Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Stressful life events and current psychological distress are associated with self-reported hypertension but not with true hypertension: results from a cross-sectional population-based study

Abstract

Background

The evidence linking stress to hypertension has been scarcely documented in population-based studies.

Methods

Participants were selected through a multi-stage probability sampling and interviewed at home, being submitted to measures of demographics, anthropometrics, blood pressure (BP), and risk factors for hypertension. Hypertension was defined as BP ≥ 140/90 mm Hg or use of BP-lowering drugs or as self-reported hypertension. Stressful life events were investigated through an inventory of nine major life events occurring in the year preceding the interview. Psychological distress was evaluated through a facial scale of expression of emotion in the last month.

Results

In the total, 1,484 adult individuals were investigated. Prevalence of hypertension was lower in individuals who reported any stressful life event in comparison with individuals who did not reported an event (34.3 versus 44.2%, P < 0.01), such as relative or friend death, loss of job, divorce, violence and migration. There was a trend for higher prevalence of hypertension in individuals with higher psychological distress in the last month, which was not longer significant after adjustment for confounding. In contrast, individuals who self-reported hypertension, but actually had normal blood pressure and were not using antihypertensive medication, reported higher numbers of stressful events.

Conclusion

Recent stressful life events and current psychological distress are not associated with hypertension. Associations between stress events and distress with self-reported hypertension are not intermediated by effects of stress on blood pressure, and may be ascribed to negative feeling about disease and not to the disease itself.

Peer Review reports

Background

Stressful life events have been long associated with hypertension [13], but the independence and strength of this association have been disputed [35]. Most evidence came from experiments in animal models [6], clinical experiments [7] and from the assessment of the association between exposure to stressors, such as job stress [8], catastrophes [9], and blood pressure measurement [10], with blood pressure. The findings from epidemiological surveys have not been homogeneous regarding the association between the occurrence of acute stressful events and sustained elevation of blood pressure [11, 12]. Differences in study design, sampling criteria, population surveyed, definition of stress events, and hypertension, may account for the discrepancy of findings [12, 13]. In some studies, the investigation of the association between stress and hypertension was a secondary objective, and stress or blood pressure were not directly determined.

In this prospective planned, population-based survey, we tried to circumvent some limitations of epidemiological studies, investigating if current psychological distress and stressful life events are associated with higher blood pressure and hypertension in individuals living in communities.

Methods

Design, participants and data collection

This study is a report of a large cross-sectional, population-based study aiming to investigate several hypotheses related to cardiovascular disease in Porto Alegre, the Study of Obesity and Risk Factors (SOFT study). Participants were identified through cross-sectional, population-based, multi-stage probability sampling and interviewed at home after giving informed consent. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Hospital de Clínicas of Porto Alegre, which is accredited by the Office of Human Research Protections as an Institutional Review Board. A sampling of individuals aged 18 to 60 years old (none to two individuals in each household according to the number of individuals living at each domicile) plus all residents older than 60 years were interviewed, in order to have an overrepresentation of elderly individuals. Data were collected with a structured and pre-tested questionnaire, which included assessment of demographic characteristics, years at school, familial history of hypertension, smoking, drug treatments, and various questions pertaining to several objectives of the SOFT study. Physical activity was assessed through the International Physical Activities Questionnaire [14]. The type, quantity, and frequency of alcoholic beverage consumption were assessed and the average daily alcohol intake was calculated. Anthropometric measurements were carried out with participants wearing light clothing and no shoes. The field work was done between 2005 and 2007.

Blood pressure measurement and definition of outcome variables

Sitting blood pressure was determined four times during the interview with an automatic and validated device, Omron 705 CP [15]. The average of these measurements was employed in analysis. Hypertension was defined by blood pressure ≥ 140/90 mmHg or use of blood pressure-lowering medication. In addition, we explored the association of stress events with the following outcomes: self-reported hypertension, when the participant reported having the diagnosis of hypertension done by a physician or a nurse, independently of the blood pressure values measured in the interview or use of blood pressure drugs; true awareness, when the awareness combined with the diagnosis of hypertension was confirmed by blood pressure or use of blood pressure drugs; false awareness, when self-reported hypertension was not confirmed by blood pressure or use of blood pressure drugs; and high blood pressure, when the mean of four measurements was = 140/90 mmHg, independently of awareness of hypertension or use of blood pressure drugs.

Measurements of stressful life events and other exposures

The questionnaire investigated the occurrence of nine stressor exposures events derived from seven questions in the year preceding the interview (Table 1). A scale of faces [16] was employed to evaluate the predominant mood in the in the last month, as a surrogate of the intensity of psychological distress. Each face consists of a circle with eyes that do not change and a mouth that varies from a smile of almost a half-circle to a similar half-circle upside down, representing variation of mood from extremely content to extremely discontent. Participants were asked about which face better represented his/her feelings for the most part of the last 30 days.

