plan and re-plan events and consulted with peers to share knowledge, solicit opinions and 5) seek help on patient cases.

Conclusion

The cognitive strategies to deal with decision complexity found in this study have important implications for design future decision support systems for the management of complex patients.

Peer Review reports

Background

Electronic Health Record (EHR) systems hold great promise for the development of Clinical Decision Support Systems (CDSS) [1]. CDSS provide intelligently filtered, patient-centered information to clinicians, potentially leading to improved performance and patient outcomes [25], [6, 7]. However, current CDSS tools in EHR systems may not be particularly suitable to assist with complex reasoning because they do not support both the automatic pattern matching of experts and high-level deliberative reasoning required in complex cases [8, 9]. Many different decision support tools are available for integration with the current EHR [10]. Better understanding of automatic pattern matching and analytical reasoning may guide the selection of optimum decision support tools that aid clinicians’ cognition.

Clinical reasoning is a complex process that uses cognition, metacognition and discipline-specific knowledge to gather and analyze patient information, weigh alternatives and evaluate the best possible treatment regimen [11]. In an attempt to understand complexity in clinical reasoning, Grant et al. found that physician-defined patient complexity reflects a wide range of medical, social and behavioral factors that are different from other measures of comorbidity and case-mix adjustments [12]. Rasmussen’s SRK (skill, rule, knowledge) model has also gained popularity in the human factors field [13]. According to this model, people use skill- and rule-based methods of decision-making when the task is less complex and previous experience can help. However, if the situation is unique, the decision-maker has to operate in a knowledge-based level that follows analytical processing with conceptual information. The analytical process relies heavily on mental simulation for assessing the options, hypotheses, actions and alternative plans under consideration [14]. The use of cognitive simulation helps in the generation of ideas about additional information that needs to be obtained and explains why people look for confirming evidence. Mental simulation, especially, helps to generate expectations for other cues not previously considered and guides the observation of changes in system variables [15].

Most CDSS capabilities available in commercial EHR systems (e.g., drug-drug interaction alerts) address low-level cognitive functions, such as reminding or alerting. On the other hand, expert clinicians reason at higher levels of abstraction. Therefore, the key in CDSS design is to provide users a comprehensive view that mitigates the “fog of war” without overloading them with information [16]. Systematic reviews have found that an effective CDSS must minimize the effort required by clinicians to process complex tasks [8]. To support high-level reasoning for clinicians, we need to understand the context of complex decision tasks, the interactions among task attributes and the factors that contribute to the complexity of specific decision tasks [17]. An intelligently designed CDSS and EHR can support clinical reasoning and also reduce mistakes.

Advances in CDSS are particularly necessary in the field of infectious diseases (ID). Despite some early success in seminal CDSS interventions for ID, little progress has been made to assist decision-making in this area [18, 19]. Understanding the complex decision process by ID experts may help in the design of advanced CDSS tools to help with tasks such early infection detection and treatment monitoring [2023]. In addition, given the public health importance of ID, improvements in the understanding of cognitive strategies in ID decision- making have larger population-based implications. By understanding experts’ cognitive mechanisms, the design of future CDSS and EHR systems can incorporate explicit, unified, accurate and comprehensive mental models that match the mental workflow experts [24, 25]. Also, understanding the cognitive mechanisms to deal with the complexity can help us to support adaptive human decision-making by providing useful decision support tools to supplement rather than replace the decision-maker [26]. Understanding the cognitive mechanisms for dealing with complexity can also contribute to the design of better decision support tools for improving mental simulation. Simulation does not necessarily improve decisions, but it often increases confidence in the decision [27]. Previous studies have not looked into the underlying cognitive mechanisms used by ID experts for designing decision support tools. In this study, we seek to address the gap by understanding the cognitive strategies of ID experts for adapting to their environment while dealing with complexity. The findings can help us to provide better decision support tools to supplement clinicians’ cognition.

The overall goal for this study is to identify the constituents of decision complexity and the cognitive strategies to inform the design of health information technology and provide high-level cognitive support to clinicians. Decision complexity affects task performance and the extent and type of decision support used by individuals in decision-making. Decision-making in complex domains is considered to be a function of decision task and the expertise of the decision-maker [28]. Our study is focused on the following research questions: (1) What factors are associated with decision-making complexity experienced by ID experts? (2) What cognitive strategies do ID experts use to deal with complexity?

Methods

Study design

We conducted semistructured interviews with ID experts using Cognitive Task Analysis (CTA) methodology [29]. CTA is a systematic and scientific method used for studying and describing complex reasoning and knowledge that experts use to perform complex tasks [30]. CTA is an effective technique to determine the cognitive skills, strategies and knowledge required to perform tasks [29]. It is composed of several methods for understanding cognition in natural settings. Previous studies were effective in not only analyzing cognitive challenges but also eliciting the organizational challenges and environmental ambiguities of complex, time-pressured, uncertain and high-risk situations using the technique [3134]. In this study, we have used “Combinatorics” of cognitive task analysis, which involved utilizing the critical decision method with critical incident interviews [3537]. We have used the RATS (Relevance of study question, Appropriateness of qualitative method, Transparency of procedure and Soundness of interpretive approach) protocol for qualitative data analysis for the transcriptions of the interviews [38]. The RATS protocol provides standardized guidelines for qualitative research methods.

