Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Associations between psychological stress, eating, physical activity, sedentary behaviours and body weight among women: a longitudinal study

Abstract

Background

There is an increased risk of obesity amongst socioeconomically disadvantaged populations and emerging evidence suggests that psychological stress may be a key factor in this relationship. This paper reports the results of cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses of relationships between perceived stress, weight and weight-related behaviours in a cohort of socioeconomically disadvantaged women.

Methods

This study used baseline and follow-up self-report survey data from the Resilience for Eating and Activity Despite Inequality study, comprising a cohort of 1382 women aged 18 to 46 years from 80 of the most socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhoods in Victoria, Australia. Women reported their height (baseline only), weight, sociodemographic characteristics, perceived stress, leisure-time physical activity, sedentary and dietary behaviours at baseline and three-year follow-up. Linear and multinomial logistic regression were used to examine cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between stress (predictor) and weight, and weight-related behaviours.

Results

Higher perceived stress in women was associated with a higher BMI, and to increased odds of being obese in cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations were found between stress and both less leisure-time physical activity, and more frequent fast food consumption. Longitudinal associations were also found between stress and increased television viewing time.

Conclusion

The present study contributes to the literature related to the effects of stress on weight and weight-related behaviours. The findings suggest that higher stress levels could contribute to obesity risk in women. Further research is needed to fully understand the mechanisms underlying these associations. However, interventions that incorporate stress management techniques might help to prevent rising obesity rates among socioeconomically disadvantaged women.

Peer Review reports

Background

Current rates of overweight and obesity in developed countries present a major threat for population health [1]. Obesity is a significant risk factor for a range of adverse health conditions, including type 2 diabetes, stroke, cardiovascular disease and various forms of cancer [2]. Certain population groups are at increased risk of overweight and obesity, with higher levels of obesity reported in women of childbearing age [3, 4], those who are socioeconomically disadvantaged or those who are living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhoods [58]. While poor diets and physical inactivity are recognised as key behaviours implicated in the aetiology of obesity [912], the determinants of the increased risk of obesity and its determinant behaviours amongst socioeconomically disadvantaged groups remain poorly understood.

One key factor suggested to be linked to the development of obesity and which may be particularly pertinent among socioeconomically disadvantaged groups is psychological stress. Several studies have reported that indicators of chronic stress are associated with greater abdominal adiposity [13, 14]. A systematic review of the literature reported less healthy eating patterns and higher body weight in individuals in lower social positions who had higher stress levels, with these patterns more apparent in women than men [15]. A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies showed that stress was associated with increasing adiposity [16]. Furthermore, higher levels of stress in the family reportedly increases children’s obesity risk [17, 18], and several studies have reported associations between work stress and obesity risk [1921]. For example, work stress has been associated with increased body mass index (BMI) at follow-up in a group of male and female employees, with findings also showing increased alcohol consumption and decreased vegetable consumption in workers with low job control [22]. However, research on these relationships has produced inconsistent results [23, 24]. For example, in a group of low-income young mothers perceived stress was not a significant predictor of obesity [23].

There are few longitudinal studies that have explored the relationships between stress, body weight and weight-related behaviours. Longitudinal studies can provide insights into the direction and potential nature of associations among these variables. It is plausible that obesity is a consequence of stress, for example reflecting the use of maladaptive coping strategies such as comfort eating or excessive sedentary behaviours [25]. Previous studies have reported that chronic stress is associated with binge or comfort type eating [26], reduced physical activity levels [27] and increased sedentary behaviours [28]. Preferences for more palatable, higher fat, energy dense foods have also been associated with stress [29, 30]. However, prospective research is limited, and confirmation of the temporal nature of these associations in longitudinal studies is required. Furthermore, few studies have explored these relationships in socioeconomically disadvantaged women. As living in a socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhood places residents at increased risk of both obesity [31] and psychological stress [32], examining associations between these factors is particularly pertinent in this vulnerable population. The aim of this study was to determine whether perceived stress was associated cross-sectionally and longitudinally with weight and weight-related behaviours in a cohort of women living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhoods.

