Skip to main content

Awareness of physical activity in healthy middle-aged adults: a cross-sectional study of associations with sociodemographic, biological, behavioural, and psychological factors

Abstract

Background

Interventions to promote physical activity have had limited success. One reason may be that inactive adults are unaware that their level of physical activity is inadequate and do not perceive a need to change their behaviour. We aimed to assess awareness of physical activity, defined as the agreement between self-rated and objective physical activity, and to investigate associations with sociodemographic, biological, behavioural, and psychological factors.

Methods

We conducted an exploratory, cross-sectional analysis of awareness of physical activity using baseline data collected from 453 participants of the Feedback, Awareness and Behaviour study (Cambridgeshire, UK). Self-rated physical activity was measured dichotomously by asking participants if they believed they were achieving the recommended level of physical activity. Responses were compared to objective physical activity, measured using a combined accelerometer and heart rate monitor (Actiheart®). Four awareness groups were created: overestimators, realistic inactives, underestimators, and realistic actives. Logistic regression was used to assess associations between awareness group and potential correlates.

Results

The mean (standard deviation) age of participants was 47.0 (6.9) years, 44.4% were male, and 65.1% were overweight (body mass index ≥ 25). Of the 258 (57.0%) who were objectively classified as inactive, 130 (50.4%) misperceived their physical activity by incorrectly stating that they were meeting the guidelines (overestimators). In a multivariable logistic regression model adjusted for age and sex, those with a lower body mass index (Odds Ratio (OR) = 0.95, 95% Confidence Interval (CI) = 0.90 to 1.00), higher physical activity energy expenditure (OR = 1.03, 95% CI = 1.00 to 1.06) and self-reported physical activity (OR = 1.13, 95% CI = 1.07 to 1.19), and lower intention to increase physical activity (OR = 0.69, 95% CI = 0.48 to 0.99) and response efficacy (OR = 0.53, 95% CI = 0.31 to 0.91) were more likely to overestimate their physical activity.

Conclusions

Overestimators have more favourable health characteristics than those who are realistic about their inactivity, and their psychological characteristics suggest that they are less likely to change their behaviour. Personalised feedback about physical activity may be an important first step to behaviour change.

Peer Review reports

Background

Regular engagement in physical activity offers many well-established health benefits, including reduced risk of obesity, type 2 diabetes, cardiovascular disease, and some cancers [1]. According to previous national and international guidelines, adults should accumulate at least 30 minutes of moderate to vigorous physical activity on 5 or more days of the week in order to derive such benefits (current guidelines encourage adults to engage in moderate-intensity aerobic physical activity for at least 150 minutes per week) [24]. Public health campaigns have been implemented to educate the general population about the wide-ranging benefits of physical activity and to increase awareness of the guidelines [5, 6]. Despite these efforts, accelerometry data from the UK and the US indicate that fewer than 5% and 10% of the adult population are achieving the recommended level of physical activity, respectively [7, 8]. The proportion of adults who meet the guidelines is unlikely to increase without the development of more effective physical activity promotion efforts. Multiple systematic reviews have concluded that existing interventions implemented in primary care and community settings are limited in that they produce only small, short-term changes in behaviour [912]. Reasons for this general lack of effectiveness remain unclear [13].

One hypothesis is that inactive adults do not perceive a need to change their level of physical activity because they are unaware that their current behaviour is inadequate [1417]. Evaluating the adequacy of one’s physical activity is difficult because it not only requires an accurate summarisation of the frequency, duration, and intensity of activity into a single metric, but also knowledge of what constitutes a healthy level. In contrast to dichotomous behaviours, such as smoking or condom use, physical activity includes numerous planned, habitual, and incidental (e.g., walking, stair climbing, or standing) activities throughout one’s day [17]. This makes the distinction between healthy and unhealthy behaviour less clear, and such a distinction may be complicated by inconsistent or changing guidelines [18].

Awareness of physical activity has previously been defined as the agreement between self-rated and actual levels of physical activity. Self-rated physical activity is assessed by asking individuals to provide a single evaluation of the quantity of physical activity they engage in (e.g., “active” or “inactive”). Actual levels of physical activity are assessed using either a self-reported measure (e.g., a physical activity questionnaire) or an objective measure (e.g., an accelerometer), both of which result in a quantified level of physical activity. It is important to highlight that the discrepancy assessed in measures of awareness of physical activity represents the accuracy of an individual’s belief about whether or not their level of physical activity is adequate, and is distinct from determining error or validity when comparing self-reported and objective measures.

