Skip to main content

Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome

Abstract

Background

Some patients awaken from coma (that is, open the eyes) but remain unresponsive (that is, only showing reflex movements without response to command). This syndrome has been coined vegetative state. We here present a new name for this challenging neurological condition: unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (abbreviated UWS).

Discussion

Many clinicians feel uncomfortable when referring to patients as vegetative. Indeed, to most of the lay public and media vegetative state has a pejorative connotation and seems inappropriately to refer to these patients as being vegetable-like. Some political and religious groups have hence felt the need to emphasize these vulnerable patients' rights as human beings. Moreover, since its first description over 35 years ago, an increasing number of functional neuroimaging and cognitive evoked potential studies have shown that physicians should be cautious to make strong claims about awareness in some patients without behavioral responses to command. Given these concerns regarding the negative associations intrinsic to the term vegetative state as well as the diagnostic errors and their potential effect on the treatment and care for these patients (who sometimes never recover behavioral signs of consciousness but often recover to what was recently coined a minimally conscious state) we here propose to replace the name.

Conclusion

Since after 35 years the medical community has been unsuccessful in changing the pejorative image associated with the words vegetative state, we think it would be better to change the term itself. We here offer physicians the possibility to refer to this condition as unresponsive wakefulness syndrome or UWS. As this neutral descriptive term indicates, it refers to patients showing a number of clinical signs (hence syndrome) of unresponsiveness (that is, without response to commands) in the presence of wakefulness (that is, eye opening).

Peer Review reports

Background

We here present a new name (unresponsive wakefulness syndrome or UWS) for an over 35-year-old syndrome with an unintended albeit persistent negative connotation: the vegetative state. The widespread use of intensive care medicine and artificial ventilation to sustain respiration and circulation has increased survival from coma. It has also led to an increasing number of patients who have awakened from coma (that is, showed eye opening, incompatible with the diagnosis of coma) yet remain unresponsive (that is, only showed reflex movements as is also the case for coma) [1]. In Europe, this clinical syndrome was initially termed apallic syndrome [2] and coma vigil [3] but it is currently known in the medical community as persistent vegetative state (PVS), a term first coined by Jennet and Plum in 1972 in their milestone Lancet paper [4]. The name vegetative state was chosen to refer to the preserved vegetative nervous functioning, meaning these patients have (variably) preserved sleep-wake cycles, respiration, digestion or thermoregulation. The term persistent was added to denote that the condition remained for at least one month after insult. In 1994, the Multi-Society Task Force on PVS defined the temporal criteria for irreversibility (that is, more than one year for traumatic and three months for non-traumatic (anoxic) etiology) and introduced the notion of permanent vegetative state [5]. It is to these latter cases that ethical and legal end-of-life issues, of withholding and withdrawal of life sustaining treatment (that is, artificial hydration and nutrition), are related [6, 7].

Over the last three decades, a growing number of physicians and healthcare workers have felt uncomfortable when referring to patients as vegetative [810], resulting in a number of papers reiterating the intellectual justification of the origins and choice of the term [11]. The conception of a vegetative nervous system goes back to 1800 when Bichat divided the nervous system into animalic and vegetative [12]. The former linked the person to her or his environment and was expressed by the muscles of voluntary locomotion and the organs of external senses. The latter identified the nutritional functions of the body. According to the Oxford English dictionary, 'to vegetate' is to 'live a merely physical life devoid of intellectual activity or social intercourse' and 'vegetative' describes 'an organic body capable of growth and development but devoid of sensation and thought'. To part, if not most, of the lay public and media, however, it has a rather pejorative undertone and seems (incorrectly) to refer to patients as being vegetable-like (for example, an internet search with the terms vegetative state and vegetable returned 26,700 hits, état végétatif and plante 19,600; stato vegetativo and vegetale 49,100 (Google search performed 8 April 2010). Many authors and social, political and religious groups have hence felt the need to emphasize these patients' clearly evident rights to be fully regarded as human beings [13, 14].