Methods

Sample

This study examined baseline (T1) and three-year follow-up (T2) data collected in 2007–08 and 2010–2011 as part of the Resilience for Eating and Activity Despite Inequality (READI) study [33]. This multilevel study followed a cohort of women aged 18 to 46 years living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhoods. Ethical approval for the study was given by the Deakin University Human Research Ethics Committee, the Victorian Department of Education and the Catholic Education Office.

Forty rural and 40 urban neighbourhoods (suburbs) were randomly selected from the most socioeconomically disadvantaged third of all areas across Victoria, Australia, according to the Australian Bureau of Statistics’ (ABS) Socioeconomic Index for Areas [34]. The sampling framework only included neighbourhoods with more than 1200 inhabitants and within 200 km from Melbourne.

One hundred fifty (150) women from each of the 80 neighbourhoods were randomly selected from the electoral roll. As voting is compulsory for Australian adults, the electoral roll provides a relatively complete record of population data in Australian residents aged 18 years and over. Where there were fewer than 150 women living in the neighbourhood (n = 3 neighbourhoods), all those who were eligible were invited to participate. A T1 self-report survey was mailed to an initial sample of 11940 women between August 2007 and January 2008. The survey assessed women’s physical activity, eating behaviours, height and weight, and a broad range of factors thought to influence these behaviours and obesity risk. A reminder protocol [35] was employed whereby letters were sent to nonresponders 10 days after the initial survey package was mailed. A second reminder letter followed including another copy of the survey after a further 10 days. The surveys were initially pilot-tested with a convenience sample of 32 women aged 18 to 46 years and minor modifications were made for clarity based on the feedback received.

A total of 4934 women returned a completed survey. Excluding those surveys marked ‘return to sender’ (n = 861) or from women who were otherwise ineligible (e.g., were deceased, or were incorrectly denoted as females on the electoral roll); this represented a response rate of 45%. Data from a further 571 women were excluded because the women no longer lived in a READI neighbourhood, nine were excluded because they were not within the desired age range (18 to 46 years), three were excluded because the survey was not completed by the woman it was addressed to, and two subsequently requested to be withdrawn from the study. This left a total of 4349 women with T1 data. Comparison of the T1 READI sample with the general population of women living in the 80 neighbourhoods recorded in the 2006 Census [36, 37] showed that a greater proportion of READI women were Australian born (89% vs. 73%), and were married or living as married (65% vs. 49%), but a lower proportion of READI women were in full-time employment (37% vs. 58%).

Three years following the T1 survey, all participants who consented to further follow-up in their T1 survey and remained in a READI neighbourhood (n = 2850) were sent a T2 survey, which repeated most of the questions in the T1 survey. Fifty-one women were excluded as they moved out of a READI neighbourhood. One thousand nine hundred twelve T2 surveys (n = 1912) were returned. Data from 483 women were excluded due to missing outcome data at T1 or T2, 81 missing covariate data and 8 missing stress scores. Some women had missing data on more than one set of variables, leaving an analysis sample of 1382.

Measures

Sociodemographic characteristics

Participants were asked to provide sociodemographic information including age, highest level of education (categorised as ‘low’ - did not complete high school, ‘medium’ - completed high school/trade certificate/diploma, or ‘high’ - completed tertiary education), marital status (categorised as ‘married’ - married/de facto, ‘previously married’ - separated/divorced/widowed, or ‘never married’), employment status (categorised as ‘working full-time’, ‘working part-time’ or ‘not currently employed in paid work’), smoking status (categorised as ‘never smoked’, ‘used to smoke’, ‘smoke occasionally’, or ‘smoke regularly’), country of birth (categorised as either ‘Australia’ or ‘other’), serious illness, long term injury or disability that prevents physical activity (categorised as ‘yes’ or ‘no’) and the number of dependent children (categorised as ‘none’, ‘one’, ‘two’, or ‘three or more’).

Weight and BMI

Participants reported their height at T1 and weight at T1 and T2. BMI was calculated for each participant at T1 and T2 by dividing weight (in kilograms) by height (in metres) squared, and categorised as healthy weight (18.5–24.9 kg m-2), overweight (25.0–29.9 kg m-2) or obese (BMI 30.0 kg m-2 or more) [2]. Due to the very low number of women in the underweight category (BMI <18.5 kg m-2) (n = 126), data for these women were combined with those in the healthy weight category.