Table 1 Stressful life events in the last year

Sample size calculation

The sample size of the SOFT study, of approximately 3,000 individuals, was calculated to grant enough power to test several prospectively planned hypotheses of the study. The present analysis was done with the first 1,474 enrolled adults. This sample size assumed a prevalence of hypertension of 30% among non-exposed and a relative risk of 1.25 (25% increase of risk among exposed), for a P alpha of 0.05 and a power of 85%.

Statistical analysis

The characteristics of the sample were described by means ± SD or frequency and proportions. Blood pressure of individuals with and without stressor exposures and categories and in the categories of the scale of faces was compared by means of an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), adjusting for age. Prevalence of hypertension, awareness of hypertension and high blood pressure by the stressor exposures was tested by Chi-square. A logistic regression model was used to control for confounding of the association between stressful life events and psychological distress evaluated by the scale of faces and hypertension, separately by gender. Risk factors for hypertension were included in the model. Analyses were done in the SPSS, version 14.0.

Results

Table 2 presents demographic and anthropometric characteristics of the study sample, together with risk factors for hypertension, blood pressure and the proportion of individuals with hypertension. Most participants were women and the mean body mass index was within the overweight range. Measures of psychological distress in the whole sample are presented in table 3. About a third of the individuals had at least one stressful life event in the last year, mostly a death of a next of kin or a friend, severe illness in the family and loss of job. Some individuals had more than one event. The rate of total events by individual was 0.5 ± 0.8. The corresponding rate for deaths was 0.2 ± 0.4 per individual. Despite the high frequency of a major life event in the preceding year, 73.8% of the individuals were content, very content or extremely content in the month preceding the interview.

Table 2 Selected characteristics of the study sample (N = 1484)
Table 3 Exposure to stressful life events and to psychological distress evaluated by the faces scale in the study sample (N = 1484)

Blood pressure was lower in individuals who had some stressful life event in the last year. For example, systolic and diastolic blood pressure of individuals that reported any life event were lower than blood pressure of those that did no report an event (124.2 ± 21.0 mmHg and 76.3 ± 12.0 mmHg versus 128.4 ± 22.4 mm Hg and 77.2 ± 12.2 mmHg, respectively, P < 0.01). These differences were no longer significant after adjustment for age (Table 4). No consistent association between the categories of the faces scale and blood pressure was observed (Table 4).

Table 4 Means of systolic and diastolic blood pressure* (mmHg) by stressor exposures and psychological distress evaluated by the faces scale (N = 1477)

Table 5 shows the association between stressful events in the last year and current distress with hypertension, self-reported hypertension and high blood pressure. Individuals who reported any death among relatives and friend in the last year had higher prevalence of hypertension defined by any criterion. The opposite occurred with loss of job, divorce, and migration. The categories of the faces scale were not consistently associated with hypertension and the proportion of individuals with high blood pressure, but presented a linear trend for an association with self-reported hypertension.

Table 5 Prevalence of true hypertension (BP ≥ 140/90 mmHg or use of BP drugs), awareness of hypertension and BP ≥ 140/90 mmHg by the occurrence of stressor exposures and psychological distress evaluated by the faces scale (N = 1484)

Table 6 presents the odds ratio for hypertension stratified by gender and for the whole sample, adjusting for age, skin color, physical activity, alcohol abuse, body mass index, strong parental history of hypertension and education. The trends observed in the bivariate analysis (table 5) disappeared, either for stress events in the last year as well for the categories of faces scale.

Table 6 The association between stressor exposures and psychological distress evaluated by faces scale with hypertension (OR and 95% CI)

Current psychological distress (figure 1) remained associated with self-reported hypertension in the multivariate analysis, particularly when the diagnosis was not confirmed by blood pressure measurement or use of blood pressure-lowering drug (false awareness). The number of stressful life events in the last year was associated only with the false awareness of hypertension (figure 2). Risk ratios for having blood pressure ≥ 140/90 mmHg, irrespective of awareness or use of blood pressure-lowering drugs, were all below 1.0 but not significant for the several categories of faces scale and the number of life events in the last year. These analyses were run separately by gender, and the estimates did not change substantially.