Settings

The study was conducted at the Salt Lake City Veterans Administration Medical Center and the University of Utah Hospital and was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB). All participants provided oral informed consent that was approved by the University of Utah central IRB (IRB Name-understanding complex clinical decision tasks for better health IT design).

Participants

Participants were 10 infectious disease experts who practice at one of the study sites. We defined “clinical expertise in infectious disease” as board certification in infectious disease, full-time work for a minimum of five years in a clinical environment and the identification by peers as an expert in the infectious disease domain. The first author contacted the participants through email and participation was voluntary. The interviews were conducted in the participants’ private offices.

Procedure

Interviews were conducted according to the Critical Decision Method (CDM), a type of CTA [39]. The CDM procedure is described in Table 1. Each ID expert was asked to describe a recent complex case that was challenging in terms of diagnosis and/or treatment. A semistructured interview script was piloted and refined. The primary author interviewed the participants. At the end of the interviews, participants were asked to provide basic demographic information. The interviews were audio-recorded and transcribed. All identifiers were removed from the transcripts. We have included the questionnaire in the supplemental material of this paper.

Table 1 Critical decision method phases

Data analysis

The research team conducted qualitative thematic analysis of the interview narratives [3941]. The analysis was conducted iteratively, with three of the coauthors (RI, CRW, GDF) independently identifying relevant concepts associated with aspects of complexity, sense-making, cognitive goals and adaptive strategies.

The goal of this analysis was to identify the major categories and themes in the verbal protocol data, including the data analysis step of CDM: data preparation, data structuring, discovering meaning and representing findings [35]. Three researchers conducted the data analysis over multiple sessions. The procedure included detailed and systematic examination of individual interviews and a structured documentation process of coding. The content analysis was conduced in four phases: initial review of the data set, data coding, synthesis and grouping codes and representation by themes. The researchers coded only the statements that increased overall complexity or uncertainty for treating the patient.

From the beginning of the data analysis, the team members collaboratively reviewed initial categories, merged the similar codes and reached consensus if there was any disagreement. First, the codes were discussed in the group meetings. Three researchers then merged their codes into a refined category. In the subsequent meetings, the categories were discussed again and merged into factors that, at the end, were part of the themes.

This collaborative approach of consensus through discussion in qualitative research is rigorous and encourages richer conceptual analysis and interpretation [42]. For example, if the patient had two or more severe health conditions such as a previous accident and HIV, that part was coded as risky patient characteristics. Once the factors such as unexpected outcome, risky patient characteristics and unusual case were grouped, the multidisciplinary research team merged them under a broader theme, such as overall clinical picture does not match pattern.

The use of coding discussions to develop coding structures improves the content of the qualitative data analysis [43, 44]. Also, the multidisciplinary nature of the data analysis team contributes to an understanding of the data from a different perspective and thus adds reflexivity to the analysis [42]. This is an inclusive and inductive process that helped us identify the major themes present in the data set. The initial set of codes emerged after the initial data analysis.

Group consensus was sought at the end of each iteration, and the resulting codes were used in the subsequent iterations. Once all transcripts were coded, similar codes were merged based on code frequency and consensus. In turn, codes were consolidated into high-level themes using data-reduction techniques such as category sorting, in which interview segments are grouped according to content similarity [45]. The final step of the data analysis involved the identification of relationships among themes. Interconnected themes emerged from this analysis. Atlast.ti®7.0, a qualitative research software, was used to conduct the data analysis.

Results

The ID experts had an average of 18.5 years of experience. Of the 10 ID experts, 2 were female and 8 were male.

Factors associated with decision-making complexity

The following themes were identified from the factors contributing to decision-making complexity: 1) the overall clinical picture does not match the pattern, 2) a lack of comprehension of the situation and 3) dealing with social and emotional pressures. These themes included several associated factors. For example, the overall clinical picture does not match pattern consisted of unexpected outcome, risky patient characteristics and unusual case. All these factors refer to situations in which the clinical manifestations of the patient do not match the recognized mental pattern of the clinician. This mismatch in the pattern matching may be the reason for increased uncertainty. In naturalistic decision-making (NDM) environments, the human mind looks for patterns that may help them select optimal courses of action and predict outcomes [46]. The complexity of the situation seems to be higher when clinicians cannot recognize a pattern that matches the patient’s case from their previous experience or training. A lack of comprehension of the situation includes the complexity factors of lack of and/or conflicting indicator data, lack of evidence about treatment effectiveness, lack of diagnosis and gaps in physician’s knowledge. These factors refer to the scarcity of information with clinical utility, which compromises situational awareness. The last theme of social and emotional pressures includes the factors frustration/regret, liability and/or fear and multiple care provider conflict. These factors contribute to clinicians’ anxiety with the decision-making process and the patient’s care.

Table 2 presents a detailed explanation of the constituents of complexity and example quotations from the interview. Emotions such as anxiety, fear or conflicts can also positively help clinicians evaluate better treatment options for the patient. Not all fear or anxiety is negative [47]. Fear and anxiety for the betterment of patients can motivate clinicians to improve clinical decision-making.

Table 2 Constituents of complexity with example quotations

Strategies used to deal with complexity

Five broad themes emerged from the data analysis: 1) watchful waiting instead of prescribing antibiotics: less is more; 2) theory of mind: projection and simulation of other practitioners’ perspectives; 3) heuristics: using shortcut mental model to simplify problems; 4) anticipatory thinking: planning and re-planning for future events; and 5) seeking help: consultation with other experts for their opinions.