Physical activity and sedentary behaviours

Physical activity at T1 and T2 was assessed using the long version of the self-administered International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ-L), a well-established survey with demonstrated test-retest reliability and validity [38]. The IPAQ-L was used to measure leisure-time physical activity (LTPA) and the amount of time women spent sitting in the last seven days. Women were also asked to report the amount of time spent sitting watching television during the past week on both weekdays and weekend days. For each of the T1 and T2 physical activity and sedentary behaviour measures, tertile splits were used to categorise women as spending ‘low’, ‘medium’ or ‘high’ amount of time engaged in the activity. Tertile splits were used due to highly skewed distributions in the data.

Food habits

Six variables were used as indicators of food habits. These were selected based on their high energy/low nutrient content and they are likely important contributors to high-energy intake and obesity risk [39, 40]. They include; potato crisps or salty snack food; chocolate or lollies; cake, doughnuts or sweet biscuits; pies, pasties or sausage rolls; fast foods; pizza; and non-diet soft drink. These variables were assessed using a Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ), which was based on several previously published and validated Australian questionnaires and assessed food habits during the previous month [4143]. There were nine response categories for each food item ranging from ‘Never or less than once/month’ to ‘6 or more times a day’. For the soft drink item, six response options ranged from ‘I don’t drink soft drink’ to ‘10 or more serves/day’. For each of the T1 and T2 food habits measures, tertile splits based on the distributions were used to categorise women as having ‘low’, ‘medium’ or ‘high’ intake of each food or drink item.

Perceived stress

Stress was measured using the 4-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), which is used to measure the extent to which individuals consider situations in their life as stressful in terms of feeling in control [44]. The PSS has previously shown adequate reliability and validity among a sample of males and females participating in a smoking cessation program [45]. The specific questions were: “During the last month how often have you: (i) felt that you were unable to control the important things in your life? (ii) felt confident about your ability to handle your personal problems? (iii) felt that things were going your way? (iv) felt difficulties were piling up so high that you could not overcome them?” Responses were categorised as ‘never’ (scored 1), ‘almost never’ (2), ‘sometimes’ (3), ‘fairy often’ (4), or ‘very often’ (5). Perceived stress scores were then calculated by reverse scoring the positively stated items (ii and iii) and then summing all scale items (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.78).

Statistical methods

The data were analysed using SPSS Statistics 18.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and STATA Version 12 (Stata-Corp, College Station, TX). Multinomial logistic regression (MLR) was used to examine the cross-sectional associations between perceived stress, weight-related behaviours and weight status. Additionally, a linear regression model was tested to examine the cross-sectional association between perceived stress and continuous BMI. Separate models were analysed for each outcome measure. T1 perceived stress was the predictor.

Longitudinal associations between perceived stress, weight-related behaviours and weight status outcomes were examined using MLR. A linear regression model was used to examine the longitudinal association between perceived stress and continuous BMI. Separate longitudinal models were tested for each of the T2 weight and weight-related behaviour outcomes, and included T1 perceived stress as the predictor. Each longitudinal model also controlled for the corresponding T1 weight or behavioural measure, e.g. in the model with T2 soft drink intake as an outcome, T1 soft drink intake was included as a covariate. In each MLR analysis, ‘low’ was the reference category for weight-related outcomes. All analyses controlled for age, education level, marital status, employment status, smoking status, the number of dependent children and country of birth, all assessed at T1. All models were also adjusted for clustering by neighbourhood. The presence of a serious illness, long term injury or disability that prevents physical activity was also controlled for in all regression analyses predicting physical activity and sedentary behaviour.

Results

T1 sociodemographic characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 1. The mean age of the sample was 35.71 years (s.d. = 7.7) and the mean BMI was 26.2 (s.d. = 5.9). Most of the women were born in Australia (92.3%) and had a medium level of education (49.7%).