Figure 1
figure 1

The association between self-report hypertension and false awareness of hypertension with psychological distress evaluated by faces scale (OR and 95% CI, adjusted for gender, age, skin color, physical activity, alcohol abuse, body mass index, strong parental history and education). *P for trend

Figure 2
figure 2

The association between self-reported hypertension and false awareness of hypertension (reported hypertension not confirmed by BP or use of BP drugs) with the number of stressor exposures in the last year (OR and 95% CI, adjusted for gender, age, skin color, physical activity, alcohol abuse, body mass index, strong parental history and education). *P for trend

Discussion

In this population-based cross-sectional study we were able to describe the prevalence of hypertension, the frequency of individuals with stressful life events in the last year and the current psychological distress evaluated by faces scale in a large sample of individuals living in communities. The estimates are not representative of the whole city in view of the oversampling of elderly individuals, but provided enough power to test the association between measures of stress and measures of hypertension. Overall, there was no association between any measure of stress and hypertension diagnosed by high blood pressure or use of blood pressure-lowering drugs. Psychological distress evaluated by faces scale, however, was associated with self-reported hypertension. Individuals that had a higher number of stressful life events in the last year reported hypertension not confirmed (false awareness) more frequently.

The belief that psychological factors affect long-term blood pressure regulation dates back to the early 20th century, since the classic hypothesis of Alexander linking repressed hostility to hypertension [17]. The belief that stress is a cause of high blood pressure and cardiovascular disease at all is deeply-rooted among populations. In a population survey in Canada, 44% of the participants believed that stress/worry was the major cause of cardiovascular disease [18]. Studies of the association between stress and hypertension have showed variable results. Most studies have identified positive trends [19, 20], but even inverse associations have been reported [12, 21]. The novel finding of our investigation was the contrasting association between self-reported hypertension and true hypertension with stress events and psychological distress evaluated by the faces scale.

The observation that young individuals with high blood pressure reported fewer life events was already reported by Theorell et al in a Sweden [22]. The lack of sensitivity to the environment seems to be a consistent trait in people who develop hypertension. In a laboratory study, subjects who had a positive family history of hypertension and who exhibited a personality pattern that included denial and unwillingness to admit to neurotic feelings or aggressiveness exhibited higher BP responsiveness during stress periods than subjects who had a negative family history [23]. A low sensitivity to pain has been also demonstrated in patients with hypertension [24]. We identified that individuals with high blood pressure had lower prevalence of migraine in a population-based study [25]. In the cohort of Nord-Trondelag there was a strong linear trend (P < .001) of decreasing prevalence of chronic musculoskeletal complaints with increasing BP values [26]. The notion that individuals with hypertension have some insensitivity to environmental stimulus is strengthened by studies that compared objective versus subjective reporting of stressful conditions. Objective stressors, such as job barriers and time pressure were significantly associated with hypertension, but not self-reported stressors at the individual level [27].

Additionally, the association between stress and hypertension reported in some studies may in fact be ascribed to the negative feeling about disease and not to the disease itself. Awareness of hypertension status (irrespective of actual blood pressure) appears to be associated with increased perceptions of psychological and physical symptoms [21]. In a meta-analysis, Jorgensen et al [28] concluded that hypertension is inversely associated with negative emotions in individuals who were unaware of the diagnosis and directly associated when they were aware of their blood pressure level. Other explanations for these associations have been proposed, such as that these individuals would look for health care more frequently, having a higher probability of having a diagnosis of hypertension. Our findings are in accordance with these interpretations, suggesting that the results of cohort studies that had used self-reported hypertension may be flawed [29, 30].

Our study has some limitations that deserve mention. First, the cross-sectional design does not preclude an inverse causality, despite the fact that the exposures to stress events were evaluated retrospectively. This aspect would influence the association of stress with self-reported hypertension, but not with true hypertension, which was not associated with stress events and categories of faces scale. Second, scale of faces has not been commonly used in the evaluation of the association of distress events and hypertension. The association of more negative categories of the faces scale with stress life events, however, was reported before [31]. Third, we used a relatively small number of major stress events, since a middle-range of from 30 to 50 items seems both optimal in terms of predictive power and efficiency in terms of use of time and space [32]. The events that we investigated, however, were those more important and expectedly associated with profound consequences on cardiovascular regulation. Fourth, the exposure of individuals to stress events could result in hypertension in the future, an association that would be captured only in a longitudinal study. It is unlikely, however, that the absence of an association between stress events and current distress with blood pressure and hypertension would result in hypertension thereafter. Fifth, we did not adjust for the overrepresentation of elderly participants, therefore our estimates of prevalence of hypertension and stress does not apply to the whole city. The oversampling of elderly individuals, however, did not influence the direction and strength of the associations, since they were independent of age in the multivariate analysis. Sixth, the measurement of blood pressure in only one visit did not capture the usual blood pressure of the participants. This is, however, a conservative bias, since the alert reaction of some individuals to blood pressure determination would artificially increase blood pressure. Moreover, blood pressure was measured four times, with an automatic validated device, at home and not at office. Since blood pressure was measured at home it could not represent the values obtained in other environments, such as medical offices and at job. Therefore, our classification of false awareness of hypertension may reflect the discrepancy between office and home measurements. And finally, we did not study the risk of more chronic stress events [33] and daily and within-day events [34].

