Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Secondary outcomes of a school-based universal resiliency training for adolescents: a cluster randomized controlled trial

Abstract

Background

The study investigated the long-term effectiveness of the adolescent cognitive behavioral resiliency training Op Volle Kracht (OVK) on the secondary outcomes: anxiety symptoms, hopelessness, happiness, life satisfaction, optimism, coping, self-efficacy, and school functioning. In addition, the study analyzed whether the secondary outcomes moderated the intervention effect on depressive symptoms.

Methods

A two-condition (intervention and control) cluster randomized controlled trial was conducted. All adolescents in the 8th grade were eligible to participate, unless they, or their parents, declined their participation. Schools were the unit of randomization. Missing data were imputed and intent to treat analyses were conducted. The results were analyzed using Latent Growth Curve Modeling across the 24-months follow-up period.

Results

The total sample consisted of 1341 adolescents (Mage = 13.91, SD =0.55, 47.3% girls, 83.1% Dutch ethnicity). The intervention and control condition consisted of 634 adolescents from 4 schools and 707 adolescents from 5 schools, respectively. OVK did not have an effect on depression, anxiety, hopelessness, happiness, and life satisfaction, but promoted cognitive coping over the course of the follow-up period. OVK showed small iatrogenic effects on optimism, active coping, social self-efficacy and school grades directly post intervention, but these effects disappeared during the follow-up period. Finally, none of the outcome variables moderated the intervention effect on depressive symptoms.

Conclusions

The universal resiliency training OVK was not effective in this Dutch sample. Implications for research and practice were discussed.

Trial registration number

NTR2879

Peer Review reports

Background

The percentage of adolescents who experience elevated levels of depressive symptoms increases across adolescence from the age of 13, with girls showing a higher increase compared to boys [1, 2]. During this period, the one year point prevalence of a depression disorder increases as well [3], and the lifetime prevalence rises from 8.4% at age 13-14 to 15.4% at age 17-18 [4]. Symptoms of depression negatively influence social, peer, and family functioning [5, 6], and they are associated with future depression disorder [7, 8]. Considering the increase in prevalence and the detrimental consequences, the prevention of depressive symptoms is important.

Depression prevention programs directed at indicated and targeted population are more likely to be effective in preventing depressive symptoms and more likely to yield in larger effect sizes compared to universal approaches (for reviews see [9–11]). However, a recent review indicated that universal depression prevention could also be effective in preventing depressive symptoms [12]. Effective depression prevention programs are often based on Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT). The need to investigate moderators of prevention effectiveness has been emphasized [13], since we have only sparse information on who achieves the best results from the CBT depression prevention programs. In addition, it is relatively unclear whether CBT intervention and prevention programs affect the proposed mechanisms of change in CBT, such as cognitive vulnerabilities, coping skills, and self-efficacy [14].

An example of an effective depression prevention program for indicated and targeted populations is the Penn Resiliency Program (PRP). PRP has shown to reduce depressive symptoms [15], to reduce anxiety symptoms and feelings of hopelessness, and to promote active coping in targeted populations [16]. Besides, in participants at high risk for depression, PRP promoted adaptive explanatory style for positive events, and prevented depression, anxiety and adjustment disorders combined [17]. PRP has shown to be less effective in universal trials and when delivered by other professionals rather than the research team members [15]. However, it has been found that PRP reduces depressive symptoms at post intervention only, implemented as universal prevention and evaluated in a non-randomized trail [18].

Despite these promising results, the Dutch equivalent of PRP, Op Volle Kracht (OVK), has not been found to be effective in preventing depressive symptoms at a two-year follow-up implemented, either as a universal prevention [Tak YR, Lichtwarck-Aschoff A, Gillham JE, Van Zundert RMP, Engels RCME: Universal school-based depression prevention ‘Op Volle Kracht’: A longitudinal cluster randomized controlled trial, Submitted], or at one year follow-up implemented as a selective depression prevention [19]. In addition, adolescent baseline depressive symptom level and gender did not moderate universal prevention effectiveness in these trials. In contrast, a short version of OVK implemented as indicated prevention in girls only groups reduced depressive symptoms in adolescent girls at six months follow-up [20].

The current paper assessed possible moderators of universal depression prevention effectiveness, analyzing whether baseline levels of anxiety, hopelessness, optimism, happiness, life satisfaction, coping or self-efficacy moderated the effectiveness of OVK in preventing depressive symptoms. In addition, the study analyzed the longitudinal effectiveness of OVK in preventing anxiety symptoms and hopelessness, and in promoting optimism, happiness, life satisfaction, adaptive coping, and self-efficacy.

The OVK program is an adapted version of the validated Penn Resiliency Program (PRP) [15, 21]. The content, examples, layout and structure of the program were adapted to suit the needs of Dutch adolescents [22]. In the OVK program, attention is directed at becoming aware of one’s thoughts, to recognize negative thoughts, to challenge those thoughts, and to change these negative thoughts into more realistic and optimistic ones. In addition, adolescents learn to cope with their problems and to improve their social skills and problem solving. Both programs are based on CBT [23], the ABC-model [24], and the hopelessness theory of depression [25]; as is described more elaborately in previous papers on OVK [Tak YR, Lichtwarck-Aschoff A, Gillham JE, Van Zundert RMP, Engels RCME: Universal school-based depression prevention 'Op Volle Kracht': A longitudinal cluster randomized controlled trial, Submitted, [22].

With regard to moderators of the effectiveness of CBT depression prevention, researchers have described that age and depressive symptom levels at baseline are positively associated with prevention effects [11]. In addition, only adolescents reporting high baseline levels of hopelessness showed a decrease in depressive symptoms after completing the prevention program [16], and only adolescents reporting elevated levels of anxiety showed a reduction in depressive symptoms up to 12-months follow-up [26]. Therefore, we hypothesized that OVK will be effective in preventing depressive symptoms among adolescents displaying high baseline levels of anxiety and hopelessness.