In addition to this malaise regarding the chosen term and its unintended denigrating connotation, some feel that referring to these patients as being in a state may (incorrectly) denote chronicity. Despite the fact that the clinical criteria of the vegetative state do not imply a temporal dimension, referring only to a clinical tableau reflecting wakeful unawareness [4], for many physicians and healthcare workers it has the negative connotation of a being a longstanding and nearly irreversible condition. The introduction of the term persistent vegetative state (too often confounded with permanent vegetative state with which it unfortunately shares the same abbreviation PVS), may have contributed to this [15]. In contrast to coma (which is an acute and transitory condition, lasting no more than days or weeks), a vegetative state may become chronic (lasting for decades) or may remain a transitory condition on the way to further recovery [16]. This recently led the Aspen Neurobehavioral Conference Workgroup to characterize a new clinical entity coined the 'minimally conscious state' (MCS), describing patients who have recovered from a vegetative state (meaning they show more than reflex motor behavior but fail to show functional communication or object use) [17]. Despite clear evidence that vegetative patients are not uniformly hopeless [18, 19], once stamped with the diagnosis VS, clinical practice shows it often is difficult to change the label, and the first signs of recovery of consciousness are too often missed. Previous studies by Childs et al. in Texas [20] and Andrews et al. in London [21] have estimated misdiagnosis of chronic patients referred to rehabilitation centers to be at around 40%. It has been argued that these older studies, performed prior to the publication of the Multi-Society Task Force on PVS criteria [5] of VS, and long before the criteria of the MCS [17], were overly pessimistic. A very recent study, however, confirmed this unacceptably high rate of diagnostic error [22]. A number of highly publicised patients also illustrate this point. Julia Tavalaro survived a brain trauma and was transferred to a tertiary care centre where she was called "the vegetable" for over six years, although she was conscious and sensate. She later wrote her memoirs in Look Up for Yes [23]. Terry Wallis, who was considered to be in a VS, made the headlines when he started to speak 19 years after his car accident. Careful analysis of his medical records quickly showed he actually recovered to a MCS within the first year after his brain trauma [24]. Finally, since the term VS was coined in 1972, an increasing number of functional neuroimaging and event related potential (ERP) studies have shown that physicians should be very careful about making strong claims about patients' awareness [2531]. This situation is further complicated when patients with such disorders of consciousness have underlying deficits in the domain of verbal or non-verbal communication functions, such as aphasia, agnosia or apraxia [32, 33].

Discussion

Given these concerns regarding the negative connotation inherent in the term vegetative state and its possible effect on vulnerable patients awakening from coma, who sometimes never recover any voluntary responsiveness but may (probably more often than initially believed) recover minimal signs of consciousness, we here propose to change the label vegetative state, thus hoping to make it easier to change their management and standards of care. The European Task Force on Disorders of Consciousness has passed a proposal to change the name to unresponsive wakefulness syndrome or UWS. If after 35 years the medical community has been unsuccessful in changing the pejorative image associated with the words vegetative state, we propose that it might be better to change the term itself. From now on, physicians can choose this neutral descriptive term to refer to patients who, as the name indicates, show a number of clinical signs (hence the use of syndrome) of unresponsiveness (meaning they fail to show non-reflex behavior or command following) in the presence of wakefulness (meaning they open their eyes spontaneously or upon stimulation). Given the above mentioned difficulty in making strong general claims about awareness in severely brain damaged patients, we have chosen here to use the clinically descriptive term unresponsive rather than the misleading unaware. After discussion, other (existing) alternatives [34] were rejected. Coma vigil was discarded because the term is a contradiction in terminis, given that coma patients by definition never open their eyes. Apallic syndrome was also rejected, as recent evidence has shown that these patients are not a-pallic (meaning without a cortex or pallium) [35], but classically show preserved albeit disconnected islands of residual (merely primary) cortical functioning [36].

