Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Utilities of the Post-anesthesia State derived by the Standard Gamble method in surgical patients

Abstract

Background

There are no published utilities for the post-anesthesia state obtained by the standard gamble method (SG).

Methods

We obtained utilities for postoperative pain, nausea, vomiting, urinary retention and myalgia from 100 adults prior to elective surgery using SG.

Results

20% of volunteer participants could not demonstrate a satisfactory understanding of the SG process. Median utilities for each adverse effect were all very close to 1.0, and no statistically significant differences were found between them.

Conclusion

Our results suggest that the avoidance of anesthesia related side effects and pain is not viewed by patients prior to surgery as being worthy of the taking of even a miniscule risk of death. This may affect the decision to utilize anesthesia techniques that trade a lower incidence of common side effects for a very low but finite risk of a catastrophic complication.

Peer Review reports

Background

Anesthesia can be given in a wide variety of different ways. Many operations can either be performed under general anesthesia, regional anesthesia (targeted blockade with local anesthetic of the nerves supplying the area to be operated on) or local anesthesia. Even within these broad classifications, a number of drugs, techniques and routes of administration may be used alone or together in a large number of potential combinations. Anesthesiologists tailor each anesthetic to the specific requirements of the case by considering factors such as the magnitude and duration of the procedure, the severity of co-morbidities exhibited by the patient, the anticipated degree of postoperative pain and nausea and the desirability of same-day home discharge, among other things. Advances in drugs, equipment and training have combined to make anesthesia a very safe process, and most anesthesia research is now focused on reducing the impact of transient adverse effects, rather than on reducing the incidence of serious injury and death (Chung & Mezei 1999). Nonetheless, the rates of catastrophic complication related to anesthesia have not been reduced to zero (Kaufman et al 2000).

Each anesthetic drug or technique choice carries its own benefits and risks and there is currently no quantitative method for distinguishing between them. The selection of anesthetic technique would therefore seem to an appropriate area for the application of decision analysis. Decision support tools might, for example, help decide if the reduced risk of perioperative pain and vomiting afforded by regional anesthesia over general anesthesia is justified in the face of the small attendant risk of permanent neurologic injury. The construction of such decision aids would require estimates of both the probability and the utility of each adverse outcome under consideration.

While relatively good estimates are available for the probability of common and rare adverse anesthesia effects, there are no formally derived utility estimates. Instead, researchers have used other methods to enable subjects to rank and quantify the degree to which they would choose to avoid them. For example, our group recently performed a study of the 'willingness to pay' type, (Rashiq & Bray 2003), in which pre-surgical patients were asked to allocate 100 theoretical dollars to each of ten post-operative states in proportion to the importance they attached to them. Pain was ranked most unpleasant by 49% of participants, and they allocated a median of $25 to avoid it. Avoiding vomiting was ranked most unpleasant by 24% of participants, with a median of $20 allocated to avoiding it.

The difficulty with this method of preference derivation is that it does not incorporate consideration of risk. Utilities, which are derived in the context of risk, are felt to better reflect subjects' true feelings since, in real life, treatment decisions are only made after attendant risks have been considered. Utilities may be derived by more than one method. However, the only available utility values for anesthesia-related effects were not derived empirically but assigned by a panel of experts. Using this method, post-operative nausea and vomiting was assigned a utility of 0.72, a figure that we thought to be implausibly low. (Reeves et al 2001). The Standard Gamble (SG) is felt by many to be the gold standard method of utility derivation, but SG has not hitherto been applied to anesthesia side effects.

The primary objective of this study was to obtain utilities for five common short-term complications of anesthesia and surgery using the standard gamble (SG) method in 100 patients presenting for elective surgery at our institution. A second objective was to compare the age, gender and educational level of subjects who could not successfully complete the SG interview with those who did so. Finally, we wished to compare the rank order of these effects for the group as a whole with that obtained by our previous willingness to pay study.

Methods

The University of Alberta Hospital is a tertiary referral centre in Western Canada with a surgical practice profile that is skewed towards complex procedures and patients with a high degree of concomitant medical illness. Almost all patients who present for elective surgery are evaluated in a Preadmission Clinic at least one day prior to admission.

Having obtained the approval of the Health Research Ethics Board of the University of Alberta, a single, trained interviewer invited a random sample of each day's Preadmission Clinic attendees to participate in the study. The only inclusion criteria were that candidate subjects be adults (18–65 years) who were being pre-admitted for elective surgery. Subjects who were cognitively or visually impaired, not fluent in written and spoken English, or too anxious about forthcoming events to participate were excluded.