Studies that assessed the proposed mechanisms of change of CBT prevention programs have shown contrasting results. It has been found that CBT treatments effectively promote cognitive change in adolescents displaying depression disorder but do not influence coping in adolescents [14]. In contrast, CBT programs implemented as universal and selective prevention and intervention did not affect attributional style or self-esteem, but successfully reduced hopelessness in adolescents [27]. Few studies have tested whether CBT prevention promotes positive outcomes, such as optimism, life satisfaction, and happiness during adolescence. This is important, since adults and adolescents who are more optimistic are more resilient, more satisfied with life [28, 29], and less vulnerable to depression [30]. Besides, higher levels of self-efficacy among adolescents [31] and the use of coping strategies as problem solving and reappraisal are associated with fewer depressive and anxiety symptoms in adults and adolescents [32]. It has been found that CBT implemented as indicated prevention promoted optimistic explanatory style as well as life satisfaction and happiness, and reduced depressive and anxiety symptoms in adolescents up to six months follow-up [33]. In addition, the improvements in explanatory style partly mediated these prevention effects. The current study therefore hypothesized that OVK will promote optimism, happiness, life-satisfaction, coping, and self-efficacy.

Methods

Trial design & randomization

To test the longitudinal effects of the OVK program, a two arm parallel cluster randomized controlled trial was performed with baseline, post, and follow-up assessments at six months, one year, 18 months and at two years [Tak YR, Lichtwarck-Aschoff A, Gillham JE, Van Zundert RMP, Engels RCME: Universal school-based depression prevention 'Op Volle Kracht': A longitudinal cluster randomized controlled trial, Submitted, [22]. Adolescents were the unit of analysis and to minimize contamination between research conditions, we randomized schools to intervention or control condition (lessons as usual) with allocation ratio of 1:1. The randomization was stratified for educational level and was performed by an independent statistician. Schools and adolescents were not blind to condition. The ethics committee of the Faculty of Social Sciences at the Radboud University Nijmegen approved the research protocol and trial design (number 16122010) registered by the Dutch Trial Registration (NTR2879).

To detect a low to medium effect (Cohen’s d =0.20) at one year follow-up on a dichotomous outcome (CDI = > 13 yes/no) while considering 20% attrition over the two year follow-up period, the clustering of participants within schools, and loss of power due to multiple imputation, the study needed to recruit 662 participants per condition (alpha < .05, power = .80). The actual intervention group consisted of 4 schools and 655 adolescents of whom only .05% declined participation (3 out of 655). The actual control group consisted of 5 schools and 735 adolescents of whom only 1.4% (10 out of 735) declined to participate. For participant characteristics, see Table 1.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics

Procedure & participants

All adolescents in 8th grade of secondary schools preparing for vocational up to university level were eligible to participate. The passive consent procedure was used, but parents were free to withdraw their adolescent from the study at any time if the adolescent did not want to participate or if they did not want their son/daughter to participate. Study outcomes were assessed by administering 50-minute questionnaires to adolescents during school hours in the classroom setting. Adolescents who were not present at school were asked by e-mail or mail and subsequently by phone to complete the questionnaire at home. Adolescents who completed the questionnaires at their leisure time at home received a gift voucher of €7.50 per assessment. At one year follow-up, five additional gift vouchers of €20 were allotted for adolescents who were absent during the assessment at school to increase their participation rate. At the two-year follow-up, all participants who completed the assessment received a gift voucher of €7.50.

Intervention

Adolescents in the intervention condition received the program OVK consisting of 16 weekly 50-minute lessons offered during the ‘mentor lesson’ from February 2011 until June 2011. During the mentor lesson, the mentor from each class discusses school related aspects, such as school activities and classroom atmosphere. In the intervention group, classrooms were split to create groups of 10-16 students. In some schools, teachers created these groups to ensure that the children who were most likely to cause trouble were evenly distributed across the OVK intervention groups. The control condition received lessons as usual. The OVK prevention program is based on the Penn Resiliency Program [21]. Adaptations in content, structure, and layout were made to match the interests and culture of the Dutch adolescent population. The first part of the program covers CBT principles and the second part focuses on coping, decision-making and problem solving. During the lessons, adolescents received classical instruction, and practiced the skills by means of role-plays, skits, and discussions. Subsequently, they completed pen and paper assignments in their personal OVK workbook. To foster the internalization and generalization of the skills learned, every lesson included homework assignments. The program included a two-hour booster session, which was delivered after the assessment at six months. During the booster session, adolescents rehearsed the main principles of OVK by watching a music performance about resiliency and by writing a poem about what they learned from OVK (see [22], for a detailed program description). Ten psychologists (1 per group) with varying experience in Cognitive Behavioral Therapy and teaching administered the OVK. One trainer was the principal author of this paper. All OVK group trainers completed a five-day OVK training in advance. Two experienced psychologists who were trained by members of the Penn Resiliency Team provided the OVK training. The number of OVK groups per trainer ranged from five to nine (M =5.67, SD =1.40). Program fidelity was monitored by organizing two meetings for group trainers with the OVK developers and research staff. The program fidelity was measured using self-reports, which were completed by group trainers after every OVK lesson.

Measures

Two independent researchers translated the scales for anxiety, hopelessness, optimism, life satisfaction, happiness and self-efficacy into Dutch; subsequently, they discussed and solved any translation disagreements.

Depressive symptoms

The primary outcome is the level of depressive symptoms measured with the Dutch version of the Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI) [34, 35], which has been shown to be reliable and valid [36]. Depressive symptoms were measured six times, at baseline and every follow-up assessment, up to two years follow-up. For each of the 27 items, adolescents had to indicate, which of the three statements best describes how they felt in the past two weeks. For example, ‘I am sad sometimes’ =0, ‘I am often sad’ =1, and ‘I am sad all the time’ =2. Depressive symptoms sum scores ranged from 0 – 54. The reliability of all assessments was high, with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .84 – .91. Due to ethical considerations, item nine, which measures suicidal thoughts and ideation, was omitted from the questionnaire after the baseline assessment. To facilitate comparison with other studies, the sum scores were adjusted after omitting item nine by multiplying the mean item score times 27.

Anxiety

Adolescents’ anxiety at baseline up to two years follow-up was assessed by the Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS), which has been found to be reliable and valid [37–39]. Adolescents had to indicate whether they agreed, yes =1, no =0, with 28 items included in the scale. Examples of anxiety items are, ‘I am afraid of a lot of things’, or ‘I am nervous’. The anxiety scores ranged from 0 – 28. Reliability was good to excellent on all assessments, with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .88 – .92.