Next, we stress the need for prospective studies on prognosis [18, 37, 38] and treatment [39, 40] in large, well-described cohorts of patients with disorders of consciousness, permitting evidence-based decision-making while respecting individual divergence in the challenging issues related to end-of-life decisions [6, 7]. Such studies will need standardized behavioral assessment and outcome scales [41]. The worldwide acceptance of the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) [42] has standardized patient assessment in the ICU and allowed proper research to be carried out in the field of coma. However, the GCS was not intended to be used on patients with post-comatose disorders of consciousness, such as UWS and MCS. Other standardized scales will need to be employed in these cases [43, 44]. We also need reliable objective para-clinical markers confirming our clinical signs of motor unresponsiveness and behavior indicative of the absence of awareness of environment and self [45]. Studies assessing the efficacy of treatment of patients with disorders of consciousness should be separated into symptomatic and curative and should take into account not only patient age, etiology and time since insult, but also the need to clearly separate UWS from MCS [46].

Conclusion

In conclusion, our proposal offers the medical community the possibility to adopt a neutral and descriptive name, unresponsive wakefulness syndrome, as an alternative to vegetative state (or apallic syndrome) which we view as outdated. We feel this is a real necessity, given that the term PVS continues to have strong negative connotations after over 35 years of use, while inadvertently risking comparisons between patients and vegetables and implying persistency from the moment of diagnosis. It should be stressed that UWS is a clinical syndrome describing patients who fail to show voluntary motor responsiveness in the presence of eyes-open wakefulness which can be either transitory on the way to recovery from (minimal) consciousness or irreversible.

Abbreviations

ERP:

event related potential

GCS:

Glasgow Coma Scale

MCS:

minimally conscious state

PVS:

persistent vegetative state

UWS:

unresponsive wakefulness syndrome

VS:

vegetative state

References

  1. Monti MM, Laureys S, Owen AM: The vegetative state. BMJ. 2010, 341: 292-296. 10.1136/bmj.c3765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Kretschmer E: Das apallische Syndrom. Z ges Neurol Psychiat. 1940, 169: 576-579. 10.1007/BF02871384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Calvet J, Coll J: Meningitis of sinusoid origin with the form of coma vigil. Rev Otoneuroophtalmol. 1959, 31: 443-445.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Jennett B, Plum F: Persistent vegetative state after brain damage. A syndrome in search of a name. Lancet. 1972, 1: 734-737. 10.1016/S0140-6736(72)90242-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. The Multi-Society Task Force on PVS: Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state (1). N Engl J Med. 1994, 330: 1499-1508. 10.1056/NEJM199405263302107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Jennett B: The vegetative state. Medical facts, ethical and legal dilemmas. 2002, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  7. Celesia GG: Persistent vegetative state: clinical and ethical issues. Suppl Clin Neurophysiol. 2000, 53: 460-462. full_text.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Shewmon DA: A critical analysis of conceptual domains of the vegetative state: sorting fact from fancy. NeuroRehabilitation. 2004, 19: 343-347.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Kotchoubey B: Apallic syndrome is not apallic: Is vegetative state vegetative?. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2005, 15: 333-356. 10.1080/09602010443000416.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Schoenle PW, Witzke W: How vegetative is the vegetative state? Preserved semantic processing in VS patients--evidence from N 400 event-related potentials. NeuroRehabilitation. 2004, 19: 329-334.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Jennett B: Thirty years of the vegetative state: clinical, ethical and legal problems. Prog Brain Res. 2005, 150: 537-543. full_text.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Bichat M-F-X: Recherches physiologiques sur la vie et la mort. 1800, Paris: Brosson Gabon

    Google Scholar 

  13. Borthwick C: The proof of the vegetable: a commentary on medical futility. J Med Ethics. 1995, 21: 205-208. 10.1136/jme.21.4.205.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Velez GJ: Death of John Paul II and the basic human care for the sick and the dying. Ethics Med. 2005, 21: 167-177.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Laureys S: Permanent vegetative state and persistent vegetative state are not interchangeable terms. British Medical Journal. 2000, 16 Oct 2000(9 Sept 2005)., [http://bmj.com/cgi/eletters/321/7266/916#10276]

