Skip to main content

Frequency-dependent selection by wild birds promotes polymorphism in model salamanders

Abstract

Background

Co-occurrence of distinct colour forms is a classic paradox in evolutionary ecology because both selection and drift tend to remove variation from populations. Apostatic selection, the primary hypothesis for maintenance of colour polymorphism in cryptic animals, proposes that visual predators focus on common forms of prey, resulting in higher survival of rare forms. Empirical tests of this frequency-dependent foraging hypothesis are rare, and the link between predator behaviour and maintenance of variation in prey has been difficult to confirm. Here, we show that predatory birds can act as agents of frequency-dependent selection on terrestrial salamanders. Polymorphism for presence/absence of a dorsal stripe is widespread in many salamander species and its maintenance is a long-standing mystery.

Results

We used realistic food-bearing model salamanders to test whether selection by wild birds maintains a stripe/no-stripe polymorphism. In experimental manipulations, whichever form was most common was most likely to be attacked by ground-foraging birds, resulting in a survival advantage for the rare form.

Conclusion

This experiment demonstrates that frequency-dependent foraging by wild birds can maintain colour polymorphism in cryptic prey.

Background

Maintenance of variation is a classic paradox in evolution because both selection and drift tend to remove variation from populations [1–3]. If one form has an advantage (e.g., being more cryptic), it should replace all others. Likewise, random drift alone will eventually result in loss of all but one form when there are no fitness differences. Maintenance of a stable polymorphism requires either recurrent mutation, a balance between dispersal and divergent selection between populations, or some form of balancing selection within populations [3]. For example, predators are expected to form a search image for the most common form if it is easier to search for one cryptic prey type than to simultaneously look for two [4–6]. If predators switch their search images to whichever prey is most common, the result is frequency-dependent selection against common prey [4–7]. Empirical tests of this frequency-dependent selection hypothesis are rare, and the link between predator behaviour and maintenance of variation in prey has been difficult to confirm [4, 8]. Here, we test whether predatory birds can act as agents of frequency-dependent selection on terrestrial salamanders using a manipulative field experiment.

Multiple species of small, forest floor-dwelling salamanders exhibit polymorphism for the presence of a dorsal stripe (Fig. 1) and what maintains this polymorphism within populations is a long-standing question [9–15]. Polymorphism is widespread both geographically and phylogenetically, occurring in many species of Plethodon throughout North America in addition to the distantly related Batrachoseps of the Pacific coast of North America [16] and Karsenia of Asia [17]. Thus, it is unlikely that this polymorphism is merely a transitional stage in the evolution of colour pattern. Presence or absence of a dorsal stripe appears to have a simple genetic basis in Plethodon cinereus and is not related to sex [12, 18].

Figure 1
figure 1

Examples of polymorphic plethodontid salamanders. Each pair of individuals was captured in the same place at the same time. a, Southern Red-backed Salamanders, Plethodon serratus, Great Smoky Mountains National Park (Tennessee, USA). b, Southern Zigzag Salamanders, P. ventralis, Knoxville (Tennessee, USA). c, California Slender Salamanders, Batrachoseps attenuatus, Napa County (California, USA).

What polymorphic salamander species seem to have in common is that they are small, slender, and locally abundant. These species often comprise a substantial fraction of the animal biomass in temperate forests and are subject to predation by ground-foraging birds and other predators that search the leaf-litter for small animals [16]. Studies of polymorphic Plethodon cinereus have shown differences between the morphs in behaviour [11, 14, 15, 19], physiology [11, 13], and geographic variation in relative abundance [10, 20]. However, none directly address how these differences might relate to a mechanism maintaining variation and there is no evidence of similar phenotypic correlations in the other polymorphic species. In fact, in P. ventralis, the frequency of the striped form is less at higher elevations [21] whereas the frequency of striped P. cinereus is greater at higher latitude and cooler microclimates in general [10, 20]. Thus, phenotypic correlations between appearance and physiology are not consistent (also see [13]) and this is not surprising given that dorsal colour pattern is unlikely to have a direct function in temperature tolerance or metabolism (Plethodon always avoid sunlight).