Previous studies of the awareness of physical activity show that as many as 61% of adults who are not achieving the recommended level of physical activity overestimate their activity [1417]. Although their inactivity places them at increased risk of a variety of chronic diseases and disorders [1], overestimators tend to have more favourable health characteristics compared to those who are realistic about their inactivity [1517]. Notably, they are also less likely to express an intention to change their behaviour [14, 16]. These findings are in accordance with the theory of planned behaviour and the Precaution Adoption Process Model, which posits that individuals are not expected to begin the process of behaviour change until after they become aware that their current behaviour is putting their health at risk [19, 20]. In this way, misperception of physical activity may be an important barrier to behaviour change, as it may result in individuals not recognising a need to increase their physical activity and being unaffected by physical activity promotion efforts.

To date, the majority of studies that have assessed awareness of physical activity have done so using scaled measures of self-rated physical activity that make no reference to physical activity guidelines, e.g., “In general, over the last year would you say you have been extremely active, moderately active, or not very active?” [14, 15, 17, 21, 22]. Responses have been interpreted dichotomously to suggest that individuals perceive themselves to be either “active” or “inactive”, and intermediate responses have been classified as “active” by default. Such methods are likely to introduce misclassification and the use of a direct measure of whether or not an individual perceives they are meeting the guidelines would be a substantial improvement [16]. Additionally, most studies have used self-reported measures of actual physical activity that have been shown to be prone to bias [1416, 23]. The use of an objective measure would provide a more valid operationalisation of awareness of physical activity [17, 21, 22].

In the present study, we assessed awareness of physical activity in a population-based sample of healthy middle-aged adults. We utilised a dichotomous measure of self-rated physical activity that makes reference to the physical activity guidelines, and we compared responses with a validated objective measure of physical activity. We also explored the associations between misperception of physical activity and various sociodemographic, biological, behavioural, and psychological factors. A deeper understanding of the factors associated with misperception could be important for determining why individuals choose to be active or inactive and might inform the development of effective strategies for promoting physical activity.

Methods

Design

This exploratory, cross-sectional study utilised data collected as part of a randomised controlled trial, the Feedback, Awareness and Behaviour (FAB) study [24]. All of the variables in the present study were collected at baseline, prior to randomisation and receipt of any intervention materials. The study obtained full ethical approval from the Cambridgeshire 2 Research Ethics Committee (reference number 07/Q0108/79). Written informed consent was obtained from each participant.

Participants and setting

Participants of the FAB study were recruited from the Fenland Study, an ongoing population-based, observational study investigating the influence of lifestyle and genetic factors on the development of diabetes, obesity, and related metabolic disorders [25]. Patients born between 1950 and 1975 and registered with participating general practices in Cambridgeshire, UK were invited to take part. Exclusion criteria assessed by general practitioners included being diagnosed with diabetes, a terminal illness with a prognosis of less than one year, or a psychotic illness. Those who were pregnant or lactating, or unable to walk unaided were also excluded. Invitations to take part in the FAB study were sent to all participants who were scheduled to attend an assessment between September 2007 and August 2008. Those who developed a rash while wearing a combined heart rate monitor and accelerometer (explained in detail below) to measure free-living physical activity or who did not provide at least three days worth of complete physical activity data were excluded from the FAB study.