Hopelessness

Hopelessness at baseline up to two years follow-up was assessed with the Beck Hopelessness Scale (BHS), which has shown to have good reliability and validity [40, 41]. The scale consists of 20 items, for example: ‘I look forward to the future with hope and enthusiasm’. Adolescents had to indicate whether the item was ‘true’ =1 for them or ‘not true’ =0. The hopelessness scores ranged from 0 – 20. Acceptable to good levels of reliability were obtained on all assessments, with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .73 – .81.

Happiness

Adolescents’ level of happiness at baseline up to two years follow-up was measured with the Cantril Ladder [42]. Adolescents were asked to mark the number that best corresponds with how they felt about their life at that moment. The ladder ranged from 0 = ‘very unhappy’, to 10 = ‘very happy’. This one item measure has been found to be reliable and valid [43].

Life satisfaction

Life satisfaction at baseline up to two years follow-up was measured with the Students Life Satisfaction Scale (SLSS) [44]. This is a reliable and valid measure [45], and consists of seven items measured on a 6-point Likert-scale ranging from ‘strongly disagree’ =1, ‘to strongly agree’ =6. The sample items are: ‘My life is going well’ and ‘I have what I want in life’. Life satisfaction scores ranged from 7 – 42. Cronbach’s alpha ranged from .86 – .88, which indicates good reliability across the study.

Optimism

The level of optimism at baseline up to two years follow-up was assessed by the Life-Orientation Test Revised (LOT-R), which is a reliable and valid measure [46]. The scale consists of six optimism items and four filler items measured on a 5-point Likert-scale, ‘strongly disagree’ =0, ‘to strongly agree’ =4. An example item is, ‘In uncertain times, I usually expect the best’. Optimism scores ranged from 0 – 24. Reliability was acceptable on all assessments, with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .63 – .75.

Coping

Adolescents’ coping at baseline up to 18 months follow-up was measured with the Dutch version of the Children Coping Strategies Checklist-Revised (CCSC-R), which is a reliable and valid measure [47]. Adolescents had to respond to 54 statements that all started with, ‘If I have a problem, I…’, followed by different scenarios, such as ‘Tell others how I like to solve the problem’. Adolescents had to indicate how often they responded in this way and choose one of the 4 responses, ‘almost never’ =1, ‘sometimes’ =2, ‘often’ =3, ‘almost always’ =4. The questionnaire consists of five subscales, active coping (12 items), for example, ‘Do something to make things better’; cognitive coping (12 items), for example, ‘Tell yourself that you know what to do’; distraction coping (nine items), for example, ‘Play sports’; avoidance coping (12 items), for example, ‘Just forget about it’; and seeking support coping (nine items), for example, ‘Tell others how you feel about the problem’. All coping scales achieved good reliability on all assessments. Cronbach’s alpha for active coping ranged from .88 –.91. Cronbach’s alpha for cognitive coping ranged from .87 – .91. Cronbach’s alpha for distraction coping ranged from .74 – .79. Cronbach’s alpha for avoidance coping ranged from .73 – .85. Cronbach’s alpha for support seeking coping ranged from .91 – .93.

Self-efficacy

Academic, social, and emotional self-efficacy was assessed with the Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (SEQ) [48] at baseline up to the 18 months follow-up. The measure has been found to be reliable and valid. This questionnaire consists of three subscales: academic, social, and emotional self-efficacy, each measured with seven items. Adolescents had to indicate on each item how well they thought they could perform the task or the skill on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = not at all to 5 = very well. Subscale sum scores ranged from 7 – 35. An example item assessing academic self-efficacy is, ‘How well do you succeed in finishing all your homework every day?’ Cronbach’s alpha ranged from .86 – .90. A sample item of social self-efficacy is, ‘How well do you succeed in staying friends with other children?’ Cronbach’s alpha ranged from .79 – .91. A sample item of emotional self-efficacy is, ‘How well do you succeed in becoming calm again when you are very scared?’ The subscale showed good reliability on all assessments, with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .85 – .91.

School grades

Adolescents had to report the last grade they obtained on Dutch, English, Mathematics, Environmental Studies, Economics and Biology tests. Since not all adolescents took all courses, the mean grade was calculated for the subjects that they took. Mean grades ranged from 0 – 10.

Classroom atmosphere

Three items were constructed to assess the relationship with classmates: ‘My classmates like to be together’, ‘My classmates are friendly and helpful’, ‘My classmates accept me’. Adolescents could answer on a 5-point liker scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = to strongly agree. Subscale sum scores ranged from 5 – 15. The scale was reliable, with Cronbach’s Alpha ranging from .76 – .90 across assessments.

Alcohol consumption

Adolescents had to indicate how many alcoholic beverages they drank during the past four weeks. Since the variable showed a largely skewed distribution and transformations did not correct the skewness, the answers were dichotomized: I drank alcohol in the past four weeks, 1 = yes, 0 = no.

Current smoking

Adolescents reported whether they currently smoked: Yes =1, no =0.

Truancy

Adolescents reported the number of lessons they skipped. Since this variable had a largely skewed distribution and transforming the data did not improve the skewness, the data was dichotomized: Truancy yes =1, truancy no =0.

Strategy of analyses

Differences at baseline between intervention and control condition in gender, age, school level, ethnicity, and outcome variables were assessed with logistic regression analysis and controlled for in subsequent analyses. Predictors of attrition across follow-up assessments were estimated with logistic regression analysis. For educational level, two dummy variables were created: Lower education consisted of pre-vocational secondary education (PVSE) =1 (which is in Dutch: VMBO) versus higher general secondary education (HGSE) =0 (HAVO), and pre-university education (PUE) =0 (VWO) [49]. Middle education consisted of HGSE =1 versus PVSE =0, and PUE =0.