Table 1 T1 (baseline) characteristics of the READI sample (n = 1382)

Table 2 shows the distributions of outcomes within the sample at T1 and T2. At T1, over half of the women were in the healthy weight range (52.2%), 26.8% overweight and 20.9% obese. Forty-seven percent of women were in the healthy weight range at T2, 29.7% overweight and 23.6% obese. Thirty-one percent of women engaged in 52 or fewer minutes of LTPA per week (the cut point for the lowest tertile of LTPA) at T1, and 32% at T2. Thirty-four percent engaged in more than 52 hours of sitting time per week, and 34% watched between 14–21 hours of television per week at T1. Similarly, 33% engaged in more than 52 hours of sitting time per week, and 35% watched between 14–21 hours of television per week at T2. Potato crisps or salty snack food intake of once or more times per week was reported by 42% of the sample at T1. However, 42% of the sample reported potato crisps or salty snack food intake 1–3 times per month at T2. Most women consumed chocolates or lollies twice or more times per week at T1 and T2. Fifty-one percent and 56% of women reported that they did not drink soft drink (excluding diet soft drink) at T1 and T2, respectively. Most women reported consuming cake, doughnuts and sweet biscuits, and pizza less than once per week, and pies, pastries or sausage rolls, and consuming fast foods (e.g. McDonalds, KFC) less than monthly at T1 and T2.

Table 2 Distributions of outcomes within READI sample at T1 and T2

Table 3 shows the cross-sectional associations between stress and weight and behavioural outcomes at T1. A positive association was found between stress levels and continuous BMI (B = 0.28, CI = 0.17, 0.39, p < 0.0005). Furthermore, for every increase of one unit on the stress scale, there was an increase of 6% (CI = 1.01,1.11, p = 0.013) in the odds of being overweight, and 13% (CI = 1.08,1.19, p <0.0005) increase in the odds of being obese. Stress levels were associated with LTPA, such that higher stress scores were associated with a lower likelihood of undertaking medium (OR = 0.93, CI = 0.88,0.99, p = 0.014) or high (OR = 0.91, CI = 0.86,0.96, p < 0.0005) levels of LTPA. There were no cross-sectional associations found between stress and sedentary behaviour measures. Similarly, few cross-sectional associations between stress and measures of food habits were found. However, stress was associated with increased odds of high intakes of fast foods (OR = 1.09, CI = 1.02,1.17, p = 0.010).

Table 3 MLR analyses of cross-sectional associations between stress and behavioural outcomes at T1

Table 4 shows the longitudinal associations between stress (T1) and weight and behavioural outcomes (T2). A positive association was found between stress levels and BMI (B = 0.085, CI = 0.04-0.13, p < 0.0005). Stress was not predictive of being in the overweight BMI category (25.0–29.9 kg m-2), but higher stress levels were associated with an increase of 11% (CI = 1.00,1.23, p = 0.043) in the odds of being obese at T2. Longitudinal associations were also found between stress levels and the likelihood of engaging in medium (OR = 0.93, CI = 0.88, 0.98, p = 0.004) or high (OR = 0.89, CI = 0.84,0.94, p <0.0005) amounts of LTPA. Associations were also found between stress and television viewing, such that each increase of one unit corresponded to a 7% increase in the odds of watching medium amounts of television (CI = 1.01,1.12, p = 0.014). Consistent with cross-sectional associations, stress and fast food intake were longitudinally associated, such that greater stress scores predicted increased likelihood of consuming high intake of fast food (OR = 1.08, CI = 1.02,1.14, p = 0.011), but there were no other dietary associations.

Table 4 MLR analyses of longitudinal associations between T1 stress and behavioural outcomes at T2

Discussion

This study examined the associations between stress, weight and weight-related behaviours in a cohort of women living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhoods. The findings of this study are generally consistent with those of similar studies in other populations regarding the relationships between stress and weight [15, 19]. The present study found that higher stress in women was associated with increased odds of having a higher BMI, and of being obese. Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations were found between stress and both less leisure-time physical activity, and more frequent fast food consumption. Longitudinally, stress was also found to be a predictor of increased television viewing time.