Measures

During the Fenland Study, participants underwent a health assessment. Anthropometric (e.g., height and weight), clinical (e.g., blood pressure and pulse rate), and physical activity measurements (e.g., heart rate, movement, oxygen consumption at rest and during a sub-maximal treadmill test) were assessed by trained staff using standard operating procedures. An oral glucose tolerance test was administered, and two blood samples were taken to assess glucose levels and blood lipids. Demographics, medical history, and general lifestyle were assessed through self-report, and the validated SF-8™ Health Survey was completed [26]. At the end of the assessment, a single-piece monitor capable of measuring both acceleration and heart rate (Actiheart®) was used to objectively measure free-living physical activity for six days and nights continuously [27]. Physical activity level (PAL) was calculated as the ratio of total energy expenditure in a 24-hour period to basal metabolic rate. The average PAL over each day that a participant wore a monitor was calculated for participants who wore a monitor on three or more days and had at least 72 hours of complete data. The objectively measured average PAL was used in the assessment of awareness of physical activity (explained in detail below). Physical activity energy expenditure (PAEE) was calculated using branched equation modelling and both acceleration and heart rate data [28]. This approach has high validity for estimating the intensity of physical activity [29, 30] and overcomes some of the key limitations associated with either accelerometers or heart rate monitors alone [27]. Self-reported physical activity was also measured during this time via the previously validated Recent Physical Activity Questionnaire [31].

Prior to the start of their Fenland Study health assessment, FAB study participants completed a baseline questionnaire, which included a measure of self-rated physical activity (explained in detail below). The measures were taken from the previously validated ProActive study questionnaires, which were largely based on the Theory of Planned Behavior and were amended where appropriate [19, 32, 33]. Time orientation (defined as the tendency to be motivated by either future or present goals in making decisions) was measured using a nine-item form of the validated Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory [34]. Concern about physical activity was measured by asking participants, “How concerned are you about your level of physical activity?” Participants rated their concern on a 4-point scale, ranging from “not at all” to “very”. Worry about physical activity was measured by asking participants, “How often have you thought about your level of physical activity?” and “How often have thoughts about your physical activity level affected your mood?” Participants answered on a 4-point scale, ranging from “not at all” to “almost all of the time” [32, 33].

Each of the following items included a statement that was evaluated on a 5-point response scale, ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree” [19, 32, 33]. Self-efficacy (e.g., “I am confident that I could be more physically active in the next two months, if I wanted to”) and response efficacy (e.g., “If I was more physically active in the next two months, it is likely that my health would improve”) were measured with two and four Likert items, respectively. Perceived importance (i.e., “Physical activity is important for maintaining good health”), subjective norm (i.e., “Most people who are important to me would want me to be more physically active”), perceived adequacy (i.e., I do enough physical activity to stay healthy), and intention to be more physically active (i.e., “I intend to be more physically active in the next two months”) each were measured using one Likert item.

In order to assess awareness of physical activity, we first informed participants that, according to national recommendations, people should be active at a moderate intensity (e.g., brisk walking) for at least 30 minutes per day at least 5 days of the week. Self-rated physical activity was then measured by asking participants to indicate whether they think they achieved this level of activity over the preceding month. We then classified participants’ objective physical activity as either inactive (PAL < 1.7) or active (PAL ≥ 1.7) in line with physical activity guidelines [2, 3]. An habitual PAL ≥ 1.7 is associated with reduced risk of overweight, obesity, type 2 diabetes, and cardiovascular disease, and is approximately equivalent to 30 minutes of moderate to vigorous physical activity per day at least 5 days of the week [3]. Participants were grouped into a 2×2 table based upon the agreement between their self-rated and objective physical activity measures. This resulted in four awareness groups: overestimators, realistic inactives, underestimators, and realistic actives (see Figure 1).

Figure 1
figure 1

Classification of participants into awareness groups ( n= 453).

Analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted using STATA software [35] and two-tailed p-values with the predefined cut-off for statistical significance set at 0.05. Descriptive statistics (means, standard deviations, numbers, and percentages) were used to describe key demographic and health characteristics. Analyses were undertaken separately for inactive participants (overestimators and realistic inactives) and active participants (realistic actives and under-estimators) to establish correlates of misperception of physical activity. Univariable associations were assessed using Student’s t-tests for continuous variables and Pearson’s chi-square tests for categorical variables. Statistically significant variables were selected for inclusion in multivariable logistic regression models, adjusted for age and sex. Multicollinearity was checked and variance inflation factor statistics for each variable were less than 1.6. Variables that did not retain their significance were manually removed, one at a time, starting with the variable with the highest p-value, until a model containing only variables with significant associations was achieved. To distinguish associations between proximal and distal factors and to avoid overadjustment (i.e., the inclusion of variables hypothesised to be on the causal pathway), the psychological variables were modelled separately from the sociodemographic, biological, and behavioural variables.