The longitudinal effectiveness of OVK was analyzed based on the intent-to-treat framework (N =1,341). Missing data were imputed 20 times, auxiliary variables were included in the models, and the imputation was done separately for the control and intervention groups. These decisions were made since imputing the model several times and including auxiliary variables leads to more accurate standard error estimates [50]. Imputations were done by multiple imputation using the predictive mean matching method in SPSS 19. The intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) for all outcome measures was calculated and ranged from .00 to .14 with a mean of .02, which implies that 2% of the variance in the outcomes was explained by school-related aspects. Therefore, all LGCM analyses controlled for the clustering of participants in schools.

To test the longitudinal effectiveness of OVK on depressive symptoms and secondary and ancillary outcomes, Latent Growth Curve Modeling (LGCM) was performed using Mplus [51]. First, single growth curves were assessed from post assessment up to 18 months or two years follow-up, depending on the number of follow-up assessments available for the outcome variable. This was done by estimating the post assessment level (intercept) and change over the follow-up period (slope) for all outcomes. Second, the model was assessed including study condition as predictor as well as the control variables age, ethnicity, school level, gender, and the baseline level of the outcome variable. Linear and quadratic slopes were tested, but since the variance of the quadratic slope was not significant for any of the outcomes, the quadratic slope was not included in the models. The model was considered to have a good fit when the fit indices were RMSEA < .05, CFI > .90. Since the chi-square value is less reliable with large sample sizes, it was not reported [52]. The analyses aimed to assess whether the secondary outcomes moderated the relation between the preventive effect of OVK and depressive symptoms over the two-year follow-up period. In these analyses, we controlled for multiple testing by applying a Bonferroni correction. The analyses for the secondary and ancillary outcomes were exploratory. Therefore, to minimize the risk on a Type II error, no Bonferroni correction was applied.

Results

From the 79 schools that were approached to participate in this project, 12 schools showed initial interest, and nine schools eventually agreed to participate. Four schools were allocated to the intervention condition (655 adolescents) and five schools to the control condition (735 adolescents). The intervention group consisted of 51 OVK groups of 10-16 students (Mode =12). Only 1.0% (13 of 1390) of the potential participants declined participation at baseline, and an additional 2.6% (36 of 1390) did not complete the baseline assessment for various reasons (see Figure 1 for the flow diagram). Attrition rates were low; the percentage of participants completing the questionnaire at baseline, post, six months, one year, 18 months, and two-year follow-up were 96.5%, 89.4%, 89.3%, 83.7%, 77.4%, and 84.5%, respectively. Overall, 1341 participants who completed baseline assessments, 634 from the intervention condition and 707 from the control condition, were included in the analyses.

Figure 1
figure 1

Participant flow.

Baseline characteristics are presented in Table 1. Some variables were distributed unevenly across conditions and were controlled for in subsequent analyses: PVSE versus HGSE and PUE (OR =0.31, 95% CI =0.18 - 0.52, p = .000), age (OR =1.24, 95% CI =1.01 - 1.53, p = .045), and ethnicity (OR =0.62, 95% CI =0.46 - 0.83, p = .002). Attrition analyses showed that across all follow-up assessments, girls (OR =0.50, 95% CI =0.30 - 0.83, p = .007) and adolescents in the control condition (OR =0.59, 95% CI =0.36 - 0.96, p = .033) were more likely to decline further participation. In addition, adolescents in PVSE were less likely to decline further study participation compared to HGSE and PUE (OR =0.19, 95% CI =0.07 - 0.51, p = .001). Moreover, adolescents in HGSE were less likely to decline further study participation compared to PVSE and PUE (OR =0.19, 95% CI =0.01 - 0.38, p = .000).

The OVK prevention was delivered according to the manual, since the treatment fidelity was 80%, according to the group trainers. Adolescents participated on average in 14-15 out of 16 lessons, and 67.8% (n =430) of the intervention group attended the booster session that was provided after the assessment at six months.

Moderation of secondary outcomes on OVK effectiveness on depressive symptoms

OVK was not effective in preventing depressive symptoms (Table 2). For a more elaborative discussion about the effectiveness of OVK on depressive symptoms, see [Tak YR, Lichtwarck-Aschoff A, Gillham JE, Van Zundert RMP, Engels RCME: Universal school-based depression prevention 'Op Volle Kracht': A longitudinal cluster randomized controlled trial, Submitted]. Moderation analyses revealed that none of the secondary outcome variables, anxiety, hopelessness, happiness, life satisfaction, optimism, coping, self-efficacy, moderated the effect of OVK on depressive symptoms. A Bonferroni correction was applied since 13 tests were performed. Alpha’s were considered significant if smaller than .05 / 13 = .0038.

Table 2 Standardized estimates of predictors, and control variables, and moderation of condition for the intercept and slope of depressive symptoms across the two-year follow-up period

Latent growth curve model findings for primary, secondary, and ancillary outcomes

First, the models for depression (primary outcome), anxiety, hopelessness, happiness, life satisfaction, optimism, coping, self-efficacy (secondary outcomes), grades, classroom atmosphere, alcohol use, smoking, and truancy (ancillary outcomes) were tested without the predictor and control variables in Model 1. For all outcome variables, model 1 had a reasonable to good fit, as can be seen in Table 3. The variance of the intercept was significant for all outcomes, indicating individual differences at post-assessment on the outcome variables. For avoidance coping, alcohol use during the past 4-weeks, and current smoking, the variance of the slope was not significant, indicating no differences among participants in the rate of change on these variables across the follow-up period. Therefore, LGCM was not conducted for these outcomes.