Watchful waiting instead of prescribing antibiotics: less is more

In general, expert ID physicians attempt to minimize antibiotic overuse. In this process, they use their clinical expertise and consensus among the team members as a means of seeking support for conservative treatment, such as avoiding overuse of antibiotics or watchful waiting to see if patients improve on their own. Experts engage the principle of “less is more” in clinical reasoning. For example,

“There was nothing that I needed to do today on that patient. Now, again, if I really thought that the risk of endocarditis was high based on the fact that she had a murmur, any other signs or stigmata of endocarditis, then we would have gotten three blood cultures before starting antibiotics.”

“And so even if it’s inappropriate, prescribing an antibiotic is felt like you’re doing something; whereas not prescribing an antibiotic is maybe the more responsible thing to do but it’s still perceived as not doing anything. So, if there’s a complication, if someone gets an antibiotic and they have a complication like say a C diff infection then ‘Eh, it’s just a complication of the antibiotics’; whereas if you don’t treat them, trying to be responsible and not treating them but then they have a complication, let’s say their infection comes back or something else happens, then people will be like, ‘Well, why didn’t you do something about it?’ So that’s your fault whereas if they got a C diff infection that’s not really your fault – that’s just the way it goes.”

Theory of mind: projection and simulation of other practitioners’ perspectives

Theory of mind refers to the cognitive ability or capacity that can attribute mental states to self and others [15]. Experts project and simulate what other practitioners might think in terms of the course of treatment for the patient in order to simplify the problem for better communication. As a result, experts mentally “simulate” possible scenarios of how other clinicians might view past decisions. For example,

“So, you know, I think nowadays, other clinicians might say, even if they don’t have HIV risk factors, you should test them. So, everybody with mono should probably have an HIV test. So maybe, we won’t just do unnecessary tests here.”

“You know, medicine folks would ask you, ‘Well, can we switch to oral now?’ I’m lik,e ‘No, I don’t think so.’ All the time for endocarditis, they’ll ask, ‘Can we use oral drugs? and I’m like, ‘Show me where in the world can you treat bacteremia with oral drugs, that’s where then you can treat them.’ And then the same people, if you don’t treat enough when they’re readmitting they’ll say, ‘Oh, he was insufficiently treated.’”

Heuristics: using shortcut mental model to simplify the problem

Experts construct heuristics to deal with complex cases in order to spare attention resources and to deal with information overload. For example,

“I think usually we would consider stopping therapy in a patient who’s had six months of therapy total, IV and oral for vertebra osteomyelitis in the absence of retained prosthetic material. However, this is his second about to near death with the same pathogen and a very similar infection. He is tolerating the antibiotic very well. So, we’re considering now leaving him on oral suppressive antibiotics indefinitely.”

“So for me it’s always more important to get the right diagnosis and then to follow the guidelines if the person applies to the guidelines. So for me, I would rather initiate a really good history and physical and then secondarily, apply the guidelines to it. And we applied the guidelines and treated him as the guidelines would recommend.”

Anticipatory thinking: planning and re-planning for future events

Anticipatory thinking is the mental projection or simulation of potential events that may affect future decisions and outcomes; it is the “what-if” component of deliberative thought [48]. The ID participants also use a chronological method to understand the patient history in depth to predict the trajectory of the disease state. This form of sense-making of looking forward rather than retrospectively, is part of the macrocognitive process of anticipatory thinking [48]. For example,

“I think the risk/benefit analysis then would favor continuing him on antibiotics because the risk of the antibiotics themselves is very low once he’s tolerated them for a certain amount of time. And the potential consequence is if he relapses from off course then it is very severe. So, in this circumstance, I suspect I’ll probably leave him on antibiotics for quite some time.”

“The question is what can be done so that the infection does not come back. The reason why that’s a question that’s fraught with some anxiety is that in this guy there is a significant downside every time you treat him with antibiotics. Every time he gets antibiotics there were complications, and I worry that there will be more complications that we might not be prepared for.”

Seeking help: consultation with other experts for their opinions

Our analysis found that experts strongly rely on and seek case consultation with other experts they trust. For example,

“We have a weekly conference for the immune-compromised ID docs. We discussed his case in that conference and just reviewed everything, sought out any other opinions, any advice as to what other people might consider for evaluation or duration of therapy and tried to come up with kind of a consensus, which I think was very valuable.”

“But I will admit there have been multiple times since I’ve contacted clinicians I don’t know. I usually contact them through email. If I cannot get hold of them, then I email my colleagues, former mentors, ID physicians working here. I generally describe them about the complex case and ask them what they would do.”

Discussion

Previous studies on complexity in medicine have focused on patient factors related to complexity [12, 17, 4953]. Different patient complexity measures have been developed based on the amount of care provided weighted by the diversity and variability of the patient [5456]. Unlike previous research, the present study contributes to the understanding of complexity from the decision-making perspective. Our results reflect the deep cognitive mechanisms of ID experts to deal with complexity from well-established qualitative methods [29, 32, 36, 5759]. The cognitive mechanisms found in our study have also been described in the context of the cognitive and decision science literature, including naturalistic decision-making, clinical reasoning, heuristics and mental simulation [15, 46, 6063]. The cognitive strategies found in this study can help inform decision support designers for choosing the appropriate tools to be integrated within EHR for dealing with the complexity factors. In the following paragraphs, we discuss how these different tools can support the cognitive strategies for the clinicians to deal with complexity.