Results

Invitations to take part in the FAB study were mailed to 730 Fenland Study participants. Of the 544 (74.5%) individuals who were assessed for eligibility, 91 (16.7%) were missing baseline data on one or more of the independent variables of interest. Those with missing data were excluded from all analyses and did not differ from those included in the analyses according to age, sex, or body mass index (BMI; all p > 0.05).

Sample characteristics

The mean (standard deviation) age of participants in the final sample (n = 453) was 47.0 (6.9) years, and 44.4% were male. Most obtained at least a secondary school level of education and were employed full-time (65.8%). On average, participants were overweight (65.1% had a BMI ≥ 25 kg/m2), with 25.4% classified as obese (a BMI >30 kg/m2). The majority were non-smokers (85.9%) who did not drink more than 10 units of alcohol per week (74.6%) and rated their overall health as either good, very good, or excellent (74.0%).

Figure 1 shows the classification of participants into four physical activity awareness groups. Approximately 55.7% of participants correctly assessed whether or not they were meeting the physical activity guidelines (28.3% realistic inactives and 27.4% realistic actives). Of the 258 (57.0%) participants who were objectively classified as inactive, 130 (50.4%) misperceived their physical activity by incorrectly stating that they were meeting the guidelines (overestimators). Of the 195 (43.0%) participants who were objectively classified as active, 71(36.4%) misperceived their physical activity by incorrectly stating that they were not meeting the guidelines (underestimators).

Univariable associations

Table 1 shows the characteristics of participants by physical activity awareness groups. There were no differences between the groups according to age, sex, education, family history of diabetes, HbA1c level, systolic blood pressure, smoking status, alcohol consumption, SF-8 physical summary, perceived importance, worry, and present time orientation. Among the inactive participants, overestimators were less likely than realistic inactives to be employed full-time. They also had a lower BMI and were more physically active than realistic inactives according to both objective and self-reported measures. Compared to realistic inactives, overestimators expressed significantly less intention to increase physical activity, subjective norm, response efficacy, self-efficacy, and concern. They also expressed greater perceived adequacy and are more oriented towards making goals for the future.

Table 1 Participant characteristics categorized by misperception of physical activity within the inactive and active groups ( n= 453)

Among the active participants, underestimators had a higher BMI than realistic actives. They also had a lower VO2 max, a higher cholesterol ratio and pulse rate, and were less physically active than realistic actives according to both objective and self-reported measures. Compared to realistic actives, underestimators expressed significantly greater intention to increase physical activity, subjective norm, response efficacy, and concern. They also expressed less perceived adequacy, scored lower on the SF-8 mental summary scale, and were less likely to rate their overall health as good, very good, or excellent.

Multivariable associations

Table 2 shows the results of logistic regression models of misperception of physical activity on the sociodemographic, biological, and behavioural variables (Model 1) and on the psychological variables (Model 2), each adjusted for age and sex. Among the inactive participants, the results of Model 1 show that the multivariable associations with overestimation were similar to the univariable associations. Model 2 shows that those with less intention to increase physical activity and response efficacy, and those with greater perceived adequacy and future-orientation, were more likely to overestimate their physical activity.

Table 2 Multivariable logistic regression models showing odds of misperceiving physical activity, adjusted for age and sex, within the inactive and active groups

Among the active participants, the results of model 1 show that a high BMI and pulse rate, and a low self-reported physical activity were associated with a greater likelihood of underestimation. Model 2 shows that those with less perceived adequacy and greater subjective norm were more likely to underestimate their physical activity.

Discussion

This is the first study to assess awareness of physical activity using a dichotomous measure of self-rated physical activity that specifically asked about adherence to physical activity guidelines and compared responses with a precise objective measure. We found that among the 57.0% of adults who were inactive, 50.4% incorrectly perceived that they were achieving the recommended level of physical activity. Our findings are similar to those of previous studies, which show that the prevalence of overestimation is between 46% and 61% [1417]. Additionally, the proportion of those who were active, but who underestimate their physical activity was low (36.4%). Considering that this small body of research has been limited by a reliance on potentially biased assessments of awareness of physical activity, our confirmation of previous findings is noteworthy and indicates that physical activity misperception is a common phenomenon across multiple populations.