To explain the maintenance of a similar visual polymorphism across many taxa, we propose that selection acts directly on appearance. The most likely function of dorsal colour pattern is crypsis (Fig. 1), though this is an untested assumption. Though there has been speculation that the red stripe of P. cinereus might be an aposematic signal [19], Brodie and Brodie [22] showed that wild birds prey extensively upon striped P. cinereus, and give no sign that the salamanders are unpalatable. Striped P. cinereus were taken just as often as D. ochrophaeus, which were assumed to be cryptic and palatable [22]. A third, completely red form of P. cinereus does appear to be a colour mimic of the toxic Red-spotted Newt eft (Notophthalmus viridescens), but our focus is on the more widespread stripe/no-stripe polymorphism. Polymorphism in cryptic prey populations might be maintained in migration-selection balance if alternative colour forms are adapted to different habitats [23], or might be promoted by frequency-dependent predation causing rare form advantage within populations [4, 8].

Empirical tests of frequency-dependent foraging on cryptic prey are rare; the best-studied examples involve either colored pellets of dough fed to free-ranging birds [7, 24, 25]or digital images of moths selected by trained Blue Jays (Cyanocitta cristata) [4, 26–28]. These studies demonstrate that birds can generate frequency-dependent selection, that individual Blue Jays learn to focus on abundant prey, and that they readily switch focus in response to changes in prey abundance. Apostatic selection by free-ranging wild birds has been shown for small pastries differing in colour [7, 25, 29], and presence or absence of a stripe [30, 31], and also for real Cepaea snail shells differing in banding pattern [32]. However, in other studies, selection changed from negative frequency-dependence (rare form advantage) to positive frequency-dependence (common form advantage) when artificial prey were provided at high densities, showing that rare form advantage is not entirely general and may depend on abundance or aggregation of prey [25, 33]. A recent study of Trinidadian guppies implicated frequency-dependent survival in the maintenance of variation in conspicuously colored males [34]. However, it is not clear whether search images are important for predators of conspicuous prey, and other factors might affect polymorphism in sexually-selected traits [35, 36]. Thus, whether frequency-dependent foraging can explain the maintenance of polymorphism in realistic prey at realistic densities remains a critical question that might need to be addressed on a case-by-case basis. Here, we show empirically that predatory birds can act as agents of frequency-dependent selection on terrestrial salamanders.

To test for frequency-dependent selection by birds, we used standardized models resembling polymorphic Plethodon salamanders (Fig. 2). A food reward (1/2 peanut) was glued to the underside of each model, and models were set out at random in 10 × 10 m plot at the edge of a woodlot in Knox county, Tennessee (see Methods). We manipulated the relative abundance of striped and unstriped prey and quantified survival by counting the number of models with peanuts still attached at the end of each day.

Figure 2
figure 2

Experimental subjects. (a) Polymorphic model salamanders. (b) Blue Jay (Cyanocitta cristata) foraging in the study area.

Results and Discussion

We observed at least five individual Blue Jays (Cyanocitta cristata) foraging on model salamanders. Other birds observed foraging on the ground in the study site included Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis), American Crows (Corvus brachyrhynchos), American Robins (Turdus migratorius), and Eastern Towhees (Pipilio erythrophthalmus). Only Blue Jays were observed taking peanuts from salamanders.

On the first test day, after unstriped models had been more abundant (9:1 for six days), more unstriped than striped model salamanders were attacked (day 7: 16/25 vs. 6/25, Fisher's exact P = 0.0096). After six days with 5 unstriped and 45 striped models, fewer unstriped models were attacked when tested at equal abundance (day 14: 11/25 vs. 19/25, Fisher's exact P = 0.0421). Whichever form had been rare (1:9) over the past six days had a significant survival advantage over the common form when presented at equal abundance (Table 1).

Table 1 "Survival" rates of model salamanders. Data are given as number survived/number presented each day.

Fig. 3 shows that striped models had higher relative fitness when at low frequency and lower relative fitness when at high frequency. Multiple logistic regression supported a significant relationship between frequency and survival on non-test days (likelihood ratio G2 = 6.37, df = 1, P = 0.0116). There was also a strong effect of "day" (G2 = 58.27, df = 12, P < 0.0001), which probably reflects variation in predator activity (predation was lighter on rainy days) and a delay in learning. There was no detectable association between survival and morph (likelihood ratio G2 = 0.225, df = 1, P = 0.635). Whichever form was rare had a survival advantage within one or two days after the relative abundances were reversed (Fig. 3). Thus, when wild birds were "trained" to expect one form to be most common, they preyed disproportionately on that form. This occurred regardless of which form was common.