Informed written consent was obtained from each participant. Interviews were conducted in a quiet room. Subject age, gender, proposed surgical procedure and highest completed educational level were recorded. A standardized explanation of the SG lottery was given using a chart designed by the investigators. This chart consisted of a line diagram of an individual walking down a path which forks two ways, with an illustration of the consequences of going down each path (figure).

The process began with the description of two hypothetical disease states. The first was intended to represent an incurable, severely physically and socially debilitating condition and the second a trivial illness with little symptom burden that could be easily controlled (Table 1, first two rows). Subjects completed SG lotteries for each of these using certain death probabilities ranging from 1 in 1,000,000,000 to 1 in 10. We began with the lowest risk of death and worked sequentially upwards, unless the subject elected to jump ahead without prompting. Every effort was made to ensure that subjects understood the process but the interviewer was careful not to coach the subject towards or away from any particular decision. The diagram was used for each lottery and while the subject was considering his or her decision, the death probability in play was kept in view. Subjects who could not complete the lottery, those who gave the same utility to both health states, and those who rated the severe illness as having a higher utility than the trivial one were deemed to have an incomplete understanding of the process. Their data were excluded from further analysis. We also excluded anyone who completed this stage but who did not give an indifference probability in at least one of the five lotteries in the subsequent phase of the study.

Table 1 Descriptions of the hypothetical illnesses and postoperative adverse effects used

Subjects who successfully completed the lottery for the two theoretical illnesses participated in the rest of the study. Recruitment continued until 100 such subjects had been collected. In this phase, short descriptions of five common adverse effects that might be experienced following anesthesia and surgery were read to the patient and also given to them in writing. These descriptions were derived for our previous work by a focus group of anesthesia providers (Table 1) Subjects then participated in individual SG lotteries for each effect. Death probability iterations were varied in the same way as before but it was made clear to subjects that they could choose any numerical probability they wished to decide their true point of indifference. The probability of death at the point of indifference was subtracted from 1.0 to yield the utility of the state. For example, if a subject was indifferent to the choice between the adverse anesthesia effect as described and a lottery with a 1% risk of death and a 99% chance of avoiding the effect, the derived utility would be 0.99. If even the smallest risk of death was not acceptable to the subject for a given adverse effect, we assigned a utility of 1.0 for the state.

Data was recorded by hand for subsequent transfer to a Microsoft Excel spreadsheet. All data sheets were checked twice for coding errors. Statistical analysis was performed using SAS version 8 for Windows. An analysis of variance (performed on a reciprocal transformation) was used to seek differences between the utilities for the various states. Chi-square tests were used to compare the gender and educational level of those who completed the study vs. those who were excluded, and a t-test was used to compare the ages of these two groups. Statistical significance for test probabilities was pre-determined to be 0.05.

Results

  1. 121

    surgical patients agreed to participate in the study. Of these, 21 were excluded, either because they did not give an appropriate response to the screening lotteries (16) or were unable to complete the protocol for some other reason (5). Those excluded were not different in age, gender or educational level from those that went on to complete the study. Thus, 100 subjects completed the study by providing indifference probabilities for some or all the adverse effects under investigation. Demographic and other characteristics of this group are given in Table 2.

Table 2 Demographic characteristics of the subject group

Five adverse effect-specific lotteries were attempted for each subject for a total of 500 lotteries. In 117 of these (23%), the subject was unwilling to take any risk of death, even when explicitly asked to choose a value smaller than 1: 100 000 000. We assigned a utility of 1.0 in these situations. There was no difference in age, gender or educational level between those who provided a non-zero risk in all 5 lotteries versus those who did so in four or fewer.

The distribution of utilities is given in Table 3. There was no statistically significant difference between them. As expected, the highest utility was given to the trivial hypothetical illness, and the debilitating hypothetical illness was given the least utility. The adverse effect with the lowest utility was pain in 35 subjects, vomiting in 30, urinary retention in 28, nausea in 4 and myalgia in 3. There was no difference between the genders for any of the individual utility estimates.

Table 3 Derived utilities for each state

Discussion

We have elicited utilities for five adverse effect states that are commonly found after anesthesia and surgery. This is the first time, to our knowledge, that the disutility of perioperative morbidity has been measured directly using the SG method.

The SG method has been widely used to measure individual preferences in conditions of uncertainty (Gafni, 1994). The utilities thus derived take potential health states and quality-adjusted years of life following medical procedures into account (Farquhar, 1984. This method has been widely used in health care practice for decades (Torrance, 1986), providing a valuable index that is useful in health care decision making processes.