Compared to those who correctly perceived themselves to be inactive, overestimators had a lower BMI and engaged in more physical activity according to both self-reported and objective measures, although not enough to be classified as active. They were also less likely to be employed full-time. In contrast, individuals who underestimated their physical activity had a higher BMI and pulse rate, and engaged in less physical activity than individuals who correctly perceived themselves to be active. These findings are in line with previous research that suggests overestimators have more positive indicators of health compared to underestimators [1517]. One plausible explanation for these results is that overestimators have an optimistic bias and they assume that they are sufficiently active simply because they have more favourable health characteristics [17]. For example, the link between physical activity and weight is well known. Overestimators may therefore take their lower BMI as a signal that their level of physical activity is adequate, regardless of whether or not it actually is. For underestimators, this mechanism may operate in reverse, with less favourable health characteristics signalling that they are insufficiently active. In order to further explore these associations, and to establish the temporality of the proposed mechanism, longitudinal studies that assess changes in awareness of physical activity and health characteristics are needed.

It is important to highlight that although overestimators appear to be healthier than those who are realistic about their inactivity, on average they were overweight and their physical activity level was inadequate. This places them at increased health risk, which may be worsened by their inaccurate belief that they do enough physical activity to stay healthy [1]. Overestimators are also less inclined to believe that physical activity has beneficial effects, and they express less intention to increase their physical activity than those who are realistic about their inactivity. Interestingly, they are also more motivated by future goals than realistic inactives. Compared to those who correctly perceived themselves to be active, underestimators had less of a belief that they do enough physical activity to stay healthy, but they did have a strong belief that those who are important want them to be more physically active. These results replicate previously shown associations between overestimation and intentions [14, 16], as well as between underestimation and subjective norm [24], suggesting that they are unlikely to be due to chance. Within the framework of health behaviour theory, each of these findings suggest that overestimators are less likely to increase their physical activity than realistic inactives, and that underestimators are more likely to increase their activity than realistic actives [19, 20]. Additional research is necessary to determine the association between misperception of physical activity and behaviour, and between changes in awareness of physical activity and changes in behaviour.

Many theory-based physical activity interventions target the psychological factors that are the hypothesised antecedents of behaviour change [9, 36]. Our results suggest that this may be ineffective in the presence of misperception of physical activity, which could act as a barrier to behaviour change. The provision of personalised feedback about physical activity following objective measurement might facilitate an accurate perception of physical activity [24, 37, 38]. In turn, this might stimulate intention to increase physical activity, which ultimately may lead to positive changes in behaviour. This pathway is consistent with several health behaviour theories [19, 20] and may be part of the mechanism underlying the effectiveness of pedometers [39, 40]. Currently, there are efforts to utilise the rapidly expanding world of inexpensive measurement devices, smart phones, and tablet computers to objectively measure the physical activity of large numbers of individuals and provide them with instantaneous, personalised feedback [41]. However, studies of the effects of personalised feedback on awareness of physical activity, intention to increase physical activity, and behaviour have been limited by small sample sizes and imprecise outcome measurement, and large randomised controlled trials with objective outcome assessment would advance research in this area [4245].

Strengths of this study include the large population-based sample and the use of a dichotomous measure of self-rated physical activity, a validated and objective measure of actual physical activity, and a range of well-assessed correlates. However, the results of this study should be considered within its limitations. Although our measure of awareness of physical activity represents an improvement in the assessment of the construct, the time frames of the self-rated (the past month) and objective (the subsequent six days and nights) physical activity measures did not overlap. The intent of both measures was to capture habitual behaviour, but measures that have matching or overlapping time frames may be an improvement. It should also be acknowledged that random error or social desirability could have influenced the measurement of self-rated and objective physical activity and may have influenced our assessment of awareness of physical activity. Additionally, we relied on single-item measures of self-rated physical activity, perceived importance, subjective norm, perceived adequacy, and intention to be physically active. A more comprehensive assessment of the psychological antecedents of behaviour change is necessary to fully understand what role psychological variables might play in facilitating awareness of physical activity. Participants were from one location in the United Kingdom and were physically and psychologically healthy. Therefore, the results might not generalise to other settings or to those who are less healthy. Finally, the cross-sectional study design prohibits the establishment of any temporal associations.