The cognitive strategies used by ID experts may help them reduce the identified complexity factors in several ways. These strategies resonate with the findings of Patterson and Woods for individuals dealing with information overload [64]. For example, anticipatory thinking, theory of mind and seeking help can support lack of comprehension of the situation. Risk assessment by using anticipatory thinking helps clinicians prioritize tasks for the best patient outcome [33, 65]. Also, heuristics can help when the overall clinical picture does not match the pattern by a short-cut mental model to fit their patients based on prior experiences [60, 63]. Moreover, watchful waiting provides clinicians the time to comprehend the situation better and reduce the complexity factor of lack of comprehension of the situation. Theory of mind may reduce social and emotional pressures by group conformity and social validation. However, social and emotional pressures make it harder to follow a watchful waiting. The relationships of the cognitive strategies with the sources of decision-making complexity are shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Relationship between cognitive mechanisms with constituents of complexity. The cognitive mechanisms help to deal with constituents of complexity. Only social and pressures make dealing with watchful waiting challenging. Here System 1 and System 2 refer, respectively, to automatic and analytical thinking processes

Dual process theory (DPT) may provide a framework to interpret the results [66]. The DPT postulates two systems of reasoning: System 1 (automatic, nonanalytic, intuitive) and System 2 (effortful, analytic, abstract and logical thinking) cognitive processes [67],[68, 69]. System 2 is activated in situations associated with a high level of novelty and uncertainty, such as when complex patients are encountered. As a result, System 2 imposes significantly higher requirements for attention and cognitive effort than System 1. The cognitive mechanisms identified in this study can be interpreted as reflecting involvement of both System 1 and System 2. In fact, clinicians transition between System 1 and System 2 to efficiently adapt to the environment to deal with complexity. The mechanism of theory of mind requires minimal cognitive capacity and therefore is more System 1 than System 2 whereas anticipatory thinking, seeking help and watchful waiting are more aligned with the System 2 approach due to their effortful nature. Similarly, heuristics, which is a more automated process and thus System 1, can help when the overall clinical picture does not match the pattern by a short-cut mental model to fit their patients based on prior experiences [60, 63].

Implications for decision support

Current and future innovative informatics tools such as patient monitoring, better documentation, better visualization and population-based decision support embedded in EHR systems can facilitate clinicians’ high-level reasoning. The mapping of these tools with the cognitive strategies is illustrated in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mapping of the CDSS tools with cognitive mechanisms and dual process theory. These CDSS tools embedded in the EHR can support both System 1 and System 2 of Dual-Process Theory. Please note that just one kind of CDSS tool may not be sufficient to help with the cognitive switching

Patient monitoring tools such as therapeutic antibiotic monitors and adverse drug event monitors embedded in the EHR have the potential to support System 2 and reduce experts’ mental anxiety in watchful waiting. These tools also provide valuable information for anticipatory thinking [7072]. For example, tele-consultation and monitoring models, such as the ECHO (Extension for Community Healthcare Outcomes) program in New Mexico, include remote patient monitoring features that may guard against or forestall potential future threats.[73] The planning and the ability to monitor the patients may help with better sense-making and preparing for the future to aid anticipatory thinking. In addition, these features may improve providers’ confidence in their decisions, reduce social and emotional pressures and as a result lead to watchful waiting.

Documenting decision trade-offs can reduce the fear of liability or the social and emotional pressures of watchful waiting. Also, better documentation tools that convey the rationale to support treatment decisions can make it easy for providers to understand previous decisions and goals to promote the notion of shared cognition, thereby supporting the theory of mind theme found in our research. Our results also suggest that supporting cognitive switching between System 1 and System 2 helps clinicians effectively manage complex clinical reasoning. For example, “Smart Forms,” a documentation-based clinical decision support system developed at Partners Healthcare, has been shown to improve decision quality and management of patients [74]. By presenting information aligned with users’ workflow, “Smart Forms” help clinicians with the automatic (System 1) thought process. At the same time, the system allows users to switch to an analytical (System 2) thought process through noninterruptive decision support recommendations for medication orders, laboratory tests, future appointments and tailored patient educational material.

Integrated visual displays can provide clinicians with information that matches the heuristics or the high-level mental models. In current EHR systems, information is often presented in a fragmented fashion, splitting a single patient record across multiple screens and modules in different formats. The disjointed records, redundancy of information and sheer volume of shifting data in multiple displays add a significant challenge to clinicians’ sense-making process [7579]. Integrated displays automatically retrieve and process information from disparate modules within the EHR to provide information overview, while preserving the option of in-depth exploration on demand [8082]. For example, a quick overview of a white board display of care coordination has been shown to improve and standardize communication in a care team in an acute care hospital [83].

Also, population-based decision support embedded in EHR systems has the potential to support System 2 with cognitive support for seeking help and watchful waiting [84]. Cognitive support to clinicians refers to maximizing clinicians’ cognitive abilities to improve clinical reasoning and decision-making with the aid of information technology [85]. Population-based decision support is a systematic application for analyzing population databases to improve the health of groups of individuals [84]. Such decision support can work as a “cognitive extension” for clinicians by providing information about treatment response for similar patients and interventions by other clinical experts. This information can help the clinician locate peer consultants who have experience with similar patients. Also, the decision not to prescribe antibiotics by other clinicians in the database can support the cognitive strategy of watchful waiting and reduce the associated social and emotional pressure.