    Google Scholar 

  16. Lavrijsen JC, van den Bosch JS, Koopmans RT, van Weel C: Prevalence and characteristics of patients in a vegetative state in Dutch nursing homes. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005, 76: 1420-1424. 10.1136/jnnp.2004.058198.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, Kelly JP, Rosenberg JH, Whyte J, Zafonte RD, Zasler ND: The minimally conscious state: Definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 2002, 58: 349-353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Dolce G, Quintieri M, Serra S, Lagani V, Pignolo L: Clinical signs and early prognosis in vegetative state: a decisional tree, data-mining study. Brain Inj. 2008, 22: 617-623. 10.1080/02699050802132503.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Dolce G, Sazbon L: The Post-traumatic Vegetative State. 2002, New York: Thieme

    Book  Google Scholar 

  20. Childs NL, Mercer WN, Childs HW: Accuracy of diagnosis of persistent vegetative state. Neurology. 1993, 43: 1465-1467.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Andrews K, Murphy L, Munday R, Littlewood C: Misdiagnosis of the vegetative state: retrospective study in a rehabilitation unit. BMJ. 1996, 313: 13-16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Schnakers C, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Giacino J, Ventura M, Boly M, Majerus S, Moonen G, Laureys S: Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. BMC Neurol. 2009, 9: 35-10.1186/1471-2377-9-35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Tavalaro J, Tayson R: Look Up for Yes. 1997, New York, NY: Kodansha America, Inc

    Google Scholar 

  24. Voss HU, Uluc AM, Dyke JP, Watts R, Kobylarz EJ, McCandliss BD, Heier LA, Beattie BJ, Hamacher KA, Vallabhajosula S, Goldsmith SJ, Ballon D, Giacino JT, Schiff ND: Possible axonal regrowth in late recovery from the minimally conscious state. J Clin Invest. 2006, 116: 2005-2011. 10.1172/JCI27021.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Owen AM, Coleman MR, Boly M, Davis MH, Laureys S, Pickard JD: Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science. 2006, 313: 1402-10.1126/science.1130197.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Laureys S, Boly M: The changing spectrum of coma. Nat Clin Pract Neurol. 2008, 4: 544-546. 10.1038/ncpneuro0887.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Monti MM, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Coleman MR, Boly M, Pickard JD, Tshibanda L, Owen AM, Laureys S: Willful Modulation of Brain Activity in Disorders of Consciousness. N Engl J Med. 2010, 362: 579-89. 10.1056/NEJMoa0905370.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Zeman A: Persistent vegetative state. Lancet. 1997, 350: 795-799. 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)06447-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Machado C, Korein J, Aubert E, Bosch J, Alvarez MA, Rodriguez R, Valdes P, Portela L, Garcia M, Perez N, Chinchilla M, Machado Y, Machado Y: Recognizing a mother's voice in the persistent vegetative state. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2007, 38: 124-126.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Coleman MR, Rodd JM, Davis MH, Johnsrude IS, Menon DK, Pickard JD, Owen AM: Do vegetative patients retain aspects of language comprehension? Evidence from fMRI. Brain. 2007, 130: 2494-2507. 10.1093/brain/awm170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Bekinschtein TA, Shalom DE, Forcato C, Herrera M, Coleman MR, Manes FF, Sigman M: Classical conditioning in the vegetative and minimally conscious state. Nat Neurosci. 2009, 12: 1343-1349. 10.1038/nn.2391.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Majerus S, Bruno MA, Schnakers C, Giacino JT, Laureys S: The problem of aphasia in the assessment of consciousness in brain-damaged patients. Prog Brain Res. 2009, 177: 49-61. full_text.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Bruno M-A, Fernández-Espejo D, Lehembre R, Tshibanda L, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Gosseries O, Lommers E, Noirhomme Q, Boly M, Napolitani M, et al: Multi-modal imaging in patients with disorders of consciousness showing "functional hemispherectomy". Prog Brain Res. 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Sazbon L, Groswasser Z: Prolonged coma, vegetative state, post-comatose unawareness: semantics or better understanding?. Brain Inj. 1991, 5: 1-2. 10.3109/02699059108998504.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. von Wild K, Gerstenbrand F, Dolce G, AS/VS' Esgo: Guidelines for quality management of Apallic Syndrome/Vegetative State. Eur J Trauma Surg. 2007, 33: 268-292. 10.1007/s00068-007-6138-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND: Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. Lancet Neurol. 2004, 3: 537-546. 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00852-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Kampfl A, Schmutzhard E, Franz G, Pfausler B, Haring HP, Ulmer H, Felber S, Golaszewski S, Aichner F: Prediction of recovery from post-traumatic vegetative state with cerebral magnetic-resonance imaging. Lancet. 1998, 351: 1763-1767. 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)10301-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Eilander HJ, Wijnen VJ, Scheirs JG, de Kort PL, Prevo AJ: Children and young adults in a prolonged unconscious state due to severe brain injury: outcome after an early intensive neurorehabilitation programme. Brain Inj. 2005, 19: 425-436. 10.1080/02699050400025299.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Cohadon F, Richer E: Deep cerebral stimulation in patients with post-traumatic vegetative state. 25 cases. Neurochirurgie. 1993, 39: 281-292.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. von Wild KR: Posttraumatic rehabilitation and one year outcome following acute traumatic brain injury (TBI): data from the well defined population based German Prospective Study 2000-2002. Acta Neurochir Suppl. 2008, 101: 55-60. full_text.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Pignolo L, Quintieri M, Sannita WG: The Glasgow outcome scale in vegetative state: a possible source of bias. Brain Inj. 2009, 23: 1-2. 10.1080/02699050802595873.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Teasdale G, Jennett B: Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. Lancet. 1974, 2: 81-84. 10.1016/S0140-6736(74)91639-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Majerus S, Gill-Thwaites H, Andrews K, Laureys S: Behavioral evaluation of consciousness in severe brain damage. Prog Brain Res. 2005, 150: 397-413. full_text.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Eilander HJ, van de Wiel M, Wijers M, van Heugten CM, Buljevac D, Lavrijsen JC, Hoenderdaal PL, de Letter-van der Heide L, Wijnen VJ, Scheirs JG, de Kort PL, Prevo AJ: The reliability and validity of the PALOC-s: a post-acute level of consciousness scale for assessment of young patients with prolonged disturbed consciousness after brain injury. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2009, 19: 1-27. 10.1080/09602010701694822.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Laureys S: Death, unconsciousness and the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005, 6: 899-909. 10.1038/nrn1789.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Leon-Carrion J, Martin-Rodriguez JF, Damas-Lopez J, Barroso y Martin JM, Dominguez-Morales MR: Brain function in the minimally conscious state: a quantitative neurophysiological study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008, 119: 1506-1514. 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.03.030.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