Conclusions

In this population-based sample of middle-aged adults, half of those who were inactive misperceived their physical activity by overestimating the amount of activity they engaged in. Overestimation was associated with a lower BMI, higher levels of physical activity according to objective and self-reported measures, lower intention to increase physical activity and response efficacy. These results are in accordance with previous research [1417]. This is significant considering that the present study is the first to assess awareness of physical activity using a dichotomous measure of self-rated physical activity that asked individuals if they were achieving the recommended level of physical activity and compared responses with a precise objective measure. Our results imply that public health messages aimed at promoting physical activity among the general population are unlikely to reach inactive adults who believe their level of physical activity is adequate. Increasing awareness of physical activity and adapting public health messages to underscore that relatively healthy, normal weight and overweight individuals who are moderately active can still garner health benefits from increasing their physical activity to the recommend level may be important in promoting physical activity. This study emphasises the need for more research into the association between misperception of physical activity and behaviour, as well as the effects of feedback on awareness of physical activity, intention to increase physical activity, and behaviour.

Abbreviations

OR:

Odds ratio

CI:

Confidence interval

FAB:

The feedback, awareness and behaviour study

PAL:

Physical activity level

PAEE:

Physical activity energy expenditure

BMI:

Body mass index.

References

  1. Lee I-M, Shiroma EJ, Lobelo F, Puska P, Blair SN, Katzmarzyk PT: Effect of physical inactivity on major non-communicable diseases worldwide: An analysis of burden of disease and life expectancy. Lancet. 2012, 380: 219-29. 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61031-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Department of Health, Physical Activity, Health Improvement and Prevention: At Least Five a Week: Evidence on the Impact of Physical Activity and Its Relationship to Health. 2004, London: A Report from the Chief Medical Officer

    Google Scholar 

  3. FAO/WHO/UNU: Human Energy Requirements: Report of a Joint FAO/WHO/UNU Expert Consultation. 2001, Rome: FAO Food and Nutrition Technical Report Series

    Google Scholar 

  4. Bull FC, The Expert Working Groups: Physical Activity Guidelines in the UK: Review and Recommendations. 2010, Loughborough: School of Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences, Loughborough University

    Google Scholar 

  5. Change4Life: Move More. http://www.nhs.uk/Change4Life/,

  6. NHS Choices: Live Well. http://www.nhs.uk/LiveWell/,

  7. Craig R, Mindell J, Hirani V: Health Survey for England 2008, Volume 1: Physical Activity and Fitness. 2009, London: The Health and Social Care Information Centre, 395-

    Google Scholar 

  8. Tucker JM, Welk GJ, Beyler NK: Physical activity in U.S. Adults: Compliance with the Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans. Am J Prev Med. 2011, 40: 454-61. 10.1016/j.amepre.2010.12.016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Van Sluijs EMF, van Poppel MNM, van Mechelen W: Stage-based lifestyle interventions in primary care: Are they effective?. Am J Prev Med. 2004, 26: 330-43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Hillsdon M, Foster C, Thorogood M: Interventions for promoting physical activity. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2005, 1: CD003180-