Current information technology systems do not take the user’s emotional state into consideration. The motivational function of emotions consists of their adaptive effects on the user’s goals [47]. Therefore, for better adaptive system design, EHRs may help the clinical user reduce stress by accounting for different constituents of social and emotional pressures. For example, watchful waiting may be stressful and may increase anxiety. However, decision support tools can automatically monitor a patient’s status to help decrease the clinician’s anxiety in a watchful waiting situation.

The sociotechnical barriers that exist in our health information technology infrastructure can benefit from a better understanding of cognitive switching from System 1 to System 2 [2023]. For example, in October 2014, a patient with Ebola virus came to a hospital in Dallas, Texas with classical symptoms of viral fever. Even though the nursing notes included travel history, it was ignored. However, an intelligently designed CDSS that encompasses local and population data could have detected the travel history as a potential threat and warned the clinicians [86]. Thus, future informatics tools that incorporate cognitive strategies into the system design may include the heuristics (System 1) of expert clinicians and thereby act as a cognitive extension to notify (System 2) clinicians about travel history when appropriate.

Implications for research and practice

Overuse of antibiotics has been a concern with respect to drug resistance and public health [87, 88]. The notion that doing less in medicine sometimes can mean more has been an important discussion in the infectious disease community [8991]. More research is needed for innovative decision support systems that can help clinicians by easing the social pressure that results from the active decision to not prescribe antibiotics.

The results of this study suggest a way to rework the paradigm of evidence-based medicine to enhance management of complex clinical tasks. Practice guidelines derived from reviews of evidence typically assume that an experienced clinician is making an assessment of the patient, which is to say that leeway for clinical judgment is allowed. However, when guidelines are incorporated into clinical decision support systems, the usual focus is to induce clinicians to accept rule-based recommendations. The role of judgment may be acknowledged, but resources are not made available to aid clinicians in reasoning through complex problems. Our hypothesis is that decision support systems should be matched to the cognitive mechanisms that clinicians use when managing complex patients. Information displays should facilitate exploration of what-if scenarios in order to improve anticipatory thinking. Better framing of the decision space would help clinicians search for appropriate heuristics and gain confidence from the experience of other clinicians.

Limitations

The Critical Decision Method (CDM) relies on clinicians’ memory of previous cases and therefore is prone to recall bias. Also, experts possess tacit knowledge that is difficult to verbalize and articulate [29]. Thus, the CTA method is limited due to knowledge that cannot be verbalized in principle. Also, since the first author conducted the data collections, this researcher may have influenced the way the interview was conducted. To guard against this bias, we piloted and constructed the questionnaire based on the CDM instrument. In addition, our results reflect the opinions and deep cognitive processes of ID experts, which may have influenced the generalizability of the results. However, as infection is prevalent in all aspects of medicine, these results can be translated for broader impact in all areas of clinical medicine.

Conclusion

The cognitive factors that may contribute to decision complexity include 1) overall clinical picture does not match the pattern, 2) lack of comprehension of the situation and 3) social and emotional pressures. ID experts use the following mechanisms to deal with decision complexity: 1) watchful waiting instead of prescribing antibiotics: less is more; 2) theory of mind: projection and simulation of other practitioners’ perspectives; 3) heuristics: using shortcut mental models to simplify problems; 4) anticipatory thinking: planning and re-planning for future events; and 5) seeking help: consultation with other experts for their opinions. Future and innovative decision support tools in the EHR may facilitate the cognitive switching from System 1 to System 2 to match experts’ high-level reasoning. CDSS and EHR designers can incorporate the cognitive mechanisms found in our study to inform the design of innovative solutions.

Abbreviations

EHR:

Electronic Health Record

CDSS:

Clinical Decision Support Systems

ID:

Infectious Diseases

CTA:

Cognitive Task Analysis

CDM:

Critical Decision Method

IRB:

Institutional Review Board

NDM:

Naturalistic Decision Making

ECHO:

Extension for Community Healthcare Outcomes

References

  1. Jones SS, Rudin RS, Perry T, Shekelle PG. Health information technology: an updated systematic review with a focus on meaningful use. Ann Intern Med. 2014;160(1):48–54. doi:10.7326/M13-1531.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Effken JA, Brewer BB, Logue MD, Gephart SM, Verran JA. Using cognitive work analysis to fit decision support tools to nurse managers’ work flow. Int J Med Inform. 2011;80(10):698–707. doi:10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2011.07.003.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Horsky J, Phansalkar S, Desai A, Bell D, Middleton B. Design of decision support interventions for medication prescribing. Int J Med Inform. 2013;82(6):492–503. doi:10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2013.02.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Sintchenko V, Coiera EW. Which clinical decisions benefit from automation? A task complexity approach. Int J Med Inform. 2003;70(2–3):309–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Sittig DF, Wright A, Osheroff JA, Middleton B, Teich JM, Ash JS, et al. Grand challenges in clinical decision support. J Biomed Inform. 2008;41(2):387–92. doi:10.1016/j.jbi.2007.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Osheroff JA, Teich JM, Middleton B, Steen EB, Wright A, Detmer DE. A roadmap for national action on clinical decision support. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2007;14(2):141–5. doi:10.1197/jamia.M2334.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Shaffer, J. and Coustasse, A. Computer physician order entry and clinical decision support systems: Benefits and concerns. Business and Health Administration Association Annual Conference 2012. Paper presented at the Business and Health Administration Association (BHAA) Annual Conference 2012, at the 48th Annual Midwest Business Administration Association International Conference. Chicago, Illinois. Mar 2012.