SL is funded by the Belgian National Funds for Scientific Research. This debate paper originated from the European Task Force on Disorders of Consciousness, founded by G. Dolce, 18 September 2009 meeting in Rome at the Italian Ministry of Health, Work and Welfare, funded by the S. Anna Institute, Crotone, Italy. The following participants attended the meeting: H. Binder (Austria), GG Celesia (USA, chairman), F. Cohadon (France), G. Dolce (Italy; organiser), R. Elefante (Italy), A. Granata (Italy), M. Quintieri (Italy), L. Lucca (Italy), G. Gigli (Italy), M. Koler (Germany), S. Laureys (Belgium, chairman), J. Leon-Carrion (Spain), A. Morresi (Italy), G. Pugliesi (Italy), P. Pugliese (Italy), W.G. Sannita (Italy & USA), L. Sazbon (Israel), E. Schmutzhard (Austria), A. Soddu (Belgium), K. von Wild (Germany). A selection of the participants, together with delegates from the United Kingdom (A. Zeman) and the Netherlands (J. Lavrijsen) participated in the writing of this consensus paper, aiming for a balanced representation in terms of geography and professional background (that is, neurology, neurosurgery, intensive care, rehabilitation, chronic and nursing home care, psychology and neuroscience). All delegates have previously participated in the development of discipline-specific position statements on disorders of consciousness or have made substantial contributions to the peer-reviewed literature.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Steven Laureys.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

All authors contributed to the content of this manuscript and have read and approved the final draft.

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Laureys, S., Celesia, G.G., Cohadon, F. et al. Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med 8, 68 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-8-68

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-8-68

Keywords