Consistent with our findings, several studies have reported associations between stress and BMI [15, 19, 46]. A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies on stress and adiposity also found that stress promotes weight gain [16]. However, a study of adolescents in the United Kingdom found that there was no association between perceived stress and increases in weight over five years [24]. Inconsistencies in these results may be due to the difficulty in measuring stress, particularly in different age groups and populations. In the study with adolescents, the PSS was used to measure stress. This measure was initially developed to subjectively measure stress in adults, and adolescents may interpret questions differently. More valid measures of stress might provide more consistency in results among different studies. The majority of previous studies of stress and weight have not assessed associated weight-related behaviours, and hence shed little insight into potential mechanisms by which stress may influence weight change or obesity risk. Existing studies on the associations between stress and physical activity, for example, have produced inconsistent results [27, 47, 48]. However, our findings concur with those of several past studies showing that stress is associated with engaging in less physical activity [27, 47]. Less engagement in physical activity due to stress may reflect challenging life circumstances and difficulty coping, which may take precedence over self-care and health-promoting behaviours like physical activity [48]. Furthermore, despite evidence of the beneficial effects of physical activity on stress [49], many individuals may find sedentary activity more rewarding in the short-term [50].

Evidence of the associations between stress and sedentary behaviours are limited. The present study found that stress was longitudinally associated with moderate amounts of television viewing. Similarly, past research has shown that highly stressed parents of ill children were found to watch more hours of television than parents of healthy children [46]. Individuals in situations of high stress are more likely to engage in unhealthy behaviours that make them feel better [51]. Therefore, stressed women may seek comfort from television viewing or use television as a distraction from stressful thoughts.

The present study reported few associations between stress and measures of food habits. This may be due to the use of the FFQ in our study, which did not assess portion size, and hence may not be a sufficiently sensitive instrument to detect any associations between consumption of larger quantities of such food items and stress. However, stress was found to predict higher intakes of fast food consumption. Consistent with our findings, a study by Bauer et al. [17] used a series of questions to measure the frequency of fast food consumption in parents and reported more frequent fast food consumption in parents with greater work-life stress. It is possible that disadvantaged women who are feeling stressed may turn to fast food as a perceived ‘quick fix’, for instance if they are time poor; or it could be that the types of foods typically purchased in fast food outlets are perceived as ‘comfort’ or ‘reward’ foods and used to cope with stress [52]. A diary study assessing daily food choice of 30 food items reported that higher intake of soft drinks and lollies, particularly chocolate have been reportedly associated with stress [48], but this was not found in the present study. These discrepant findings may be attributable to the different food intake measures used, with a diary study potentially capturing daily variances in consumption more readily than the FFQ used in our study.

This study had a number of strengths. These include analyses of a large sample from a population of women living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhoods and who are at high risk of weight gain. The large sample size also allowed control for a range of key covariates. Furthermore, this study is one of few longitudinal studies assessing the relationships between stress, weight and weight-related behaviours. Limitations of this study include the reliance on self-report data, although established and validated measures were used where possible (e.g., the IPAQ-L to measure physical activity). Height and weight were self-reported which may have led to an underestimate of prevalence of overweight and obesity. This may have resulted in misestimation of the strength of associations between stress and overweight and obesity. However, recent evidence suggests substantial agreement between self-report and measured height and weight among Australian women [53]. Food habits were assessed with only a selected subset of FFQ items, and while these were based on previously validated scales, the validity of this subset of questions alone is not established. There was a modest response rate to the survey, and considerable loss to follow-up. For example, longitudinal analyses in this study were based on a sample of which approximately 10% were originally sampled. Since we have no information on weight status or stress from non-respondents to the initial mailout, we cannot conclude how this bias may affect results. However, such response and attrition are not atypical for this population [54, 55]. It should also be acknowledged that associations between stress and weight could operate in the reverse direction to that tested in the present study. That is, weight gain and obesity may lead to increased stress, for instance due to weight-related stigmatization or poor physical or mental health associated with obesity. Consistent with this hypothesis, several studies have shown an association between obesity and future symptoms of depression [5658]. This remains a question for future research.

Acknowledging the study’s limitations, and the need for further confirmation of the mechanisms underlying the associations observed here, the findings from this study have important implications for public health practice, suggesting a potential key role for psychological stress in weight and weight-related behaviours. Public health interventions might benefit from the inclusion of stress management in weight loss interventions to address psychological health and maximise individuals’ weight loss and weight maintenance attempts. Particularly, the role of physical activity in reducing stress (and weight) could be emphasised in specifically targeted programs.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the present study demonstrated some cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between perceived psychological stress and BMI, as well as leisure-time physical activity, sedentary behaviour and fast food consumption. Developing intervention strategies to improve coping skills during situations of stress might assist women in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhoods to manage their weight more effectively.