  8. Kawamoto K, Houlihan CA, Balas EA, Lobach DF. Improving clinical practice using clinical decision support systems: a systematic review of trials to identify features critical to success. BMJ. 2005;330(7494):765.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Miller RA. Computer-assisted diagnostic decision support: history, challenges, and possible paths forward. Adv Health Sci Educ. 2009;14(1 SUPPL):89–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Welch BM, Kawamoto K. Clinical decision support for genetically guided personalized medicine: a systematic review. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2013;20(2):388–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Simmons B. Clinical reasoning: concept analysis. J Adv Nurs. 2010;66(5):1151–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Grant RW, Ashburner JM, Hong CS, Chang Y, Barry MJ, Atlas SJ. Defining patient complexity from the primary care physician’s perspective: a cohort study. Ann Intern Med. 2011;155(12):797–804. doi:10.1059/0003-4819-155-12-201112200-00001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Rasmussen J. Risk management in a dynamic society: a modelling problem. Saf Sci. 1997;27(2):183–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Converse S. Shared mental models in expert team decision making. Individual and group decision making: Current Issues. 1993;221

  15. Markman KD, Klein WM, Suhr JA. Handbook of imagination and mental simulation. New York: Psychology Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Wu HW, Davis PK, Bell DS. Advancing clinical decision support using lessons from outside of healthcare: an interdisciplinary systematic review. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2012;12:90. doi:10.1186/1472-6947-12-90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Sintchenko V, Coiera E. Decision complexity affects the extent and type of decision support use. AMIA Annu Symp Proc. 2006:724–8

  18. Khabbaz RF, Moseley RR, Steiner RJ, Levitt AM, Bell BP. Challenges of infectious diseases in the USA. Lancet. 2014;384(9937):53–63. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(14)60890-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Tleyjeh IM, Nada H, Baddour LM. VisualDx: decision-support software for the diagnosis and management of dermatologic disorders. Clin Infect Dis. 2006;43(9):1177–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Fong IW. Challenges in infectious diseases. New York: Springer; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  21. Mandl KD, Overhage JM, Wagner MM, Lober WB, Sebastiani P, Mostashari F, et al. Implementing syndromic surveillance: a practical guide informed by the early experience. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2004;11(2):141–50. doi:10.1197/jamia.M1356.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Morens DM, Folkers GK, Fauci AS. The challenge of emerging and re-emerging infectious diseases. Nature. 2004;430(6996):242–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. El-Kareh R, Hasan O, Schiff GD. Use of health information technology to reduce diagnostic errors. BMJ Quality & Safety. 2013;22 Suppl 2:ii40–51. doi:10.1136/bmjqs-2013-001884.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Burtscher MJ, Manser T. Team mental models and their potential to improve teamwork and safety: a review and implications for future research in healthcare. Saf Sci. 2012;50(5):1344–54. doi:10.1016/j.ssci.2011.12.033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Papp KK, Huang GC, Lauzon Clabo LM, Delva D, Fischer M, Konopasek L, et al. Milestones of critical thinking: a developmental model for medicine and nursing. Acad Med. 2014;89(5):715–20. doi:10.1097/acm.0000000000000220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Islam R, Weir C, Del Fiol G. Clinical complexity in medicine: a measurement model of task and patient complexity. Methods of Information in Medicine. 2015;54(5). doi:10.3414/me15-01-0031.

  27. Roth EM, Woods DD, Pople Jr HE. Cognitive simulation as a tool for cognitive task analysis. Ergonomics. 1992;35(10):1163–98. doi:10.1080/00140139208967389.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Sintchenko V, Coiera E, editors. Decision complexity affects the extent and type of decision support use. AMIA Annual Symposium Proceedings; 2006: American Medical Informatics Association.

  29. Hoffman RR, Militello LG. Perspectives on cognitive task analysis: historical origins and modern communities of practice. New York: Psychology Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Lobach DSG, Bright TJ. Enabling Health Care Decisionmaking Through Clinical Decision Support and Knowledge Management. Evidence Report/Technology Assessments. Rockville: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Pugh CM, DaRosa DA. Use of cognitive task analysis to guide the development of performance-based assessments for intraoperative decision making. Mil Med. 2013;178(10 Suppl):22–7. doi:10.7205/milmed-d-13-00207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Islam R, Weir C, Fiol GD, editors. Heuristics in managing complex clinical decision tasks in experts’ decision making. Healthcare Informatics (ICHI), 2014 IEEE International Conference on; 2014 15–17 Sept. 2014.