Our first finding was that almost 20% of those who agreed to participate in our study could not satisfactorily complete the screening test. We took this to mean that they did not fully understand the SG exercise. An alternative interpretation is that they really did think that the two hypothetical disease states would be equally burdensome, or our 'trivial' hypothetical illness was worse than the 'debilitating'. We think this interpretation implausible, but if it were true, we would have unwittingly selected out respondents who characterize illness burden in this highly unconventional way. We did not find any differences in age, gender or educational level between those who went on to complete the study and those who did not and must therefore conclude that another factor or factors that we did not measure is responsible for this difference. Approximately half of our sample had at least some post-secondary education, which is broadly in line with the Canadian population as a whole.

The abstract and cognitively demanding nature of the SG has been noted by others (Gold et al 1996), and has been cited as a threat to the validity of its results. An alternative viewpoint is that if it is carefully explained and illustrated visually, as we did, the SG is a feasible method of obtaining utilities in both sick and healthy people, and that the additional inconveniences it imposes on interviewer and respondent are outweighed by its superior theoretical basis (Torrance 1986). We selected the SG for its theoretical benefits alone.

For almost all subjects, the disutility of the adverse effects was very low. They placed little value in absolute terms in avoiding them. The median utility for postoperative vomiting, for instance, was 0.999900. This calls into question the assumption that was made in a previous decision analysis of anesthesia for cataract surgery, in which the utility of postoperative nausea and vomiting was estimated by an expert panel of health care providers to be 0.72 (Reeves et al 2001). Our data are more consistent with the results of a large-scale community based survey of the general public in Canada (Mittmann et al 1999), which gave average utilities for common chronic health conditions on a 0–1 scale. In that study, disease-specific utilities ranged from 0.92 for acne to 0.58 for Alzheimer's disease. In that context, the allocation by our subjects of much greater utilities to temporary anesthesia and surgery-related side-effects, even if severe and debilitating makes sense.

A factor that may have contributed to these results is the SG itself: individuals have been found to attach a higher preference to avoiding a loss than to making a gain, even if the loss and gain are of the same magnitude (Tversky and Kahneman, 1981). While more recent theories of decision-making processes offer more complex models to explain these processes (e.g., Busemeyer et al., 2005), loss aversion remains an evidence-based theory (Usher and McClelland, 2004). Utilities derived by SG therefore likely reflect this innate risk-aversion, resulting in higher scores than other methods. This is exemplified by a study in which the simultaneous use of SG and the Rating Scale to derive utilities in chronic musculoskeletal pain yielded reproducibly higher utilities by SG (Goosens et al 1999). We may have obtained higher utilities by our interactive method than would have been obtained by an alternative method such as a self-completed questionnaire (Hammerschmidt et al 2004). We offered risks in sequential order from lowest to highest, rather than a 'ping-pong' approach (offering a very low risk, then a very high one and going back and forth to find the point of indifference). We did this because we thought it would be less overwhelming for our subjects. However, if the subject ended the lottery prematurely because (s)he was tired or uncertain, our method would have biased the result towards higher utilities.

We were unable to demonstrate statistically significant differences between the utilities for any of the states we described. This may in part reflect a ceiling effect. While the rank order of the utility for the adverse effects is not completely consistent with our previous work in the area (Rashiq and Bray 2003), pain was the state to which participants attached the lowest utility in both studies, and they did so in the same proportions to that which was found previously by others (Gan et al 2001, Macario et al 1999) using different methods of preference derivation.

Our use of death as the anchor for the low end of the utility scale may be questioned. The use of an alternative reference state would likely have yielded lower utilities (Torrance 1986). However, recognized adverse effects of commonly performed anesthesia procedures include not only transient phenomena such as nausea and pain but very rare catastrophes. Peripheral nerve block, performed thousands of times daily in operating rooms worldwide is documented to have caused nerve damage leading to lifelong and severe disability (Kaufman 1999). The incidence of permanent paraplegia following regional anesthesia is estimated to be of the order of 1 in 10 000 (Sage 1999). Most anesthesiologists performing regional anesthesia will therefore never be associated with such a case in their entire careers and many feel that the benefits of regional anesthesia (less pain, nausea, and a more rapid convalescence, for example) outweigh its risk in many circumstances. However, if a decision analysis were to be used to decide whether regional or general anesthesia should be used to avoid postoperative nausea, using a published utility for paraplegia of 0.329 (Ustun 1999) and our utility for postoperative nausea of 0.999900, we think that the rationale for this choice would be less clear.