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Müller-Riemenschneider F, Reinhold T, Nocon M, Willich SN: Long-term effectiveness of interventions promoting physical activity: A systematic review. Prev Med (Baltim). 2008, 47: 354-68. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.07.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Orrow G, Kinmonth A-L, Sanderson S, Sutton S: Effectiveness of physical activity promotion based in primary care: Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. BMJ. 2012, 344: e1389-10.1136/bmj.e1389.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Bauman AE, Reis RS, Sallis JF, Wells JC, Loos RJF, Martin BW: Correlates of physical activity: Why are some people physically active and others not?. Lancet. 2012, 380: 258-71. 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60735-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Ronda G, Van Assema P, Brug J: Stages of change, psychological factors and awareness of physical activity levels in The Netherlands. Health Promot Int. 2001, 16: 305-14. 10.1093/heapro/16.4.305.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Lechner L, Bolman C, Van Dijke M: Factors related to misperception of physical activity in The Netherlands and implications for health promotion programmes. Health Promot Int. 2006, 21: 104-12. 10.1093/heapro/dal011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Van Sluijs EM, Griffin SJ, Van Poppel MNM: A cross-sectional study of awareness of physical activity: Associations with personal, behavioral and psychosocial factors. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2007, 9: 1-9.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Watkinson C, van Sluijs EM, Sutton S, Hardeman W, Corder K, Griffin SJ: Overestimation of physical activity level is associated with lower BMI: A cross-sectional analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010, 7: 68-10.1186/1479-5868-7-68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Thompson D, Batterham AM, Markovitch D, Dixon NC, Lund AJS, Walhin J-P: Confusion and conflict in assessing the physical activity status of middle-aged men. PLoS One. 2009, 4: e4337-10.1371/journal.pone.0004337.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Ajzen I: The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1991, 50: 179-211. 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Weinstein ND: The precaution adoption process. Heal Psychol. 1988, 7: 355-386.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Corder K, van Sluijs EM, McMinn AM, Ekelund U, Cassidy A, Griffin SJ: Perception versus reality: Awareness of physical activity levels of British children. Am J Prev Med. 2010, 38: 1-8. 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.08.025.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Corder K, van Sluijs EM, Goodyer I, Ridgway CL, Steele RM, Bamber D, Dunn V, Griffin SJ, Ekelund U: Physical activity awareness of British adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011, 165: 603-9. 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.94.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Helmerhorst HJF, Brage S, Warren J, Besson H, Ekelund U: A systematic review of reliability and objective criterion-related validity of physical activity questionnaires. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012, 9: 103-10.1186/1479-5868-9-103.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Watkinson C, van Sluijs EMF, Sutton S, Marteau T, Griffin SJ: Randomised controlled trial of the effects of physical activity feedback on awareness and behaviour in UK adults: The FAB study protocol [ISRCTN92551397]. BMC Public Health. 2010, 10: 144-10.1186/1471-2458-10-144.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Rolfe E, Loos R, Druet C, Stolk R, Ekelund U, Griffin S, Forouhi N, Wareham N, Ong K: Association between birth weight and visceral fat in adults. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010, 92: 347-352. 10.3945/ajcn.2010.29247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Ware J, Kosinski M, Dewey J, Gandek B: How to Score and Interpret Single-Item Health Status Measures: A Manual for Users of the SF-8 Health Survey. 2001, Lincoln, RI: QualityMetric Incorporated

    Google Scholar 

  27. Brage S, Brage N, Franks PW, Ekelund U, Wareham NJ: Reliability and validity of the combined heart rate and movement sensor Actiheart. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2005, 59: 561-70. 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602118.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Brage S, Brage N, Franks PW, Ekelund U, Wong M-Y, Andersen LB, Froberg K, Wareham NJ: Branched equation modeling of simultaneous accelerometry and heart rate monitoring improves estimate of directly measured physical activity energy expenditure. J Appl Physiol. 2004, 96: 343-51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Strath SJ, Brage S, Ekelund U: Integration of physiological and accelerometer data to improve physical activity assessment. Med Sci Sport Exerc. 2005, 37: S563-S571. 10.1249/01.mss.0000185650.68232.3f.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Thompson D, Batterham A, Bock S, Robson C, Stokes K: Assessment of low-to-moderate intensity physical activity thermogenesis in young adults using synchronized heart rate and accelerometry with branched-equation modeling. J Nutr. 2006, 136: 1037-1042.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Besson H, Brage S, Jakes R, Ekelund U, Wareham N: Estimating physical activity energy expenditure, sedentary time, and physical activity intensity by self-report in adults. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010, 91: 106-114. 10.3945/ajcn.2009.28432.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Williams K, Prevost AT, Griffin S, Hardeman W, Hollingworth W, Spiegelhalter D, Sutton S, Ekelund U, Wareham N, Kinmonth AL: The ProActive trial protocol - a randomised controlled trial of the efficacy of a family-based, domiciliary intervention programme to increase physical activity among individuals at high risk of diabetes [ISRCTN61323766]. BMC Public Health. 2004, 4: 48-10.1186/1471-2458-4-48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Sutton S, French DP, Hennings SJ, Mitchell J, Wareham NJ, Griffin S, Hardeman W, Kinmonth AL: Eliciting salient beliefs in research on the theory of planned behaviour: The effect of question wording. Curr Psychol. 2003, 22: 234-251. 10.1007/s12144-003-1019-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Crockett RA, Weinman J, Hankins M, Marteau T: Time orientation and health-related behaviour: measurement in general population samples. Psychol Health. 2009, 24: 333-350. 10.1080/08870440701813030.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. StataCorp: Stata Statistical Software: Release 12. 2011