Table 3 Model 1 for depressive symptoms, secondary outcomes, and ancillary outcomes

Second, for the primary, secondary, and ancillary outcomes, Model 2 included study condition as predictor and age, ethnicity, school level, gender, and the baseline level of the outcome variable as control variables. As can be seen in Table 4, OVK condition showed an iatrogenic effect on the intercept (the post assessment) of optimism, active coping, social self-efficacy, and school grades. This indicates that at post-assessment (immediately following the intervention), adolescents in the intervention condition reported less optimism (intervention group: M =14.65, SD =3.90; control group: M =15.80, SD =4.07; Cohen’s d = .29), less active coping (intervention group: M =28.87, SD =7.37; control group: M =30.23, SD =6.94; Cohen’s d = .19), less social self-efficacy (intervention group: M =25.63, SD =5.67; control group: M =26.58, SD =4.21; Cohen’s d = .19), and lower school grades (intervention group: M =7.07, SD =1.70; control group: M =7.22, SD =1.40; Cohen’s d = .09). In contrast, OVK condition was positively associated with the slope of active coping and social self-efficacy, indicating that from post assessment to 18 months follow-up, the iatrogenic effect of active coping and social self-efficacy diminished, since the intervention group showed a larger increase compared to the control group during this period. By means of separate regression analyses in which the control variables were included, it was analyzed whether condition predicted the final assessment. This showed that at 18 months follow-up, the intervention group did not differ in active coping (β =0.036, SE =0.045, p = .415; Cohen’s d = .11; intervention group: M =29.69, SD =6.67; control group: M =28.95, SD =6.89) or on social self-efficacy (β =0.060, SE =0.052, p = .253; Cohen’s d = .12; intervention group: M =25.78, SD =5.05; control group: M =25.17, SD =5.33) compared to the control group.

Table 4 Model 2: Standardized estimates of OVK condition for the intercept and slope of secondary and ancillary outcomes across the two-year follow-up period

Although OVK condition was not associated with the slope of school grades or optimism across follow-up, adolescents did not differ significantly in school grades at two-year follow-up (β = -0.068, SE =0.043, p = .112; Cohen’s d = .17; intervention group: M =6.56, SD =1.01; control group: M =6.73, SD =0.99) or in optimism at two-year follow-up (β = -0.076, SE =0.043, p = .078; Cohen’s d = .18; intervention group: M =15.40, SD =4.27; control group: M =16.14, SD =4.10) compared to the control group. In addition, OVK condition was positively associated with the slope of cognitive coping from post assessment up to 18 months follow-up, which indicated that OVK promoted the use of cognitive coping during the follow-up period. At 18 months follow-up, the intervention group reported significantly more cognitive coping compared to the control group (β =0.095, SE =0.031, p = .002; Cohen’s d = .23; intervention group: M =28.39, SD =6.50; control group: M =26.84, SD =7.04).

Discussion

Overall, the OVK depression prevention and resiliency training tested in a universal adolescent sample was not effective in preventing depression, anxiety, and hopelessness and in promoting optimism, life satisfaction, happiness, adaptive coping and self-efficacy. Baseline levels of secondary outcomes did not moderate the OVK effectiveness in preventing depressive symptoms. It should be noted that immediately following OVK, the intervention group reported less optimism, less active coping, less social self-efficacy, and lower school grades, but these effect sizes were considered small or trivial, and these iatrogenic effects diminished over the follow-up period. Besides these iatrogenic effects, OVK had a small positive effect on cognitive coping, since adolescents in the intervention group reported a stronger increase in cognitive coping over the follow-up period, and reported significantly higher levels of cognitive coping at 18 months follow-up, compared to the control group.

Factors related to study design and characteristics of the OVK program might explain why OVK was not successful in preventing negative development and promoting positive development. In previous studies, the PRP program has never been tested in a strictly universal trial with a randomized design. As discussed in the primary outcome paper of OVK [Tak YR, Lichtwarck-Aschoff A, Gillham JE, Van Zundert RMP, Engels RCME: Universal school-based depression prevention 'Op Volle Kracht': A longitudinal cluster randomized controlled trial, Submitted], the universal trials in which a preventive effect of PRP on depressive symptoms is reported included less than 20% of the addressed population, included participants through active consent, and provided the program after school hours [17, 53, 54]. In the current trial, more than 96% of the addressed sample participated in the OVK program through passive consent, and OVK was provided as a regular lesson during school hours. Adolescents who chose to participate in a study in their leisure time might differ from adolescents in the current trial on various characteristics as motivation to participate, psychological burden and support from parents, although mean depressive symptom levels of adolescents in the current trial did not differ from the ones in these PRP trials.

Second, characteristics of the sample could partly explain why OVK was not effective as a prevention program. The adolescents in the current study did not actively chose to participate in the OVK program, and they reported on average few depressive and anxiety symptoms, low levels of hopelessness and low levels of maladaptive coping. Therefore, OVK might have made them aware of their own negative cognitions and shortcomings in coping, since OVK focuses on coping with stressful daily events and changing negative thoughts into realistic ones. This awareness might have made adolescents feel more insecure about their own capacities. This might have resulted in a small but negative effect on optimism, social self-efficacy, active coping strategies, and even lower grades. Some other universal studies have also reported iatrogenic effects of universal CBT-prevention on depressive symptoms [19, 55]. Although the effect sizes reported in the current study were small or trivial, and no differences were apparent at two years follow-up, these findings argue against implementing OVK as universal prevention or resiliency training.

Answering the question about whether universal depression prevention in general is an effective way to prevent depression is beyond the scope of this article. In a recent review by Merry et al. (2012), it was concluded that although the effect sizes are smaller compared to indicated and targeted programs, universal programs are effective in preventing depressive symptoms [12]. In this review, the analysis of the effectiveness of universal prevention also included trials in which only 13 – 53% of the addressed population participated [53, 56] and included trials, which were directed at adolescents at an increased risk of depression [57, 58]. Inclusion of these trials makes the statement that universal depression prevention is effective less convincing. However, some universal trials that include more than 70% of the addressed population are effective in preventing depressive symptoms [59, 60]. To conclude, evidence regarding the effectiveness of universal trials is inconsistent. Therefore, identifying the effective components and mechanisms of these effective universal programs, i.e., the processes they address, the ways in which they engage adolescents, and the way they include the environment, is still very important.

An important aim of the current study was to identify moderators of OVK effectiveness on depressive symptoms and to investigate whether OVK had an effect on the proposed mediators of change in CBT based depression prevention. In the current study, none of the secondary outcomes moderated the prevention effectiveness on depressive symptoms. However, considering the characteristics of this universal sample, we see that not only do all mean scores of the secondary outcomes fall within the healthy range, there is also very little variation in these scores. Only very few adolescents report high levels of anxiety or hopelessness, for example. This restricted range in the secondary outcome measures makes it difficult to find any potential role of moderating factors. Future studies should focus on and investigate potential moderators of prevention effectiveness in order to improve depression prevention.