Conclusion

We conclude that it is possible to obtain utilities for the post-anesthesia state by the SG method albeit that a significantly large subgroup (23%) of people asked will not be able to give meaningful responses. We did not demonstrate any differences in age, gender or education between those who completed the study and those who did not. The rank order of utilities for the adverse effects was somewhat different from that which we obtained by the willingness-to-pay methodology in a previous study, but pain and vomiting were still the effects rated as least desirable.

We find the utility of common post-anesthesia side effects to be very high, and suggest that this finding would not support the use of anesthesia techniques that carry even a very small risk of catastrophic complication in decision analysis models, even if those techniques effectively reduce common side-effects.

References

  1. Busemeyer JR, Townsend JT, Diederich A, Barkan R: Contrast effects or loss aversion?. Psychol Rev. 2005, 112 (1): 253-255. 10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Chung F, Mezei G: Adverse outcomes in ambulatory anesthesia. Canadian Journal of Anaesthesia. 1999, 46 (5 Pt 2): R18-R34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Farquhar P: State of the art: Utility assessment methods. Management Science. 1984, 30 (11): 1283-1300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Gafni A: The standard gamble method: what is being measured and how it is interpreted. Health Serv Res. 1994, 29 (2): 207-224.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Gan T, Sloan F, Dear GL, El Moalem HE, Lubarsky DA: How much are patients willing to pay to avoid postoperative nausea and vomiting?. Anesthesia & Analgesia. 2001, 92 (2): 393-400. 10.1097/00000539-200102000-00022.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Gold MR, Patrick DL, Torrance GWF: Identifying and Valuing outcomes. Cost-Effectiveness in Health and Medicine. Edited by: Gold MR, Seigel JE, Russel LB, Weinstein MC. 1996, New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  7. Goossens ME, Vlaeyen JW, Rutten-van Molken MP, van der Linden SM: Patient utilities in chronic musculoskeletal pain: how useful is the standard gamble method?. Pain. 1999, 80 (1–2): 365-375. 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00232-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Hammerschmidt T, Zeitler HP, Gulich M, Leidl R: A comparison of different strategies to collect standard gamble utilities. Med Decis Making. 2004, 24 (5): 493-503. 10.1177/0272989X04269239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Kaufman BR, Nystrom E, Nath S, Foucher G, Nystrom A: Debilitating chronic pain syndromes after presumed intraneural injections. Pain. 2000, 85 (1–2): 283-286. 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00243-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Macario A, Weinger M, Carney S, Kim A: Which clinical anesthesia outcomes are important to avoid?. The perspective of patients. Anesthesia & Analgesia. 1999, 89 (3): 652-658. 10.1097/00000539-199909000-00022.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Mittmann N, Trakas K, Risebrough N, Liu BA: Utility scores for chronic conditions in a community-dwelling population. Pharmacoeconomics. 1999, 15 (4): 369-376. 10.2165/00019053-199915040-00004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Rashiq S, Bray P: Relative value to surgical patients and anesthesia providers of selected anesthesia related outcomes. BMC Medical Informatics & Decision Making. 2003, 3 (1): 3-10.1186/1472-6947-3-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Reeves SW, Friedman DS, Fleisher LA, Lubomski LH, Schein OD, Bass EB: A decision analysis of anesthesia management for cataract surgery. American Journal of Ophthalmology. 2001, 132 (4): 528-536. 10.1016/S0002-9394(01)01159-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Sage DJ: Major Neurologic Injury Following Regional Anesthesia. Complications of Regional Anesthesia. Edited by: Finucane BT. 1999, London, Churchill Livingstone

    Google Scholar 

  15. Torrance GW: Measurement of health state utilities for economic appraisal. J Health Econ. 1986, 5 (1): 1-30. 10.1016/0167-6296(86)90020-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Tversky A, Kahneman D: The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science. 1981, 211 (4481): 453-458.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Usher M, McClelland JL: Loss aversion and inhibition in dynamical models of multialternative choice. Psychol Rev. 2004, 111 (3): 757-769. 10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.757.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Ustun TB, Rehm J, Chatterji S, Saxena S, Trotter R, Room R: Multiple-informant ranking of the disabling effects of different health conditions in 14 countries. WHO/NIH Joint Project CAR Study Group. Lancet. 1999, 354 (9173): 111-115. 10.1016/S0140-6736(98)07507-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Saifudin Rashiq.

Additional information

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

SR conceived the study idea. All authors designed the study. DE performed the data collection. SR and BDD performed the data analysis. SR and BDD wrote the manuscript and DE read, amended and approved the final draft.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rashiq, S., Edlund, D. & Dick, B.D. Utilities of the Post-anesthesia State derived by the Standard Gamble method in surgical patients. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak 6, 8 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6947-6-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6947-6-8

Keywords