Abbreviations

READI:

Resilience for eating and activity despite inequality

SIFA:

Socioeconomic index for areas

PSS:

Perceived stress scale

FFQ:

Food frequency questionnaire

IPAQ:

International physical activity questionnaire

LTPA:

Leisure-time physical activity

MLR:

Multinomial logistic regression.

References

  1. Crawford D, Jeffery R: Obesity Prevention and Public Health. 2005, New York: Oxford University Press Inc

    Google Scholar 

  2. World Health Organisation: Obesity: preventing and managing the global epidemic. Report of a WHO consultation. Technical Report Series 894. 2000, Geneva: WHO

    Google Scholar 

  3. Ball K, Brown W, Crawford D: Who does not gain weight? Prevalence and predictors of weight maintenance in young women. Int J Obesity. 2011, 26 (12): 1570-1578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Ball K, Crawford D, Ireland P, Hodge A: Patterns and demographic predictors of 5-year weight change in a multi-ethnic cohort of men and women in Australia. Public Health Nutr. 2003, 6 (3): 269-280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Ball K, Crawford D: Socioeconomic status and weight change in adults: a review. Soc Sci Med. 2005, 60 (9): 1987-2010. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.08.056.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Sobal J, Stunkard AJ: Socioeconomic status and obesity: a review of the literature. Psychol Bull. 1989, 105 (2): 260-

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Ellaway A, Anderson A, Macintyre S: Does area of residence affect body size and shape?. Int J Obesity. 1997, 21 (4): 304-308. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0800405.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Harrington DW, Elliott SJ: Weighing the importance of neighbourhood: a multilevel exploration of the determinants of overweight and obesity. Soc Sci Med. 2009, 68 (4): 593-600. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.11.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Bellows-Riecken KH, Rhodes RE: A birth of inactivity? A review of physical activity and parenthood. Prev Med. 2008, 46 (2): 99-110. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2007.08.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. MacFarlane A, Abbott G, Crawford D, Ball K: Sociodemographic and behavioural correlates of weight status among women with children living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhoods. Int J Obesity. 2009, 33 (11): 1289-1298. 10.1038/ijo.2009.167.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Ball K, Crawford D, Warren N: How feasible are healthy eating and physical activity for young women?. Public Health Nutr. 2004, 7 (03): 433-441.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. George GC, Milani TJ, Hanss-Nuss H, Freeland-Graves JH: Compliance with dietary guidelines and relationship to psychosocial factors in low-income women in late postpartum. J Am Diet Assoc. 2005, 105 (6): 916-926. 10.1016/j.jada.2005.03.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Brunner E, Marmot M, Nanchahal K, Shipley M, Stansfeld S, Juneja M, Alberti KGMM: Social inequality in coronary risk: central obesity and the metabolic syndrome. Evidence from the Whitehall II study. Diabetologia. 1997, 40 (11): 1341-1349. 10.1007/s001250050830.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Rosmond R, Björntorp P: Occupational status, cortisol secretory pattern, and visceral obesity in middle-aged men. Obesity. 2000, 8 (6): 445-450. 10.1038/oby.2000.55.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Moore CJ, Cunningham SA: Social position, psychological stress, and obesity: a systematic review. J Acad Nutrition Dietetics. 2012, 112 (4): 518-526. 10.1016/j.jand.2011.12.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Wardle J, Chida Y, Gibson EL, Whitaker KL, Steptoe A: Stress and Adiposity: a Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies. Obesity. 2011, 19 (4): 771-778. 10.1038/oby.2010.241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Bauer KW, Hearst MO, Escoto K, Berge JM, Neumark-Sztainer D: Parental employment and work-family stress: associations with family food environments. Soc Sci Med. 2012, 75 (3): 496-504. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.03.026.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Koch FS, Sepa A, Ludvigsson J: Psychological stress and obesity. J Pediatr. 2008, 153 (6): 839-844. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.06.016. e3