Figure 3
figure 3

Relative fitness of model salamanders. Daily survival rates of striped relative to unstriped models (right-hand axis, filled symbols) in comparison to the frequency of striped models (left-hand axis, open symbols). Dashed line illustrates equal frequency and equal fitness.

Selection by birds can be heterogeneous and these results should be tested at other sites. However, decades of research have consistently found that captive and free-ranging wild birds cause frequency-dependent selection on all manner of food items differing in appearance [4, 6, 7, 25, 26, 28]. We show that this very general process can operate on the kind of phenotypic polymorphism seen commonly in small salamanders.

We have not tested the perceptual and behavioural mechanisms underlying frequency-dependent foraging, therefore we have avoided invoking search image formation as the specific ultimate cause of rare form advantage. Nevertheless, this study joins a growing body of literature showing that birds often switch foraging behaviour in response to the relative abundance of alternative cryptic prey and this can promote the maintenance of colour polymorphism [4, 8]. The cognitive processes underlying this phenomenon are important subjects for further research.

In addition, a crucial question for the maintenance of polymorphism is to what extent frequency-dependent foraging by visual predators is counteracted by other selective processes, including non-visual predation. Other sources of mortality and other phenotypic differences between morphs might interfere with or enhance the stable coexistence of distinct colour morphs. The conceptual link between promoting polymorphism and promoting coexistence of similar species [37] merits greater attention.

Conclusion

Rare form advantage owing to frequency-dependent selection by visual predators has been proposed to explain maintenance of polymorphism in many cryptic species [4]. The experiment reported here provides evidence that the key mechanism (frequency-dependent foraging) operates with realistic prey at realistic densities in the wild. The maintenance of colour variation in terrestrial salamanders might be explained by the oldest and most obvious hypothesis – rare form advantage arises because predators tend to overlook rare prey.

Methods

Model salamanders were constructed with modelling clay (Sculpeyâ„¢). One model was sculpted by hand and used to create a flexible mould. This mould was then used to make models of consistent size and body position. Models were either grey-brown or grey-brown with an ochre stripe from the base of the head to the base of the tail (Fig. 2a). A food reward was associated with each model salamander by adhering one peanut half to the bottom of the model using edible library paste (40 g corn starch, 1 L water, boiled and cooled).

The field site was an open area adjacent to a woodlot in Knox County, TN. Polymorphic P. ventralis are known from within 2 km. To avoid spurious loss of models, we used as foraging units 100 black plastic trays (0.185 m2) placed in a 10 × 10 array and filled with partially decomposed leaf-litter (natural forest floor substrate). One m of open lawn was left between adjacent trays. Each day, we used a random number generator to assign model salamanders to 50 trays. The ordering of striped and unstriped models was also random; as a result, each tray position had a 50% chance of having a model salamander each day and the probability that the model was striped was the frequency of striped models for that day. The resulting density of 1/2 m-2 was realistic for small Plethodon and Batrachoseps salamanders [16]. Models were placed haphazardly within the trays and arranged so that the attached peanut could not be seen without moving the model. This design ensured that foraging birds had the opportunity to learn where to look for salamanders and that all "surviving" salamanders would be recovered at the end of each day. In addition, it was important to make sure that each tray was not guaranteed to provide a salamander, so that sometimes giving up and moving to another was the right foraging decision [27].

The experiment was divided into two training periods of six days each and two test days, similar to protocols used with green and brown pastry pellets[7, 25]. First, 5 striped and 45 unstriped salamander models were presented each day from 8:00 am until 7:30 pm (it usually took this entire time for the birds to attack roughly half of the salamander models). At the end of the day, the flats were gathered and the number of "surviving" salamanders counted (those with the peanut still attached). Six days of training were followed immediately by a test day in which 25 of each morph were presented. After the first test day, another six-day training period was started, this time with frequencies of 45 striped and 5 unstriped salamander models. The second test day (25 of each morph) immediately followed the second training period (Table 1).