Conclusion

Selected stressful life events in the last year and current psychological distress evaluated by the faces scale are not associated with hypertension in individuals living in communities. The association between stress events in the last year and current psychological distress with self-reported hypertension, together with the absence of association with high blood pressure, suggests that the reported associations between stress and reported hypertension are not intermediated by effects of stress on blood pressure.

References

  1. Nyklicek I, Vingerhoets JJ, Van Heck GL: Hypertension and objective and self-reported stressor exposure: a review. J Psychosom Res. 1996, 40: 585-601. 10.1016/0022-3999(95)00647-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Parati G, Antonicelli R, Guazzarotti F, Paciaroni E, Mancia G: Cardiovascular effects of an earthquake. Direct evidence by ambulatory blood pressure monitoring. Hypertension. 2001, 38: 1093-1095. 10.1161/hy1101.095334.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Schwartz AR, Gerin W, Davidson KW, Pickering TG, Brosschot JF, Thayer JF, Christenfeld N, Linden W: Toward causal model of cardiovascular responses to stress and the development of cardiovascular disease. Psychosom Med. 2003, 65: 22-35. 10.1097/01.PSY.0000046075.79922.61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Rozanski A, Blumenthal JA, Kaplan J: Impact of psychological factors on the pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease and implications for therapy. Circulation. 1999, 99: 2192-2217.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Nicolson DJ, Dickinson HO, Campbell F, Cook J, Renton F, Ford GA, Mason J: Relaxation therapies for the management of essential hypertension in adults (Protocol for a Cochrane Review). The Cochrane Library. 2006, Oxford: Update Software, 1

  6. Bernatova I, Csizmadiova Z: Effect of chronic social stress on nitric oxide synthesis and vascular function in rats with family history of hypertension. Life Sci. 2006, 78: 1726-1732. 10.1016/j.lfs.2005.08.005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Light KC, Girdler SS, Sherwood A, Bragdon EE, Brownley KA, West SG, Hinderliter AL: High stress responsivity predicts later blood pressure only in combination with positive family history and high life stress. Hypertension. 1999, 33: 1458-1464.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Andren L, Hansson L, Bjorkman M, Jonsson A: Noise as a contributory factor in the development of elevated arterial pressure. A study of the mechanisms by which noise may raise blood pressure in man. Acta Med Scand. 1980, 207: 493-498.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Gerin WA, Chaplin WD, Schwartz JEB, Holland JC, Alter RE, Wheeler RF, Duong DG, Pickering TGA: Sustained blood pressure increase after an acute stressor: the effects of the 11 September 2001 attack on the New York City World Trade Center. J Hypertens. 2005, 23: 279-284. 10.1097/00004872-200502000-00009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Pickering TG, James GD, Boddie C, Harshfield GA, Blank S, Laragh JH: How common is white coat hypertension?. JAMA. 1988, 259: 225-228. 10.1001/jama.259.2.225.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Menendez Villalva C, Montes Martinez A, Nunez Losada C, Fernandez Dominguez MJ, Gamarra Mondelo T, Bujan Garmendia S: Environmental stress and cardiovascular reactivity: the effect of stressful life events on hypertensive patients. Aten Primaria. 2002, 30: 631-637.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Radi S, Lang T, Lauwers-Cancès V, Diène E, Chatellier G, Larabi L, De Gaudemaris R, IHPAF group: Job constraint and arterial hypertension: different effects in men and women: the IHPAF II case-control study. Occup Environ Med. 2005, 62: 711-717. 10.1136/oem.2004.012955.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Rutledge T, Hogan BE: A quantitative review of prospective evidence linking psychological factors with hypertension development. Psychosom Med. 2002, 64: 758-766. 10.1097/01.PSY.0000031578.42041.1C.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Guidelines for Data Processing and Analysis of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) – Short and Long Forms. 2005, [http://www.ipaq.ki.se/scoring.pdf]