    Google Scholar 

  36. Hardeman W, Sutton S, Griffin S, Johnston M, White A, Wareham NJ, Kinmonth AL: A causal modelling approach to the development of theory-based behaviour change programmes for trial evaluation. Health Educ Res. 2005, 20: 676-87. 10.1093/her/cyh022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. DiClemente CC, Marinilli AS, Singh M, Bellino LE: The role of feedback in the process of health behavior change. Am J Health Behav. 2001, 25: 217-27. 10.5993/AJHB.25.3.8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Proper KI, van der Beek AJ, Hildebrandt VH, Twisk JWR, van Mechelen W: Short term effect of feedback on fitness and health measurements on self reported appraisal of the stage of change. Br J Sports Med. 2003, 37: 529-34. 10.1136/bjsm.37.6.529.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Bravata D, Smith-Spangler C, Sundaram V, Gienger A, Lin N, Lewis R, Stave C, Olkin I, Sirard J: Using pedometers to increase physical activity and improve health: A systematic review. JAMA. 2007, 298: 2296-2304. 10.1001/jama.298.19.2296.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Tudor-Locke C, Lutes L: Why do pedometers work?: a reflection upon the factors related to successfully increasing physical activity. Sport Med. 2009, 39: 981-93. 10.2165/11319600-000000000-00000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. King AC, Ahn DK, Oliveira BM, Atienza AA, Castro CM, Gardner CD: Promoting physical activity through hand-held computer technology. Am J Prev Med. 2008, 34: 138-42. 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.09.025.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Marcus B, Bock B, Pinto B: Efficacy of an individualised, motivationally-tailored physical activity intervention. Ann Behav Med. 1998, 20: 174-180. 10.1007/BF02884958.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kroeze W, Werkman A, Brug J: A systematic review of randomized trials on the effectiveness of computer-tailored education on physical activity and dietary behaviors. Ann Behav Med. 2006, 31: 205-23. 10.1207/s15324796abm3103_2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Smeets T, Brug J, de Vries H: Effects of tailoring health messages on physical activity. Health Educ Res. 2008, 23: 402-13. 10.1093/her/cyl101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Van Stralen MM, de Vries H, Mudde AN, Bolman C, Lechner L: The long-term efficacy of two computer-tailored physical activity interventions for older adults: main effects and mediators. Heal Psychol. 2011, 30: 442-52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank all staff from the MRC Epidemiology Functional Group Team in particular for study coordination, data collection, physical activity data processing, anthropometry data processing, data management, associated laboratory work, business operations, IT, and research governance. We thank Stephen J Sharp for providing advice on the statistical analyses. We also thank those who participated in the study. This study was funded by The Medical Research Council (MC_U106179474), and was conducted at the MRC Epidemiology Unit in Cambridge, UK. CW and protocol development were supported by the European Union (Integrated Project LSHM-CT-2006-037197 in the Framework Programme 6 of the European-Community). SJG receives support from the Department of Health NIHR Programme Grant funding scheme (RP-PG-0606-1259).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Simon J Griffin.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

JGG, CW, KC, SJG, and EMFvS defined the research question. JGG wrote the statistical analysis plan, conducted the statistical analyses, and drafted the manuscript. JGG, KC, SS, SJG, and EMFvS have contributed to the interpretation of the results and were involved in critical revisions. CW, SS, SJG, and EMFvS contributed to the development of the FAB study and the measures utilised. CW created the study materials and coordinated the study throughout. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access  This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.

The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

To view a copy of this licence, visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Godino, J.G., Watkinson, C., Corder, K. et al. Awareness of physical activity in healthy middle-aged adults: a cross-sectional study of associations with sociodemographic, biological, behavioural, and psychological factors. BMC Public Health 14, 421 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-421

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-421

Keywords