We used 2 × 2 contingency tables to test the null hypothesis of equal predation rates for striped and unstriped models on the two test days. For the 12 "training" days, we tested for an effect of relative abundance on survival using logistic regression with individual survival as the response variable, day as a random effect, and morph and frequency category as fixed effects. The goodness-of-fit of the model was adequate (likelihood ratio test relative to a saturated model: G2 = 4.03, df = 11, P = 0.708) and adding interaction terms did not significantly improve the fit. For all 14 days of the experiment, we estimated fitness (survival value) of striped relative to unstriped models as w S = [(S A + 0.5)/(S B + 0.5)] ÷ [(U A + 0.5)/(U B + 0.5)], where S A and U A are the numbers of surviving striped and unstriped models, and S B and U B are the numbers before selection. This estimates the ratio of the survival probability of striped relative to unstriped models [38]. For example, when the two forms have equal fitness, w S = 1.0 and when a striped individual is twice as likely to survive as an unstriped individual, w S = 2.0.

References

  1. Ford EB: Ecological genetics. 1964, London: Methuen and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Futuyma DJ: Evolution. 2005, Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Hartl DL, Clark AG: Principles of population genetics. 1997, Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, Inc., 3

    Google Scholar 

  4. Bond AB: The evolution of color polymorphism: Crypticity, searching images, and apostatic selection. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution and Systematics. 2007, 38: 489-514. 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.38.091206.095728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Poulton EB: The colours of animals: Their meaning and use, especially considered in the case of insects. 1890, New York: Appleton

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  6. Tinbergen L: The natural control of insects on pinewoods. I. Factors influencing the intensity of predation by songbirds. Arch Neerlandaises Zool. 1960, 13: 265-343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Allen JA, Clarke BC: Evidence for apostatic selection by wild passerines. Nature. 1968, 220: 501-502. 10.1038/220501a0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Punzalan D, Rodd FH, Hughes KA: Perceptual processes and the maintenance of polymorphism through frequency-dependent predation. Evolutionary Ecology. 2005, 19: 303-320. 10.1007/s10682-005-2777-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Burger JW: Plethodon cinereus (Green) in eastern Pennsylvania and New Jersey. American Naturalist. 1935, 64: 578-586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Lotter F, Scott NJ: Correlation between climate and distribution of the color morphs of the salamander Plethodon cinereus. Copeia. 1977, 1977: 681-690. 10.2307/1443166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Moreno G: Behavioral and physiological differentiation between the color morphs of the salamander, Plethodon cinereus. Journal of Herpetology. 1989, 3: 335-341. 10.2307/1564043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Highton R: Geographic variation in genetic dominance of color morphs of Red-backed Salamander, Plethodon cinereus. Genetics. 1975, 80: 363-374.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Petruzzi EE, Niewiarowski PH, Moore FB-G: The role of thermal niche selection in maintenance of a colour polymorphism in redback salamanders (Plethodon cinereus). Frontiers in Zoology. 2006, 3: 10-10.1186/1742-9994-3-10.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Anthony CD, Venesky MD, Hickerson C-AM: Ecological separation in a polymorphic terrestrial salamander. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2008, 77: 646-653. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2008.01398.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Test FH: Seasonal differences in populations of the red-backed salamander in southeastern Michigan. Papers of the Michigan Academy of Science, Arts, and Letters. 1955, 40: 137-153.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Petranka JW: Salamanders of the United States and Canada. 1998, Washington, D. C.: Smithsonian Institution Press