  15. O'Brien E, Mee F, Atkins N, Thomas M: Evaluation of three devices for self-measurement of blood pressure according to the revised British Hypertension Society Protocol: The Omron HEM-705CP, Philips HP 5332 and Nissei DS-175. Blood Pr Monitor. 1996, 1: 55-61.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Mc'Dowell I, Newell C: Measuring Health. A Guide to Rating Scales and Questionnaires. Cap. 5: Psychological Well-Being. 1996, New York:Oxford University Press, 2

    Google Scholar 

  17. Alexander F: Psychoanalytic study of case of essential hypertension. Psychossom Med. 1939, 31: 139-152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Kirkland SA, MacLean DR, Langille DB, Joffres MR, MacPherson KM, Andreou P: Knowledge and awareness of risk factors for cardiovascular disease among Canadians 55 to 74 years of age: results from the Canadian Heart Health Surveys,1986–1992. CMAJ. 1999, 161 (8 Suppl): S3-S9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Perez LH, Gutierrez LA, Vioque J, Torres Y: Relation between overweight, diabetes, stress and hypertension: a case-control study in Yarumal – Antioqia, Colombia. Eur J Epidemiol. 2001, 17: 275-280. 10.1023/A:1017975925554.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Everson SA, Kaplan GA, Goldberg DE, Salonen JT: Hypertension incidence is predicted by high levels of hopelessness in finish men. Hypertension. 2000, 35: 561-567.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Tennant C: Life stress and hypertension. J Cardiovasc Risk. 2001, 8: 51-56. 10.1097/00043798-200102000-00007.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Theorell T, Svensson J, Knox S, Waller D, Alvarez M: Young men with high blood pressure report few recent life events. J Psychosom Res. 1986, 30 (2): 243-249. 10.1016/0022-3999(86)90055-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Jorgensen RS, Houston BK: Family history of hypertension, personality patterns, and cardiovascular reactivity to stress. Psychosom Med. 1986, 48: 102-117.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Absi M, France C, Harju A, Wittmers L: Adrenocortical and nociceptive responses to opioid blockade in hypertension-prone men and women. Psychosom Med. 2006, 68: 292-298. 10.1097/01.psy.0000203240.64965.bd.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Wiehe M, Costa FS, Moreira LB, Stoll MR, Fuchs FD: Migraine is more frequent in individuals with optimal and normal blood pressure: a population-based study. J Hypertens. 2002, 20: 1303-1306. 10.1097/00004872-200207000-00016.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Hagen K, Zwart JA, Holmen J, Svebak S, Bovim G, Stovner LJ: The Nord-Trondelag Health Study Does hypertension protect against chronic musculoskeletal complaints? The Nord-Trondelag Health Study. Arch Intern Med. 2005, 165: 916-922. 10.1001/archinte.165.8.916.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Greiner BA, Krause N, Ragland D, Fisher JM: Occupational stressors and hypertension: a multi-method study using observer-based job analysis and self-reports in urban transit operators. Soc Sci Med. 2004, 59: 1081-1094. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.12.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Jorgensen RS, Johnson BT, Kolodziej ME, Schreer GE: Elevated blood pressure and personality: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 1996, 120: 293-320. 10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.293.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Levenstein S, Smith MW, Kaplan GA: Psychosocial predictors of hypertension in men and women. Arch Intern Med. 2001, 161: 1341-1346. 10.1001/archinte.161.10.1341.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Cozier Y, Palmer JR, Horton NJ, Fredman L, Wise LA, Rosenberg L: Racial discrimination and the incidence of hypertension in US black women. Ann Epidemiol. 2006, 16: 681-687. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2005.11.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Sparrenberger F, Santos IS, Lima RCL: Association of stressful life events and psychological distress: a community-based study. Cad Saude Publica. 2004, 20 (1): 249-258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Turner J, Wheaton B: Checklist measurement of stressful life events. Measuring stress. Edited by: Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU. 1997, New York: Oxford University Press, 29-58.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Lepore SJ: Measurement of chronic stressors. Measuring stress. Edited by: Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU. 1997, New York: Oxford University Press, 102-120.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Eckenrode J, Bolger N: Daily and within-day event measurement. Measuring stress. Edited by: Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU. 1997, New York: Oxford University Press, 80-101.

    Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grants from CNPq, CAPES and FAPERGS, FIPE-Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre, Brazil.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Flávio D Fuchs.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have nocompeting interests.

Authors' contributions

SCF and LBM conceived, designed and coordinated the study, and prepared the questionnaires. FS directed part of the field work. FS and FDF performed the statistical analysis and prepared the drafts of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sparrenberger, F., Fuchs, S.C., Moreira, L.B. et al. Stressful life events and current psychological distress are associated with self-reported hypertension but not with true hypertension: results from a cross-sectional population-based study. BMC Public Health 8, 357 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-8-357

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-8-357

Keywords