  33. Sitterding MC, Ebright P, Broome M, Patterson ES, Wuchner S. Situation awareness and interruption handling during medication administration. West J Nurs Res. 2014;36(7):891–916. doi:10.1177/0193945914533426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Thyvalikakath TP, Dziabiak MP, Johnson R, Torres-Urquidy MH, Acharya A, Yabes J, et al. Advancing cognitive engineering methods to support user interface design for electronic health records. Int J Med Inform. 2014;83(4):292–302. doi:10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2014.01.007.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Crandall B, Klein GA, Hoffman RR. Working minds: a practitioner’s guide to cognitive task analysis. Cambride: MIT Press; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Klein G. Cognitive task analysis of teams. Cognitive Task Analysis. 2000;11:417–29.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Craig C, Klein MI, Griswold J, Gaitonde K, McGill T, Halldorsson A. Using cognitive task analysis to identify critical decisions in the laparoscopic environment. Hum Factors. 2012;54(6):1025–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Clark J. How to peer review a qualitative manuscript. Peer Review in Health Sciences. 2003;2:219–35.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Crandall B, Klein G, Hoffman R. Working minds: a practitioner’s guide to cognitive task analysis. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Wong BW, Blandford A. Analysing ambulance dispatcher decision making: Trialing emergent themes analysis. 2002. Ergonomics Society of Australia

  41. Hoffman RR, Crandall B, Shadbolt NR. Use of the critical decision method to elicit expert knowledge: a case study in the methodology of cognitive task analysis. Hum Factors. 1998;40:254–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Barry CA, Britten N, Barber N, Bradley C, Stevenson F. Using reflexivity to optimize teamwork in qualitative research. Qual Health Res. 1999;9(1):26–44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Barbour RS. Checklists for improving rigour in qualitative research: a case of the tail wagging the dog? BMJ. 2001;322(7294):1115–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Berends L, Johnston J. Using multiple coders to enhance qualitative analysis: the case of interviews with consumers of drug treatment. Addict Res Theory. 2005;13(4):373–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Coxon APM. Sorting data: collection and analysis. London: Sage; 1999.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  46. Klein G. Naturalistic decision making. Hum Factors. 2008;50(3):456–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Calvo RA, D’Mello S, Gratch J, Kappas A. The Oxford handbook of affective computing. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Mosier KL, Fischer UM. Informed by knowledge: expert performance in complex situations. New York: Psychology Press; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Bedny GZ, Karwowski W, Bedny IS. Complexity evaluation of computer-based tasks. Int J Hum Comput Interact. 2012;28(4):236–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Franklin A, Liu Y, Li Z, Nguyen V, Johnson TR, Robinson D, et al. Opportunistic decision making and complexity in emergency care. J Biomed Inform. 2011;44(3):469–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Jenkins DP. Cognitive work analysis: coping with complexity. Burlington, VT: Ashgate Publishing, Ltd.; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Kannampallil TG, Schauer GF, Cohen T, Patel VL. Considering complexity in healthcare systems. J Biomed Inform. 2011;44(6):943–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Meyer TS, Muething JZ, Lima GA, Torres BR, del Rosario TK, Gomes JO. Radiological emergency response for community agencies with cognitive task analysis, risk analysis, and decision support framework. Work (Reading, Mass). 2012;41 Suppl 1:2925–32. doi:10.3233/wor-2012-0659-2925.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Katerndahl DA, Wood R, Jaen CR. A method for estimating relative complexity of ambulatory care. Ann Fam Med. 2010;8(4):341–7. doi:10.1370/afm.1157.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Vance J, Sprivulis P. Triage nurses validly and reliably estimate emergency department patient complexity. Emerg Med Australas. 2005;17(4):382–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Shippee ND, Shah ND, May CR, Mair FS, Montori VM. Cumulative complexity: a functional, patient-centered model of patient complexity can improve research and practice. J Clin Epidemiol. 2012;65(10):1041–51. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2012.05.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Crandall B, Klein G, Hoffman R. Incident-based CTA: Helping practitioners “tell stories”. Working minds: a practitioners’ or ‘s guide to cognitive task analysis. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2006. p. 69–90.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Militello LG, Hutton RJ. Applied cognitive task analysis (ACTA): a practitioner’s toolkit for understanding cognitive task demands. Ergonomics. 1998;41(11):1618–41. doi:10.1080/001401398186108.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Weir CR, Nebeker JJR, Hicken BL, Campo R, Drews F, LeBar B. A cognitive task analysis of information management strategies in a computerized provider order entry environment. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2007;14(1):65–75.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Gigerenzer G, Hertwig R, Pachur T. Heuristics: the foundations of adaptive behavior. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc.; 2011.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  61. Gigerenzer G, Kurzenhäuser S. Fast and frugal heuristics in medical decision making. Science and Medicine in Dialogue: Thinking Through Particulars and Universals. I assume this is the journal title. 2005:3–15.

  62. Gorini A, Pravettoni G. An overview on cognitive aspects implicated in medical decisions. Eur J Intern Med. 2011;22(6):547–53. doi:10.1016/j.ejim.2011.06.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Kahneman D, Klein G. Conditions for intuitive expertise: a failure to disagree. Am Psychol. 2009;64(6):515–26. doi:10.1037/a0016755.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Patterson ES, Woods DD, Tinapple D, Roth EM, editors. Using cognitive task analysis (CTA) to seed design concepts for intelligence analysts under data overload. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting; 2001: Sage Publications.