    Google Scholar 

  17. Min MS, Yang SY, Bonnett RM, Vieites DR, Brandon RA, Wake DB: Discovery of the first Asian plethodontid salamander. Nature. 2005, 435: 87-90. 10.1038/nature03474.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Highton R: The inheritance of the color phases of Plethodon cinereus. Copeia. 1959, 1: 33-37. 10.2307/1440097.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Venesky MD, Anthony CD: Antipredator adaptations and predator avoidance by two color morphs of the Eastern Red-backed Salamander, Plethodon cinereus. Herpetologica. 2007, 63: 450-458. 10.1655/0018-0831(2007)63[450:AAAPAB]2.0.CO;2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Gibbs JP, Karraker NE: Effects of warming conditions in Eastern North American forests on Red-backed Salamander morphology. Conservation Biology. 2006, 20: 913-917. 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2006.00375.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Highton R: Geographic protein variation and speciation in the Plethodon dorsalis complex. Herpetologica. 1997, 53: 345-356.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Brodie ED, Brodie ED: Differential avoidance of mimetic salamanders by free-ranging birds. Science. 1980, 208: 181-182. 10.1126/science.208.4440.181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Nosil P: Reproductive isolation caused by visual predation on migrants between divergent environments. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B. 2004, 271: 1521-1528. 10.1098/rspb.2004.2751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Allen JA: Frequency-dependent selection by predators. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B. 1988, 319: 485-503. 10.1098/rstb.1988.0061.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Weale ME, Whitwell D, Raison HE, Raymond DL, Allen JA: The influence of density on frequency-dependent food selection: A comparison of four experiments with wild birds. Oecologia. 2000, 124: 391-395. 10.1007/s004420000399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Bond AB, Kamil AC: Apostatic selection by blue jays produces balanced polymorphism in virtual prey. Nature. 1998, 395: 594-596. 10.1038/26961.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Bond AB, Kamil AC: Visual predators select for crypticity and polymorphism in virtual prey. Nature. 2002, 415: 609-614. 10.1038/415609a.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Bond AB, Kamil AC: Spatial heterogeneity, predator cognition, and the evolution of color polymorphism in virtual prey. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2006, 103: 3214-3219. 10.1073/pnas.0509963103.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Allen JA: Wild birds prefer to eat the more familiar of artificial morphs that are similar in color. Heredity. 1984, 53: 705-715. 10.1038/hdy.1984.128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Raymond DL: Wild birds prefer the familiar of striped and unstriped artificial prey. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 1984, 23: 229-235. 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1984.tb00141.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Raymond DL, Allen JA: Wild birds prefer the familiar color pattern when feeding on similar artificial morphs. Oikos. 1990, 57: 175-179. 10.2307/3565937.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Allen JA, Raymond DL, Geburtig MA: Wild birds prefer the familiar morph when feeding on pastry-filled shells of the landsnail Cepaea hortensis (Mull). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 1988, 33: 395-401. 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1988.tb00452.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Allen JA, Weale ME: Anti-apostatic selection by wild birds on quasi-natural morphs of the land snail Cepaea hortensis : a generalised linear mixed models approach. Oikos. 2005, 108: 335-343. 10.1111/j.0030-1299.2005.12523.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Olendorf R, Rodd FH, Punzalan D, Houde AE, Hunt C, Reznick DN, Hughes KA: Frequency-dependent survival in natural guppy populations. Nature. 2006, 441: 633-636. 10.1038/nature04646.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Sinervo B, Calsbeek R: The developmental, physiological, neural, and genetical causes and consequences of frequency-dependent selection in the wild. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution and Systematics. 2006, 37: 581-610. 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.37.091305.110128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Sinervo B, Lively C: The rock-paper-scissors game and the evolution of alternative male strategies. Nature. 1996, 380: 240-243. 10.1038/380240a0.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Clarke BC: Balanced polymorphism and the diversity of sympatric species. Taxonomy and geography. Edited by: Nichols D. 1962, Oxford: Systematics Assoc, 47-70.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Haldane JBS: The estimation and significance of the logarithm of the ratio of frequencies. Annals of Human Genetics. 1956, 20: 309-311. 10.1111/j.1469-1809.1955.tb01285.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank R. Fitzpatrick for help with models and fieldwork; and J. Allen, N. Sanders, J. Fordyce, and one anonymous reviewer for comments on the manuscript. This project was supported by the University of Tennessee Pre-collegiate Research Scholars Program.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Benjamin M Fitzpatrick.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

BMF Conceived the project, collected and analyzed the data, and drafted the manuscript. KS and RI helped troubleshoot the experimental design, collect and analyze data, and revise the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fitzpatrick, B.M., Shook, K. & Izally, R. Frequency-dependent selection by wild birds promotes polymorphism in model salamanders. BMC Ecol 9, 12 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6785-9-12

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6785-9-12

Keywords