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Block JP, He Y, Zaslavsky AM, Ding L, Ayanian JZ: Psychosocial stress and change in weight among US adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2009, 170 (2): 181-192. 10.1093/aje/kwp104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Nishitani N, Sakakibara H: Relationship of obesity to job stress and eating behavior in male Japanese workers. Int J Obesity. 2005, 30 (3): 528-533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Berset M, Semmer NK, Elfering A, Jacobshagen N, Meier LL: Does stress at work make you gain weight? A two-year longitudinal study. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2011, 37 (1): 45-10.5271/sjweh.3089.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Tsutsumi A, Kayaba K, Yoshimura M, Sawada M, Ishikawa S, Sakai K, Gotoh T, Nago N: Association between job characteristics and health behaviors in Japanese rural workers. Int J Behav Med. 2003, 10 (2): 125-142. 10.1207/S15327558IJBM1002_03.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Rohrer J, Rohland B: Psychosocial risk factors for obesity among women in a family planning clinic. BMC Fam Pract. 2004, 5 (1): 20-10.1186/1471-2296-5-20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. van Jaarsveld CHM, Fidler JA, Steptoe A, Boniface D, Wardle J: Perceived stress and weight gain in adolescence: a longitudinal analysis. Obesity (Silver Spring, Md). 2009, 17 (12): 2155-2161. 10.1038/oby.2009.183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Sulkowski ML, Dempsey J, Dempsey AG: Effects of stress and coping on binge eating in female college students. Eat Behav. 2011, 12 (3): 188-191. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2011.04.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Elfhag K, Rössner S: Who succeeds in maintaining weight loss? A conceptual review of factors associated with weight loss maintenance and weight regain. Obes Rev. 2005, 6 (1): 67-85. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2005.00170.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Ng DM, Jeffery RW: Relationships between perceived stress and health behaviors in a sample of working adults. Health Psychol. 2003, 22 (6): 638-

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Brisson C, Larocque B, Moisan J, Vézina M, Dagenais GR: Factors at work, smoking, sedentary behavior, and body mass index: a prevalence study among 6995 white collar workers. J Occup Environ Med. 2000, 42 (1): 40-10.1097/00043764-200001000-00011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Adam TC, Epel ES: Stress, eating and the reward system. Physiol Behav. 2007, 91 (4): 449-458. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Leigh Gibson E: Emotional influences on food choice: sensory, physiological and psychological pathways. Physiol Behav. 2006, 89 (1): 53-61. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.024.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. King T, Kavanagh AM, Jolley D, Turrell G, Crawford D: Weight and place: a multilevel cross-sectional survey of area-level social disadvantage and overweight/obesity in Australia. Int J Obesity. 2005, 30 (2): 281-287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Steptoe A, Feldman PJ: Neighborhood problems as sources of chronic stress: development of a measure of neighborhood problems, and associations with socioeconomic status and health. Ann Behav Med. 2001, 23 (3): 177-185. 10.1207/S15324796ABM2303_5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Ball K, Abbott G, Cleland V, Timperio A, Thornton L, Mishra G, Jeffery R, Brug J, King A, Crawford D: Resilience to obesity among socioeconomically disadvantaged women: the READI study. Int J Obesity. 2011, 36 (6): 855-865.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Australian Bureau of Statistics: Census of Population and Housing - SEIFA 2001. 2001, Canberra, ACT: ABS

    Google Scholar 

  35. Dillman DA: Mail and telephone surveys. 1978, New York: Wiley

    Google Scholar 

  36. Australian Bureau of Statistics: Australian Census of Population and Housing 2006. 2006, Canberra, ACT: ABS

    Google Scholar 

  37. Australian Bureau of Statistics: General Social Survey 2006 User Guide. 2007, Canberra, ACT: ABS

    Google Scholar 

  38. Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjöström M, Bauman AE, Booth ML, Ainsworth BE, Pratt M, Ekelund U, Yngve A, Sallis JF: International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2003, 35 (8): 1381-10.1249/01.MSS.0000078924.61453.FB.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Prentice AM, Jebb SA: Fast foods, energy density and obesity: a possible mechanistic link. Obes Rev. 2003, 4 (4): 187-194. 10.1046/j.1467-789X.2003.00117.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Tam CS, Garnett SP, Cowell CT, Campbell K, