Skip to main content

Dogs with separation-related problems show a “less pessimistic” cognitive bias during treatment with fluoxetine (Reconcile™) and a behaviour modification plan

Abstract

Background

Canine separation-related problems (SRP) (also described as “separation anxiety” or “separation distress”) are among the most common behavioural complaints of dog owners. Treatment with psychoactive medication in parallel with a behaviour modification plan is well documented in the literature, but it is unknown if this is associated with an improvement in underlying affective state (emotion and mood) or simply an inhibition of the behaviour. Cognitive judgement bias tasks have been proposed as a method for assessing underlying affective state and so we used this approach to identify if any change in clinical signs during treatment was associated with a consistent change in cognitive bias (affective state).

Five dogs showing signs of SRP (vocalising – e.g. barking, howling-, destruction of property, and toileting – urination or defecation- when alone) were treated with fluoxetine chewable tablets (Reconcile™) and set on a standard behaviour modification plan for two months. Questionnaires and interviews of the owners were used to monitor the clinical progress of the dogs. Subjects were also evaluated using a spatial cognitive bias test to infer changes in underlying affect prior to, and during, treatment. Concurrently, seven other dogs without signs of SRP were tested in the same way to act as controls. Furthermore, possible correlations between cognitive bias and clinical measures were also assessed for dogs with SRP.

Results

Prior to treatment, the dogs with SRP responded to ambiguous positions in the cognitive bias test negatively (i.e. with slower running speeds) compared to control dogs (p < 0.05). On weeks 2 and 6 of treatment, SRP dogs displayed similar responses in the cognitive bias test to control dogs, consistent with the possible normalization of affect during treatment, with this effect more pronounced at week 6 (p > 0.05). Questionnaire based clinical measures were significantly correlated among themselves and with performance in the cognitive bias test.

Conclusion

These results demonstrate for the first time that the clinical treatment of a negative affective state and associated behaviours in a non-human species can produce a shift in cognitive bias. These findings demonstrate how the outcome of an intervention on a clinical problem can be evaluated to determine not only that the subject’s behaviour has improved, but also its psychological state (welfare)

Background

Separation-related problems (SRP) in dogs are a common behavioural complaint of owners [1-3]. Approximately 14%-20% of dog patients [4,5] from general veterinary practice show SRP signs such as vocalising, destruction of property, and toileting when alone [6]. However, it is suspected that up to 50-56% of the whole dog population may display clinical symptoms of SRP at some point in their life [7,8], which out of the total population of approximately 70 million dogs in US and 70 million in Europe [9,10] represents about 35 million dogs with SRP in the Europe and 35 million dogs in US.

Even though this behavioural condition is common, the underlying motivation(s) of SRP and the causal aetiology of this phenomenon are still a matter of debate [11-13]. It has been suggested that an animal’s affective state (emotional predisposition or moods) can be indirectly assessed through the use of cognitive bias (CB) testing [14]. CB testing is based on the finding that decision making is affected by background affective state and, in the case of ambiguous stimuli; individuals experiencing negative affect tend to make more negative judgements than those in a more positive emotional state [14-17], for example, anxious people tend to interpret ambiguous situations more unfavourably. In spatial CB tests, animals are trained to discriminate between bowls placed in two different locations, one with food present (R+), and one without food (R-). Subsequently, bowls were placed in ambiguous, unrewarded locations (‘probes’) between the learned locations. A more “optimistic” bias is reflected in running relatively faster to the ambiguous probes, and a more “pessimistic” bias inferred by the converse [17]. Recently, it has been found that dogs in rescue kennels with SRP-like behaviour tested on a modified spatial cognitive bias test appear to have more negative underlying affective state than dogs not showing this behaviour [18]. However, it is not known whether these findings are transferable to the clinical condition of household pets.

These cases can often be managed effectively with a behaviour modification plan plus a pharmaceutical adjunct, such as a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, like fluoxetine, or a non-specific serotonin reuptake inhibitor, like clomipramine [19-22]. There is strong evidence for an increased rate of response to the behaviour modification plan when such medication is included [19-21]. However, because serotonergic medication can cause a general inhibition of behaviour, especially at higher doses [23], it is not known if the combined treatment with medication is associated with a concomitant improvement in well-being; i.e. whether the programme including medication improves the underlying affective state of the animal or simply inhibits the problem behaviour.

The aim of the present study was therefore to investigate whether clinical improvement of SRP-afflicted dogs, during treatment with a combination of fluoxetine and a standard behaviour modification plan, resulted in measurable changes in CB tests consistent with either an improvement in underlying affective state or general motor inhibition. In the former, we would hypothesise that running speeds should increase due to greater “optimism”, whereas in the latter they would be expected to decrease as a result of behavioural inhibition.

Methods

Ethics statement

This research was approved by the School of Life Sciences Ethics Committee at the University of Lincoln, UK. The owners of all the dogs have given written informed consent for the use of their dogs in that research.

Since previous studies (e.g. [19]) have demonstrated a greater improvement in the problem using a behaviour modification plan combined with fluoxetine compared to a placebo control, and because the aim of this study was primarily to investigate the correlates of improvement when using fluoxetine in conjunction with a behaviour modification plan, a placebo was considered unnecessary as well as undesirable from an ethical perspective.

Subjects

Two groups of dogs were recruited for the study, one with SRP, hereby referred to as the clinical group, and one without SRP or any other diagnosed anxiety problem, designated the control group (Additional file 1: Table S1). All dogs were assessed and diagnosed by a veterinary clinician specialising in behavioural medicine. The purpose of the control group was to provide a time related control for the dogs with SRP and to control for the effects of repeated CB testing in dogs, since it has been suggested that judgement CB tests may be subject to learning effects that limit repeatability [24].

Recruitment of dogs with SRP was through advertisements in the local media in the Lincoln UK area and through veterinary clinics in both London and Lincoln. Inclusion and exclusion criteria for study selection were predefined (Table 1). The recruitment period occurred from July 2010 until February 2011. In total, 22 dog owners expressed interest in taking part in the study. Among these, 14 owners were not willing to comply with the study protocol requirements. One dog was excluded before the beginning of the study due to a clinical history of seizures, and another was excluded during treatment for medical reasons (co-incidental gastric dilatation and volvulus). One dog was also excluded due to its low food motivation, making it difficult to train in the CB test. Dogs excluded from the study were provided with a behaviour consultation and modification plan, without psychoactive medication (unless the owner was willing to cover the medication’s cost) in accordance with the agreed management of subjects described in our submission for ethical approval. Therefore, the clinical group was composed of five dogs of various breeds with SRP, 3 males (2 neutered and one intact) and 2 females (both neutered). The average age of the dogs was 4.2 years (range 2–6.5 years) and they had been owned by their current owners for 2.4 years on average (range 2 months - 6 years). Their average body weight was 22.6 kg (range 11–30.7 kg).

Table 1 Inclusion and exclusion criteria for dogs taking part in the study

For the control group, pet dogs were recruited on the basis of an absence of any SRP-related incidents or any other anxiety problem. These dogs were recruited via local advertisement within the University of Lincoln to both university students and staff. The owners had to complete a questionnaire about their dogs’ behaviour. Only if the dogs fulfilled inclusion criteria 1–4 in Table 1, above and met none of the exclusion criteria, were they included in the study. Eight dogs of various breeds were recruited. One dog was subsequently excluded due to low food motivation. This left 3 males (all neutered) and 4 females (3 neutered and 1 intact). The average age of the dogs was 4.9 years old (range 7 months and 11.5 years old), and they had been owned for, on average, 4.6 years (range 7 months - 10 years). Their average body weight was 19.6 kg (range 10.5 - 27 kg).

Cognitive Bias (CB) test

The dogs underwent an assessment of cognitive judgement bias before and during treatment. The CB test was based on Mendl et al. [18]. The dogs were trained to discriminate a food bowl placed in one of two different locations, one with food and one without. The locations were the same distance from the start position as illustrated in Figure 1. Dogs were trained such that when the bowl was placed to one side of the room, (the positive location, R+), it would contain food, and when it was placed on the other side, (the negative location, R-), the bowl would be empty (Figure 1). Following a random allocation procedure, the positive location was to the right for three dogs from the clinical group and for four dogs from the control group, whereas for the remaining two dogs from the clinical group and three dogs from the control group it was to their left. The learning phase was completed after a minimum 15 trials, and determined to be complete when, for three consecutive negative trials, their average absolute speed (m/s) towards the negative location was less than the average speed to the positive location. Dogs were then tested with three ambiguous probes positioned between the learned ‘reference’ locations: (1) midway (MID) between the positive and the negative locations, (2) midway between the positive and MID probe locations (NR+); (3) midway between the negative and MID probe locations (NR-) (Figure 1). All ambiguous locations were unrewarded (Additional file 1). The testing phase contained nine exposures to the three ambiguous probes in total, i.e. three for each probe, interspersed between exposures to the learned ‘reference’ locations. In total, the testing phase included 40 trials, as testing started with a probe trial.

Figure 1
figure 1

Schematic representation of the Cognitive Bias test layout. The dog, starting from the start position, moves to one of five locations on an arc 3–4 metres away, marked by the presence of a food bowl (only present at one location at any one trial). R-: Non Food position (negative location), NR- : Near Non Food ambiguous position, MID: Middle ambiguous position, NR+: Near Food ambiguous position and R+: Food position (positive location). Different distances were used on the basis of available space, but were consistent within subjects.

Study design

In accordance with other clinical trials for this behaviour problem, baseline measures were gathered over a two week period before the onset of 8 weeks of treatment [14-16]. For the control group, the study lasted only 6 weeks as the dogs did not require either the pre-enrolment visit which was used to instigate measurement of baseline severity of SRP nor the follow-up at the end of the treatment period (Table 2). Three CB tests were performed by both groups with the same interval between tests +/− 2 days (Table 2).

Table 2 Study schedule of events for the clinical and control groups

Clinical group

At the first visit (Pre-enrolment) the owners completed a pre-screening questionnaire about their dog and provided a behaviour history of their dog’s separation behaviour (approximately duration 1–1.5 hours) (Table 1). The inclusion and exclusion criteria were reviewed and a physical examination was performed on the dog by a veterinarian specialising in behavioural medicine (CK), with diagnoses confirmed by the supervising behaviour specialist (DM). Suitable dogs were then pre-enrolled for the study and the owner completed an informed consent form. Then, the eliciting contexts (EC) were identified, i.e. times when the owner left the dog on its own, e.g. for work or to go shopping, which might evoke a SRP-related response [13]. For the following two weeks, the owners recorded on individualised recording sheets, for each EC, the specific separation-related behaviours (SR-BEH) of the dog (dog’s diary). A similar record was kept for the last two weeks of the treatment period for comparative purposes. At the baseline visit, if the dogs had shown more than eight SR-BEH during the two-week pre-treatment period, they were enrolled as cases.

At the baseline visit, dogs were assessed for the first time with a cognitive bias test (CB1) and the owners completed a 3-part questionnaire about their dog’s tendency for attachment towards them, the SR-BEH it showed, and provided a “separation anxiety global score” for the SRP (SAGS) (Additional file 1). The same questionnaire was completed during the telephone follow-up sessions. All questionnaire measures related to the owner’s assessment of the frequency or intensity of behaviours over the previous two weeks rather than on any specific day. A standardised behaviour modification plan (Table 3) was explained to the owners and Fluoxetine (Reconcile™, Elanco, 1-2 mg/kg by mouth, once daily) was provided at this time. The next two visits (week +2 & week +6) were during the treatment period and were booked at this time for two and six weeks after baseline respectively, when the owners again completed the same 3-part questionnaire and the dog was again CB tested.

Table 3 Summary of behaviour modification plan, used in treatment of cases

The same medication and dose (Reconcile™, Elanco, 1-2 mg/kg by mouth, once daily) as has been used previously in a placebo controlled clinical trial was used [19]. A placebo tablet treated group was scientifically unnecessary for the aims of the present study, which was to investigate whether clinical improvement of SRP-afflicted dogs during treatment resulted in measurable changes in CB tests and not to determine the effect of the different treatment components. For the same reason, owners were not blinded as there was no placebo group and also because the owners’ perception of their dog’s behaviour, recorded via questionnaire, could be compared with the more objective CB tests. A control ‘no treatment’ group tested at similar intervals was used to control for time related effects and the effects of repeat testing with the CB tests.

Control group

Similar to the clinical group, at the first enrolment visit the owners completed a pre-screening questionnaire about their dog and provided a behaviour history about the dog’s separation behaviour. The inclusion and exclusion criteria were reviewed and a physical examination was performed on the dog by the same veterinarian as the clinical subjects. Then, potential eliciting contexts were identified, i.e. times when the owner left the dog on its own, e.g. during work or shopping, which might theoretically evoke a problem response. Retrospectively for the previous two weeks, the owners recorded for each EC the (SR-BEH). If the dogs had shown only one or no SR-BEH during the two-week pre-treatment period, they were enrolled onto the study and the owner completed an informed consent form.

Then the owners completed the same 3-part questionnaire about their dog’s tendency for attachment towards them and SR-BEH, and a global score for the SR-BEH (SAGS). Dogs that could complete the CB test procedure and fulfilled the EC’s frequency requirements (Table 1) were included in the study data set. Owners were also asked to report any behaviour changes in their dog during the assessment period, in order to avoid including dogs with SRP in the control group.

The dogs of the control group performed the CB test on baseline, and 2 and 6 weeks later, on weeks +2 and +6 respectively. They were tested in equivalent conditions to the dogs in the clinical Group.

Statistical analysis

The age, the duration of their ownership and the body weight of subjects in the clinical and control groups were compared using a Mann–Whitney U test. Due to the different sizes and possible food motivations of the dogs, the running speeds of each dog was adjusted as recommended by Mendl et al. [18], to take into account the mean speed of subjects to get to the positive and negative locations at the end of training. Adjusted speed during testing was calculated as follows:

$$ \begin{array}{l}\mathbf{Adjusted}\ \mathbf{speed}\kern0.5em =\kern0.5em \Big[\left(\mathrm{mean}\ \mathrm{speed}\ \mathrm{t}\mathrm{o}\ \mathrm{positive}\ \mathrm{location}\ \hbox{--}\ \mathrm{speed}\ \mathrm{t}\mathrm{o}\ \mathrm{probe}\ \mathrm{location}\right)\\ {}\times \kern0.5em \mathrm{mean}\ \mathrm{speed}\ \mathrm{t}\mathrm{o}\ \mathrm{negative}\ \mathrm{location}\times 100\Big]\\ {}/\Big[\left(\mathrm{mean}\ \mathrm{speed}\ \mathrm{t}\mathrm{o}\ \mathrm{positive}\ \mathrm{location}\ \hbox{--}\ \mathrm{mean}\ \mathrm{speed}\ \mathrm{t}\mathrm{o}\ \mathrm{negative}\ \mathrm{location}\right)\\ {}\times \kern0.5em \mathrm{speed}\ \mathrm{t}\mathrm{o}\ \mathrm{probe}\ \mathrm{location}\Big].\end{array} $$

Consequently, if the speed to the probe location was equal to the mean speed to positive (food) location, the adjusted score was 0, and if the speed to probe location was equal to the mean speed to negative (non-food) location, the score was 100 (Additional file 1). Only adjusted speeds towards the ambiguous probes are subsequently assessed and reported.

The data were analysed using SPSS 17.0. Normality was assessed using Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests, and, as some factors were significantly different to normal (P < 0.05), all data were analysed using non-parametric statistics.

A statistical plan was formulated to accommodate the relatively small sample size, (we used 12 dogs in total, compared to 9 similar studies testing cognitive bias in dogs, rats, starlings and sheep that used from 6 to 32 animals (mean number of subjects: 19.9) [18,24-31], and consideration that a Bonferonni correction would be too conservative and likely to result in Type II errors [32]. As a precaution against the risk of Type I errors, the statistical plan prioritised testing of the primary hypotheses of interest (differences at baseline and time of maximum clinical effect i.e. week 6). The effect size (r) was calculated in each instance [33]. Subsequent data exploration was performed, conditional upon the outcome of these primary findings.

The first priority was to establish whether the clinical group differed significantly to the control group in their CB performance towards ambiguous probes at baseline, as would be expected given the results of Mendl et al. [18], (Mann–Whitney U-test). Where differences between groups were found, these were evaluated in line with the prediction that the clinical group should show a negative affective bias.

The next analysis compared the performance of subjects in CB tests between the clinical and control groups at week +6, using Mann–Whitney tests. In order to minimise the risk of Type II errors, this considered only the data relating to those probes which had been found to have been significantly different between groups at baseline. On the basis of these results, a similar assessment was made considering data from week +2 of treatment, in order to help identify the timescale of any change. After this, we compared within-groups over time, but for all ambiguous probes using Wilcoxon tests.

Only after these analyses were complete, was the consistency of the non-significant differences between groups at baseline assessed. In this way priority was given to the effect of treatment on those measures of most potential value first, reducing the risk of Type II errors from multiple testing.

The clinical assessment data were used to explore the convergent validity of possible inferences about affective state from the results of these CB tests. Specifically, changes in the relative frequency of SRP within eliciting contexts, separation anxiety global score, separation anxiety behaviour scores (the sum of individual problem behaviours shown: inappropriate urination, inappropriate defecation, destructive behaviour and excessive vocalisation) and attachment towards owner score at baseline versus weeks +2 and +6 were assessed for their significance (Wilcoxon signed rank test). Correlations between all measures were then explored using Kendall’s tau.