  65. Wegwarth O, Gaissmaier W, Gigerenzer G. Smart strategies for doctors and doctors-in-training: heuristics in medicine. Med Educ. 2009;43(8):721–8. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03359.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Sherman JW, Gawronski B, Trope Y. Dual-process theories of the social mind. New York Guilford Publications; 2014

  67. Dhanaraj C, Lyles MA, Steensma HK, Tihanyi L. Managing tacit and explicit knowledge transfer in IJVs: the role of relational embeddedness and the impact on performance. J Int Bus Stud. 2004;35(5):428–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Thammasitboon S, Cutrer WB. Diagnostic decision-making and strategies to improve diagnosis. Curr Probl Pediatr Adolesc Health Care. 2013;43(9):232–41. doi:10.1016/j.cppeds.2013.07.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Marcum JA. An integrated model of clinical reasoning: dual-process theory of cognition and metacognition. J Eval Clin Pract. 2012;18(5):954–61. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2753.2012.01900.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Timmermans DR, Sprij AJ, de Bel CE. The discrepancy between daily practice and the policy of a decision-analytic model: the management of fever of unknown origin. Med Decis Making. 1996;16(4):357–66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Brown KA, Khanafer N, Daneman N, Fisman DN. Meta-analysis of antibiotics and the risk of community-associated Clostridium difficile infection. Antimicrob Agents Chemother. 2013;57(5):2326–32. doi:10.1128/AAC.02176-12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. MacKenzie A. Balancing the benefits and risks of empirical antibiotics for sinusitis: a teachable moment. JAMA Intern Med. 2014;174(8):1221–2. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.2124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Arora S, Thornton K, Murata G, Deming P, Kalishman S, Dion D, et al. Outcomes of treatment for hepatitis C virus infection by primary care providers. N Engl J Med. 2011;364(23):2199–207.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Schnipper JL, Linder JA, Palchuk MB, Einbinder JS, Li Q, Postilnik A, et al. “Smart Forms” in an electronic medical record: documentation-based clinical decision support to improve disease management. J Am Med Inform Assoc. 2008;15(4):513–23.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Zhang J, Norman DA. Representations in distributed cognitive tasks. Cogn Sci. 1994;18(1):87–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Zhang Y, Li Z, Wu B, Wu S. A spaceflight operation complexity measure and its experimental validation. Int J Ind Ergon. 2009;39(5):756–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Elliott AF, Davidson A, Lum F, Chiang MF, Saaddine JB, Zhang X, et al. Use of electronic health records and administrative data for public health surveillance of eye health and vision-related conditions in the United States. Am J Ophthalmol. 2012;154(6 Suppl):S63–70. doi:10.1016/j.ajo.2011.10.002.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Zhang J, Patel VL, Johnson KA, Smith JW, Malin J. Designing human-centered distributed information systems. IEEE Intell Syst. 2002;17(5):42–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  79. Laxmisan A, Hakimzada F, Sayan OR, Green RA, Zhang J, Patel VL. The multitasking clinician: decision-making and cognitive demand during and after team handoffs in emergency care. Int J Med Inform. 2007;76(11–12):801–11. doi:10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2006.09.019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Shneiderman B. Enabling visual discovery. Science. 2014;343(6171):614-. doi:10.1126/science.1249670.

  81. Shneiderman B. Medical illuminations using evidence, visualizations and statistical thinking to improve healthcare. Science. 2014;343(6171):614.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  82. Shneiderman B, Plaisant C, Hesse B. Improving health and healthcare with interactive visualization methods. IEEE Computer Special Issue on Challenges in Information Visualization. 2013:58–66

  83. Wong HJ, Caesar M, Bandali S, Agnew J, Abrams H. Electronic inpatient whiteboards: improving multidisciplinary communication and coordination of care. Int J Med Inform. 2009;78(4):239–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Krall MAG, Samore MH. Big data and population-based decision support. In: Greenes RA, editor. Clinical decison support: the road to broad adoption. 2nd ed. Oxford: Elsevier Inc.; 2014. p. 363–78.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  85. Newell A, Carmichael A, Gregor P, Alm N, Waller A. Information technology for cognitive support. 2003

  86. Carroll LN, Au AP, Detwiler LT, Fu TC, Painter IS, Abernethy NF. Visualization and analytics tools for infectious disease epidemiology: a systematic review. J Biomed Inform. 2014;51:287–98. doi:10.1016/j.jbi.2014.04.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Murray JS, Amin PM. Overprescribing antibiotics in children: an enduring public health concern. J Spec Pediatr Nurs. 2014;19(3):266–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Ascioglu S, Samore MH, Lipsitch M. A new approach to the analysis of antibiotic resistance data from hospitals. Microb Drug Resist. 2014;20:6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Bell M. Antibiotic misuse: a global crisis. JAMA Intern Med. 2014;174(12):1920–1. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.3289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Bernstein RK. More can be life threatening. JAMA Intern Med. 2014;174(6):858. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.1229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Sullivan T. Antibiotic overuse and Clostridium difficile: a teachable moment. JAMA Intern Med. 2014;174(8):1219–20. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.2299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This project was supported by the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (grant R36HS023349) and Department of Veterans Affairs Research and Development (grand CRE 12–230). Dr. Islam was supported by National Library of Medicine training grant (T15-LM07124). The authors are grateful to all the members of the Infectious Diseases team at the University of Utah and VA Salt Lake City Health System.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Roosan Islam.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

Authors’ contributions

RI, CRW and GDF designed and conducted the study, analyzed the data and wrote the paper. MHS and MJ assisted in analyzing the data and reviewed versions of the manuscript. RI and MHS obtained funding for the study. All authors have read and approved the final transcript.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

CDM ID Script. (DOCX 94 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Islam, R., Weir, C.R., Jones, M. et al. Understanding complex clinical reasoning in infectious diseases for improving clinical decision support design. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak 15, 101 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12911-015-0221-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12911-015-0221-z

Keywords