With respect to the possible mechanisms of change in CBT prevention, adolescents in the OVK condition reported a larger increase in cognitive coping across 18-month follow-up period compared to the control group. The focus of OVK is on changing cognitions, that is, on replacing negative thoughts by those that are more realistic. This might explain why the effects were found for the cognitive coping and not for the other coping strategies, which is in line with several other studies see [14]. Besides cognitive coping, OVK did not affect change in other possible mediators. This general lack of effect might be explained by the fact that adolescents in our sample were not highly motivated to fully engage in the program and to incorporate the skills learned in their own daily lives. The adolescents mentioned that they thought that the program was too long. They indicated that they would have preferred to have more time to discuss their own negative experiences, and they wanted to discuss the positive situations they had experienced as well. Motivation and engagement could be enhanced by adapting the program to better suit the individual needs of the adolescents, such as using adolescents own daily hassles and positive experiences in all exercises, as was also mentioned by Gillham et al. (2006) [17]. It has been shown that a certain degree of flexibility in applying treatment modules – rather that rigid adherence to the program protocol – is associated with better outcomes in CBT based interventions [61].

Strengths and limitations

The main strength of this study is its randomized controlled design executed in a large sample. Second, a high percentage of the universal sample addressed for participation actually participated at baseline, and a small dropout percentage across the follow-up assessments was obtained, which increases the validity of our study. Third, the effect of OVK on possible mediators of the effectiveness of CBT-based prevention was examined. Fourth, the current study tested whether other psychological constructs, i.e. anxiety, hopelessness and optimism moderate the OVK effectiveness on depressive symptoms.

Besides its strengths, this study has some limitations. First, the current study was not able to test the effectiveness of OVK on attributions or cognitions. Future studies should fill this gap, since in CBT-based programs, the aim is to change negative cognitions [23]. Second, the measure used for happiness consisted of a single item, which might be less reliable and valid in comparison with a multiple item questionnaire, although this measure was used reliably in previous research [43]. Third, the LOT-R that assessed optimism showed low but acceptable reliability in the current sample, and it has shown comparable levels of reliability in other studies [29, 62]. It seems that the LOT-R is measuring two separate constructs, optimism and pessimism, since both constructs explain unique variance in well-being and are often only moderately correlated [62]. This could explain the low overall reliability of the optimism scale. In light of this low reliability, the results should be confirmed before firm conclusions can be drawn. The fourth limitation is that a recall bias might have been present in the way school grades were assessed. Adolescents had to self-report their latest grades for each subject by recall. Future studies should use official grades provided by schools. Besides, the latest grade does not always reflect the average grade for a specific subject. Therefore, the results concerning school grades should be interpreted with caution. Using self-reports to assess alcohol, smoking, and truancy may be a limitation as well, since adolescents might feel reluctant to report using those substances or skipping a class. However, previous researchers have shown that self-report is a reliable and valid way to assess smoking [63] and alcohol use [64]. Moreover, adolescents were assured their answers would be handled confidentially. A final shortcoming of the present study is that the current sample included a smaller percentage of adolescents belonging to minority groups and adolescents with a low educational background than is present in the Dutch population. Therefore, generalizing the results of this study to the entire Dutch population is not possible.

Conclusions

OVK was not effective in preventing depressive and anxiety symptoms or in preventing hopelessness and increasing optimism, happiness, life-satisfaction, and self-efficacy. OVK showed small iatrogenic short-term effects on optimism, active coping, social self-efficacy, and school grades, but these disappeared over time. In addition, OVK showed a small positive long-term effect on cognitive coping. None of the secondary outcome variables moderated the intervention effect on depressive symptoms. The importance of investigating the effectiveness of prevention programs before implementing them is highlighted. Based on this study, it can be concluded that OVK should not be implemented as universal prevention in its current form, although future research should affirm or attest these statements before firm conclusions can be drawn.

References

  1. Twenge JM, Nolen-Hoeksema S: Age, gender, race, socioeconomic status, and birth cohort differences on the Children's Depression Inventory: A meta-analysis. J Abnormal Psych. 2002, 111 (4): 578-588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Hankin BL, Abramson LY: Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability-transactional stress theory. Psychol Bull. 2001, 127 (6): 773-796.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Costello EJ, Copeland WE, Angold A: Trends in psychopathology across the adolescent years: What changes when children become adolescents, and when adolescents become adults?. J Child Psycho Psychiatry. 2011, 52 (10): 1015-1025. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02446.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Merikangas KR, He JP, Brody D, Fisher PW, Bourdon K, Koretz DS: Prevalence and Treatment of Mental Disorders Among US Children in the 2001-2004 NHANES. Pediatrics. 2010, 125 (1): 75-81. 10.1542/peds.2008-2598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Jaycox LH, Stein BD, Paddock S, Miles JNV, Chandra A, Meredith LS, Tanielian T, Hickey S, Burnam MA: Impact of teen depression on academic, social, and physical functioning. Pediatrics. 2009, 124 (4): 596-605. 10.1542/peds.2008-3348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Verboom CE, Sijtsema JJ, Verhulst FC, Penninx BWJH, Ormel J: Longitudinal Associations Between Depressive Problems, Academic Performance, and Social Functioning in Adolescent Boys and Girls. Dev Psychol. 2014, 50 (1): 247-257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Roza SJ, Hofstra MB, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC: Stable prediction of mood and anxiety disorders based on behavioral and emotional problems in childhood: A 14-year follow-up during childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood. Am J Psychiat. 2003, 160 (12): 2116-2121. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Seeley JR, Stice E, Rohde P: Screening for Depression Prevention: Identifying Adolescent Girls at High Risk for Future Depression. J Abnormal Psych. 2009, 118 (1): 161-170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Calear A, Christensen H: Systematic review of school-based prevention and early intervention programs for depression. J Adolesc. 2010, 33 (3): 429-438. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.07.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Horowitz JL, Garber J: The prevention of depressive symptoms in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review. J Consult Clin Psych. 2006, 74 (3): 401-415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Stice E, Shaw H, Bohon C, Marti CN, Rohde P: A meta-analytic review of depression prevention programs for children and adolescents: Factors that predict magnitude of intervention effects. J Consult Clin Psych. 2009, 77 (3): 486-503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Merry SN, Hetrick SE, Cox GR, Brudevold-Iversen T, Bir JJ, McDowell H: Cochrane Review: Psychological and educational interventions for preventing depression in children and adolescents. Evid-Based Child Health. 2012, 7 (5): 1409-1685. 10.1002/ebch.1867.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Garber J: Depression in children and adolescents - Linking risk research and prevention. Am J Prev Med. 2006, 31 (6): 104-125. 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.07.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Chu BC, Harrison TL: Disorder-specific Effects of CBT for Anxious and Depressed Youth: A Meta-analysis of Candidate Mediators of Change. Clin Child Fam Psych. 2007, 10 (4): 352-372. 10.1007/s10567-007-0028-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Brunwasser SM, Gillham JE, Kim ES: A meta-analytic review of the Penn Resiliency Program's effect on depressive symptoms. J Consult Clin Psych. 2009, 77 (6): 1042-1054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Gillham JE, Reivich KJ, Brunwasser SM, Freres DR, Chajon ND, Kash-MacDonald VM, Chaplin TM, Abenavoli RM, Matlin SL, Gallop RJ, Seligman MEP: Evaluation of a group cognitive-behavioral depression prevention program for young adolescents: A randomized effectiveness trial. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2012, 41 (5): 621-639. 10.1080/15374416.2012.706517.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Gillham JE, Hamilton J, Freres DR, Patton K, Gallop R: Preventing depression among early adolescents in the primary care setting: A randomized controlled study of the Penn Resiliency Program. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2006, 34 (2): 203-219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Challen AR, Machin SJ, Gillham JE: The UK Resilience Programme: A school-based universal non-randomized pragmatic controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psych. 2014, 82 (1): 75-89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Kindt KCM, Kleinjan M, Janssens JMAM, Scholte RHJ: Evaluation of a school-based depression prevention program among adolescents from low-income areas: A randomized controlled effectiveness trial. Int J Env Res Public Health. 2014, 11 (5): 5273-5293. 10.3390/ijerph110505273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Wijnhoven ALMW, Creemers DHM, Vermulst AA, Scholte RJ, Engels CRME: Randomized controlled trial testing the effectiveness of a depression prevention program (‘Op Volle Kracht’) among adolescent girls with elevated depressive symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psych. 2014, 42 (2): 217-228. 10.1007/s10802-013-9773-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Gillham JE, Reivich KJ, Jaycox LH, Seligman MEP: Prevention of depressive symptoms in schoolchildren: Two-year follow-up. Psychol Sci. 1995, 6 (6): 343-351. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00524.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Tak YR, Van Zundert RMP, Kuijpers RCWM, Van Vlokhoven BS, Rensink HFW, Engels RCME: A randomized controlled trial testing the effectiveness of a universal school-based depression prevention program 'Op Volle Kracht' in the Netherlands. BMC Public Health. 2012, 12: 21-10.1186/1471-2458-12-21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Beck AT: Cognitive theory and the emotional disorders. 1976, New York: International Universities Press

    Google Scholar 

  24. Ellis A: Reasons and emotion in psychotherapy. 1962, New York: Lyle Stuart

    Google Scholar 

  25. Hankin BL, Abramson LY, Siler M: A prospective test of the hopelessness theory of depression in adolescence. Cognitive Ther Res. 2001, 25 (5): 607-632. 10.1023/A:1005561616506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Lowry-Webster HM, Barrett PM, Dadds MR: A universal prevention trial of anxiety and depressive symptomatology in childhood: Preliminary data from an Australian study. Behav Change. 2001, 18 (1): 36-50. 10.1375/bech.18.1.36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Mychailyszyn MP, Brodman DM, Read KL, Kendall PC: Cognitive-Behavioral School-Based Interventions for Anxious and Depressed Youth: A Meta-Analysis of Outcomes. Clin Psycho: Sci Prac. 2012, 19 (2): 129-153. 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2012.01279.x.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Seligman MEP, Rashid T, Parks AC: Positive psychotherapy. Am Psychol. 2006, 61 (8): 774-788.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Extremera N, Duran A, Rey L: Perceived emotional intelligence and dispositional optimism-pessimism: Analyzing their role in predicting psychological adjustment among adolescents. Pers Indiv Differ. 2007, 42 (6): 1069-1079. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Southwick SM, Vythilingam M, Charney DS: The psychobiology of depression and resilience to stress: Implications for prevention and treatment. Annu Rev Clin Psycho. 2005, 1: 255-291. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Muris P: Relationships between self-efficacy and symptoms of anxiety disorders and depression in a normal adolescent sample. Pers Indiv Differ. 2002, 32 (2): 337-348. 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00027-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S: Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010, 30 (2): 217-237. 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Seligman MEP, Schulman P, Tryon AM: Group prevention of depression and anxiety symptoms. Behav Res Ther. 2007, 45 (6): 1111-1126. 10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Kovacs M: The Children’s Depression, Inventory (CDI). Psychopharmacol Bull. 1985, 21: 995-998.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Timbremont B, Braet C, Roelofs J: Children’s Depression Inventory: Handleiding. 2008, Amsterdam: Pearson

    Google Scholar 

  36. Evers A, Braak MSL, Frima RM, van Vliet-Mulder JC: COTAN Documentatie. 2009–2011, Amsterdam: Boom test uitgevers

    Google Scholar 

  37. Reynolds CR, Paget KD: National normative and reliability data for the Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale. Sch Psychol Rev. 1983, 12: 324-336.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Reynolds CR, Richmond BO: What I think and feel: A revised measure of children's manifest anxiety (Reprinted from Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, vol 6, pg 271-280, 1978). J Abnorm Child Psych. 1997, 25 (1): 15-20. 10.1023/A:1025751206600.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Reynolds CR, Richmond BO: What I Think and Feel - Revised Measure of Childrens Manifest Anxiety. J Abnorm Child Psych. 1978, 6 (2): 271-280. 10.1007/BF00919131.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Beck AT, Weissman A, Lester D, Trexler L: Measurement of Pessimism - Hopelessness Scale. J Consult Clin Psych. 1974, 42 (6): 861-865.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Steed L: Further Validity and Reliability Evidence for Beck Hopelessness Scale Scores in a Nonclinical Sample. Educ Psychol Meas. 2001, 61 (2): 303-316. 10.1177/00131640121971121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Cantril H: The intensity of an attitude. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 1946, 41 (2): 129-135.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Abdel-Khalek A: Measuring happiness with a single-item scale. Social Behav Personality. 2006, 34 (2): 139-150. 10.2224/sbp.2006.34.2.139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Huebner ES: Initial Development of the Student's Life Satisfaction Scale. Sch Psychol Int. 1991, 12 (3): 231-240. 10.1177/0143034391123010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Seligson JL, Huebner ES, Valois RF: Preliminary validation of the Brief Multidimensional Students' Life Satisfaction Scale (BMSLSS). Soc Indic Res. 2003, 61: 121-145. 10.1023/A:1021326822957.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW: Distinguishing Optimism from Neuroticism (and Trait Anxiety, Self-Mastery, and Self-Esteem) - a Reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994, 67 (6): 1063-1078.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Boo GM, Wicherts JM: Assessing Cognitive and Behavioral Coping Strategies in Children. Cogn Ther Res. 2007, 33 (1): 1-20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Muris P: A brief questionnaire for measuring self-efficacy in youths. J Psychopathol Behav. 2001, 23 (3): 145-149. 10.1023/A:1010961119608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Secondary education. http://www.government.nl/issues/education/secondary-education,

  50. Graham JW: Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009, 60 (1): 549-576. 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Muthén LK, Muthén BO: Mplus User’s Guide. 1998–2010, Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén, 6

    Google Scholar 

  52. Raykov T, Marcoulides GA: A first course in structural equation modelling. 2006, New York: Taylor & Francis Group, 2

    Google Scholar 

  53. Chaplin TM, Gillham JE, Reivich K, Elkon AGL, Samuels B, Freres DR, Winder B, Seligman MEP: Depression prevention for early adolescent girls - a pilot study of all girls versus co-ed groups. J Early Adolescence. 2006, 26 (1): 110-126. 10.1177/0272431605282655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Gillham JE, Reivich KJ, Freres DR, Chaplin TM, Shatte AJ, Samuels B, Elkon AGL, Litzinger S, Lascher M, Gallop R, Seligman MEP: School-based prevention of depressive symptoms: A randomized controlled study of the effectiveness and specificity of the Penn Resiliency Program. J Consult Clin Psych. 2007, 75 (1): 9-19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Pössel P, Martin NC, Garber J, Hautzinger M: A randomized controlled trial of a cognitive-behavioral program for the prevention of depression in adolescents compared with nonspecific and no-Intervention control conditions. J Couns Psychol. 2013, 60 (3): 432-438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Cutuli JJ, Chaplin TM, Gillham JE, Reivich KJ, Seligman MEP: Preventing co-occurring depression symptoms in adolescents with conduct problems: The Penn Resiliency Program. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006, 1094 (1): 282-286. 10.1196/annals.1376.035.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Cardemil EV, Reivich KJ, Seligman MEP: The prevention of depressive symptoms in low-income minority middle school students. Prevention & Treatment. 2002, 5 (1): No Pagination Specified

    Google Scholar 

  58. Roberts CM, Kane R, Bishop B, Cross D, Fenton J, Hart B: The prevention of anxiety and depression in children from disadvantaged schools. Behav Res Ther. 2010, 48 (1): 68-73. 10.1016/j.brat.2009.09.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Pössel P, Seemann S, Hautzinger M: Impact of comorbidity in prevention of adolescent depressive symptoms. J Couns Psychol. 2008, 55 (1): 106-117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Merry SN, McDowell H, Wild CJ, Bir J, Cunliffe R: A randomized placebo-controlled trial of a school-based depression prevention program. J Am Acad Child Psy. 2004, 43 (5): 538-547. 10.1097/00004583-200405000-00007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Weisz JR, Chorpita BF, Palinkas LA, Schoenwald SK, Miranda J, Bearman SK, Daleiden EL, Ugueto AM, Ho AY, Martin J, Gray J, Alleyne A, Langer DA, Southam-Gerow MA, Gibbons RD, Research Network on Youth Mental Health: Testing Standard and Modular Designs for Psychotherapy Treating Depression, Anxiety, and Conduct Problems in Youth. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2012, 69 (3): 274-282. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Creed PA, Patton W, Bartrum D: Multidimensional Properties of the Lot-R: Effects of Optimism and Pessimism on Career and Well-Being Related Variables in Adolescents. Journal Career Asses. 2002, 10 (1): 42-61. 10.1177/1069072702010001003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Dolcini MM, Adler NE, Lee P, Bauman KE: An assessment of the validity of adolescent self-reported smoking using three biological indicators. Nicotine Tob Res. 2003, 5 (4): 473-483. 10.1080/1462220031000118586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Brener ND, Kann L, McManus T, Kinchen SA, Sundberg EC, Ross JG: Reliability of the 1999 Youth Risk Behavior Survey Questionnaire. J Adolesc Health. 2002, 31 (4): 336-342. 10.1016/S1054-139X(02)00339-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

We want to thank the schools and all adolescents who participated in this research as well as the OVK trainers, the developers of OVK, and all the students who assisted with the data collection.

Funding

ZonMw, Behavioural Science Institute, Trimbos-Institute.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yuli R Tak.

Additional information

Competing interests

A conflict of interest for Prof. RCMEE should be reported, since he has become CEO of the Trimbos-Institute in January 2014. Dr. MK transferred to the Trimbos-Institute in September 2014. However the study and data analysis were conducted before 2014. The other authors have no conflict of interest to declare.

Authors’ contributions

YRT carried out the data collection, data analyses, and wrote the manuscript. MK assisted with data analyses. AL-A assisted with writing the manuscript. RCMEE participated in designing the study and helped write the manuscript. All authors agreed on the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tak, Y.R., Kleinjan, M., Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A. et al. Secondary outcomes of a school-based universal resiliency training for adolescents: a cluster randomized controlled trial. BMC Public Health 14, 1171 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-1171

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-1171

Keywords