Results

Between-group analysis – clinical group and control group

Dogs in the clinical and control group did not show any statistical difference in their age, the duration of their ownership and their body weight (U = 16.5, Z = −0.163, p = 0.88; U = 11, Z = −1.061, p = 0.343, U = 13.5, Z = − 0.651, p = 0.53 respectively).

Initially, the clinical group’s baseline score to the NR- position was significantly slower than that of the control group U = 5, z = −2.03, p = 0.042, r = −0.59 (Table 4). Specifically, the clinical group’s score (Median = 68.9) tended to be relatively closer to the negative (R-) position (Median = 100), and the score of the control group (Median = 21.2) relatively closer to the positive (R+) position (Median = 0). There was no significant difference between the two groups in the baseline median speed towards the MID and NR+ probes (MID U = 11, z = −1.06, p = 0.291, r = −0.31, NR+ U = 9, z = −1.38, p = 0.167, r = −0.4 respectively (Table 4).

Table 4 Median adjusted speeds with fastest and slowest speeds in parentheses toward ambiguous probes at baseline and during treatment

The effect of treatment on response to just the NR- probe was therefore next compared between the two groups in accordance with the statistical analysis plan outlined above. At week 6 there was no significant difference between the treatment and control groups in their response to the NR-probe (U = 7, z = −1.71, p = 0.11, r = −0.49), and there was also no significant difference between the two groups at week 2 (U = 16, z = −0.24, p = 0.88, r = −0.07). Thus, the two groups started the study differing in their response to the NR- probe stimulus at baseline but this difference had disappeared by week +2 and continued to be absent in week +6 of treatment (Figure 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Adjusted speeds of the two groups towards the NR- position. (0 = Adjusted Speed towards Food Position and 100 = Adjusted speed towards Non-food position). * indicates a significant difference between the two groups at this time.

Within-group analysis - clinical group

The median score of the clinical group towards the MID position was significantly faster at week 6 than at baseline (Table 4, z = −2.023, p = 0.043, r = −0.9). The median response to the NR+ position was also significantly faster at week 6 compared to baseline (Table 4, z = = −2.023, p = 0.043, r = −0.9). However there was no significant change in response to the NR- position (z = −0.674, p = 0.5, r = −0.30) for the same comparison.

Comparisons between the baseline and week +2 did not show significant differences for any of the three ambiguous probes, although the response to the MID probe was borderline (NR+: z = −1.21, p = 0.23, r = −0.54; MID: z = −0.67, p = 0.05, r = −0.3; and NR-: z = −0,41 p = 0.69, r = −0.18). Thus, changes in response towards the MID and NR+ positions were only evident after six weeks of treatment, with no changes for the NR- probe.

Within-group analysis – control group

There was a significant effect of repeated CB testing on the Control group, with dogs moving significantly more slowly to all three ambiguous probes at both week +2 (MID: z = −2.197, p = 0.028, r = −0.83; NR+: z = −2.366, p = 0.018, r = 0.9; NR-: z = −2.366, p = 0.018, r = 0.9) and week +6 (MID: z = −2.366, p = 0.018, r = 0.9; NR+: z = −2.366, p = 0.018, r = 0.9; NR-: z = −2.197, p = 0.028, r = −0.83) compared to baseline (week 0) performance (Table 4).

Changes in clinical signs following treatment – clinical group

The four clinical measures of treatment efficacy showed a statistically significant improvement when comparing the two weeks preceding treatment (assessment at baseline) to the last two weeks of the treatment (assessment at week +8 of treatment). The proportion of eliciting contexts in which SRP were expressed decreased from a median of 0.9 to 0.14 (z = −2.032, p = 0.042, r = −0.9). The separation anxiety global score fell from a median of 3 (severe) to 0 (absent) (z = −2.041, p = 0.041, r = −0.91). The sum of individual behaviours (inappropriate urination, inappropriate defecation, destructive behaviour and excessive vocalisation) fell significantly from a median of 4 to 0 (z = −2.060, p = 0.039, r = − 0.921). Scores for attachment towards the owner fell from a median of 15 to 4 (z = −2.032, p = 0.042, r = −0.909).

Correlation between clinical measures – clinical group

The clinical measures of separation anxiety severity: frequency of the eliciting contexts; separation anxiety-related behaviour scores; separation anxiety global score; and attachment behaviour scores were all significantly correlated (p < 0.01 in all instances, Table 5).

Table 5 Correlations (Kendall’s tau) between clinical measures and associated statistical significance (2-tailed)

Correlation between CB and clinical measures – clinical group

Within the group receiving treatment, all clinical measures of SRP correlated significantly with the adjusted speed towards MID (recorded at baseline, week +2 and week +6), but none with the adjusted speed towards the NR- probe. Adjusted speed towards the NR+ probe correlated significantly with separation anxiety global score and attachment score, but not with eliciting context frequency and separation anxiety behaviour scores (Table 6).

Table 6 Correlations (Kendall’s tau test) across time between median speeds and clinical measures with associated significance (2-tailed)

Discussion

These results show, for the first time, that clinical treatment of a naturally occurring negative affective state and associated behaviours in a non-human species can produce a significant shift in cognitive bias as well as a large improvement in clinical behaviour measures as determined by statistical effect size. The difference evident at baseline in CB testing between treatment and control groups (i.e. response to NR- probe) disappeared by week 2 of treatment and remained absent after 6 weeks. It seems that in normal (i.e. Control) dogs that the running speed towards this probe reduces with repeat testing, whereas in the Clinical group, such a reduction in speed over time is not seen. We interpret this as an indication that the intervention on the Clinical group reduced the negative bias that develops towards this probe over time. This change together with the increased speed of Clinical group dogs to the two other probe locations across the study period meant that, in effect, the clinical population normalized with regards to the CB test over the course of the study. The four clinical measures used to assess the severity of SRP in this study showed convergent validity between themselves, and also demonstrated a statistically significant improvement with treatment implementation, reinforcing the findings from the CB tests on these subjects. The clinical group, not only showed signs of clinical improvement but also increased running speed to the MID and NR+ probes, which is consistent with the “optimistic” affective bias predicted for an antidepressant. However, we acknowledge that at present, it is not conclusive which aspect of the treatment plan, fluoxetine, the behaviour modification plan or the combination of the two, evoked the observed shift in cognitive bias. This question could be addressed in the future with the use of a double blind placebo controlled study using a similar protocol. This does not detract from the importance of the current study whose aim was to examine, as a critical first step, the association between the dogs’ behavioural improvement during treatment and possible changes in their underlying affective state, rather than make assumptions about this on the basis of superficial behavioural changes which could have an alternative interpretation and markedly different welfare implications. Subsequent studies will specify the impact of each individual component of the treatment plan.

A previous study which tested dogs housed at a rescue shelter found that dogs in a predictive test of SRP appeared to show a more ‘pessimistic’ cognitive bias towards the MID probe, but not towards the NR+ and NR- probes [18]. By contrast, in our study, before treatment implementation, dogs with clinically diagnosed SRP showed a ‘pessimistic’ bias to only the NR- probe compared to controls. Both studies agree that dogs with SRP appear to have a pessimistic cognitive bias, suggestive of a background negative affective state, and this adds to the evidence that SRP is a serious welfare concern that needs addressing. However, the difference between the studies in the particular probes which appear to reveal this effect (MID vs NR-) may reflect differences in either the population tested (clinical cases in the current study, versus shelter dogs undergoing a predictive test in the case of Mendl et al. [18]) or test environment (home versus shelter). It has been proposed that different types of affective state might result in differentiable performances towards specific probes, i.e. anxiety may result in changes largely towards the NR- probe while depression-like states would largely induce changes towards the NR+ probe [16,17]. In which case, our observed differences at the probe located closest to the unrewarded/negative reference location (NR-) at baseline suggest that the dogs in our clinical group experienced an increased sensitivity to aversion, a feature of many anxiety disorders. However, the response to treatment within the group was most marked towards the NR+ location and this is consistent with fluoxetine primarily being an antidepressant rather than tranquilizer.

The control dogs showed evidence of learning that the probes were unrewarded during repeat testing, as has been previously found in sheep [24]. As our control dogs did not have any obvious reason to change affective state during the study, and there were no notable changes in their environment according to their owner, it seems reasonable to suppose that the reduction in speed to the ambiguous probes reflects learning that these probes were unrewarded. In contrast, we observed that our clinical dogs increased their speed to the probes over time. An affective explanation for this result would be that the dogs become increasingly ‘optimistic’ during treatment. Nonetheless, it is important to consider alternative, non-affective explanations, and one obvious consideration relates to increased food motivation as a result of the medication. However, a side-effect of fluoxetine is to suppress appetite [20], and so the side effects of fluoxetine would, if at all, be expected to reduce running speed towards the probes (i.e. the opposite effect to what was found in this study), therefore the observed increase in running speed over time cannot obviously be accounted for by an increased motivation for food as a result of the medication.

In the clinical group, daily administration of fluoxetine in combination with a behaviour modification plan, significantly improved all clinical measures used for monitoring the dog’s behaviour, confirming other studies [19]. This improvement correlated (Table 6) with an increase in the speed with which clinical dogs ran towards the MID probe, suggesting that the behavioural improvement in the SRP was also associated with an improvement in the dog’s underlying affective state. Again, this finding is contrary to a potential alternative non-affective explanation, that the beneficial behavioural effects of serotonergic agents on problem behaviour in companion animals may be due simply to their broad inhibitory effects on behaviour [34]. If this were the case, then although clinical signs may improve (e.g. less separation related behaviours), response speed in the CB test would be expected to decrease rather than increase following administration of fluoxetine. It is worth noting that citalopram, another selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) agent has been shown to increase memory of positive materials (e.g. positive personality traits) in healthy human volunteers [35], and this positive effect on affective state seems the most parsimonious potential explanation for the effect seen. Serotonin is an important neurotransmitter that innervates the amygdala directly [36] and indirectly affects reward seeking behaviour [37]. Furthermore, fluoxetine, as a 5HT2C antagonist, also has disinhibitory effects on the release of norepinephrine and dopamine within the prefrontal cortex [36]. Thus, fluoxetine may functionally reorganize the mesolimbic dopamine system, to increase reward seeking and sensitivity [37].

Conclusion

Following treatment with a behaviour modification plan and fluoxetine, dogs with SRP not only improved their behaviour when alone but also showed an apparent improvement in the initial pessimistic affective state associated with the potential availability of rewards, at other times. Importantly, this is the first study to suggest that the clinical improvement of a spontaneously occurring behavioural problem is also associated with changes in affective state as determined by a CB test, and that SSRIs do indeed have an antidepressant effect on dogs.

References

  1. Pageat P. Pathologie du comportement du chien. In: Editions du Point Veterinaire. Maisons Alfort: Cedex; 1995. p. 288–93.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Voith VL, Borchelt PL. Separation anxiety in dogs. In: Voith VL, editor. Readings in Companion Animal Behaviour, Veterinary Learning Systems. Borchelt, Trenton: Veterinary Learning Systems; 1996. p. 124–39.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Overall KL. Clinical Behavioural Medicine for Small Animals. Mosby: St. Louis; 1997. p. 6–8.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Landsberg GM, Hunthausen WL, Ackerman L. Fear and Phobias, Separation Anxiety. In: Landsberg GM, Hunthausen WL, Ackerman L, editors. Handbook of behavior problems of the dog and cat. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, USA: Saunders Company; 2003. p. 258–67.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Guthrie A. Dogs behaving badly- canine separation disorder research. Vet Pract. 1999;31:12–3.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Blackwell EJ, Casey RA, Bradshaw JWS. Controlled trial of behavioural therapy for separation-related disorders in dogs. Vet Rec. 2006;158:551–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Bradshaw JWS, McPherson JA, Casey RA, Larter IS. Aetiology of separation-related behaviour in domestic dogs. Vet Rec. 2002;151:43–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Soares GM, Pereira JT, Paixão RL. Exploratory study of separation anxiety syndrome in apartment dogs. Cienc Rural. 2010;40(3):548–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. European Pet Food Industry Association. Facts and Figures 2010. [http://www.fediaf.org/press-area/press-releases/news-detail/artikel/fediaf-updated-2010-statistics/]

  10. US Pet Ownership. US Pet Ownership & Demographics Sourcebook. Schaumburg, IL, USA: American Veterinary Medical Association; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Appleby D, Pluijmakers J. Separation anxiety in dogs: The function of homeostasis in its development and treatment. Clin Tech Small Anim Pract. 2004;19:205–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Overall KL. Natural animal models of human psychiatric disorders: assessment of mechanism and validity. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2000;24:727–76.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Mills DS, Dube MB, Zulch H. Affective processes and the organization of behaviour. In: Mills DS, Dube MB, Zulch H, editors. Stress and pheromonatherapy in small animal clinical behaviour. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell; 2013. p. 37–68.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  14. Harding EJ, Paul ES, Mendl M. Animal behaviour – cognitive bias and affective state. Nature. 2004;427:312.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Paul ES, Harding EJ, Mendl M. Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:469–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Mendl M, Burman OHP, Parker RMA, Paul ES. Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;118:161–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Burman OHP, Parker R, Paul ES, Mendl M. A spatial judgement task to determine background emotional state in laboratory rats. Rattus norvegicus. Anim Behav. 2008;76:801–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Mendl M, Brooks J, Basse C, Burman O, Paul E, Blackwell E, et al. Dogs showing separation-related behaviour exhibit a ‘pessimistic’ cognitive bias. Curr Biol. 2010;20:R839–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Simpson BS, Landsberg GM, Reisner IR, Ciribassi JJ, Horwitz D, Houpt KA, et al. Effects of reconcile (fluoxetine) chewable tablets plus behavior management for canine separation anxiety. Vet Ther. 2007;8:18–31.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Landsberg GM, Melese P, Sherman BL, Neilson JC, Zimmerman A, Clarke TP. Effectiveness of fluoxetine chewable tablets in the treatment of canine separation anxiety. J Vet Behav. 2008;3:12–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. King JN, Simpson BS, Overall KL, Appleby D, Pageat P, Ross C, et al. Treatment of separation anxiety in dogs with clomipramine: results from a prospective, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group, multicenter clinical trial. App Anim Behav Sci. 2000;67(4):255–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Simpson B. Treatment of separation-related anxiety in dogs with clomipramine. Results from a multicentre, blinded, placebo-controlled clinical trial. In: Mills DS, Heath SE, Harrington LJ, editors. Proceedings of the First International Conference on Veterinary Behavioural Medicine. Birmingham, U.K: South Mimms, Universities Federation for Animal Welfare; 1997. p. 143–54.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Kostal L, Savory CJ. Behavioral responses of restricted-fed fowls to pharmacological manipulation of 5-HT and GABA receptor subtypes. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1996;53(4):995–1004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Doyle RE, Vidal S, Hinch GN, Fisher AD, Boissy A, Lee C. The effect of repeated testing on judgment biases in sheep. Behav Processes. 2010;83:349–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Bateson M, Matheson SM. Performance on a categorisation task suggests that removal of environmental enrichment induces ‘pessimism’ in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Anim Welfare. 2007;16(S):33–6.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Matheson SM, Asher L, Bateson M. Larger, enriched cages are associated with ‘optimistic’response biases in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). App Anim Behav Sci. 2008;109(2):374–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Burman OH, Parker R, Paul ES, Mendl MT. Anxiety-induced cognitive bias in non-human animals. Physiol Behav. 2009;98(3):345–50.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Brilot BO, Normandale CL, Parkin A, Bateson M. Can we use starlings’ aversion to eyespots as the basis for a novel ‘cognitive bias’ task? App Anim Behav Sci. 2009;118(3):182–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Doyle RE, Fisher AD, Hinch GN, Boissy A, Lee C. Release from restraint generates a positive judgement bias in sheep. App Anim Behav Sci. 2010;122(1):28–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Doyle RE, Lee C, Deiss V, Fisher AD, Hinch GN, Boissy A. Measuring judgement bias and emotional reactivity in sheep following long-term exposure to unpredictable and aversive events. Physiol Behav. 2011;102(5):503–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Sanger ME, Doyle RE, Hinch GN, Lee C. Sheep exhibit a positive judgement bias and stress-induced hyperthermia following shearing. App Anim Behav Sci. 2011;131(3):94–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Nakagawa S. A farewell to Bonferroni: the problems of low statistical power and publication bias. Behav Ecol. 2004;15(6):1044–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Science. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988. p. 77–81.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Mills DS. Medical paradigms for the study of problem behaviour: a critical review. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2003;81(3):265–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Harmer CJ, Shelley NC, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Increased positive versus negative affective perception and memory in healthy volunteers following selective serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibition. Am J Psychiat. 2004;161(7):1256–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Stahl SM. Antidepressants. In: Stahl SM, editor. Stahl’s essential psychopharmacology: Neuroscientific basis and practical applications. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2008. p. 511–666.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Panksepp J. Affective neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. p. 144–64.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the dogs and their owners for their time and their dedication. We would like to thank Elanco for providing us Fluoxetine (Reconcile™) free of charge after reviewing the protocol of the study, without any comments or restrictions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel S Mills.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests. The authors have no funding to report.

Authors’ contributions

Study conception and design: DM and CK, Acquisition of data: CK, Analysis and interpretation of data: CK, DM, OB, Drafting of manuscript: CK, Critical revision: CK, DM, OB. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Additional methodological information.

Rights and permissions

Open Access  This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.

The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

To view a copy of this licence, visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Karagiannis, C.I., Burman, O.H. & Mills, D.S. Dogs with separation-related problems show a “less pessimistic” cognitive bias during treatment with fluoxetine (Reconcile™) and a behaviour modification plan. BMC Vet Res 11, 80 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12917-015-0373-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12917-015-0373-1

Keywords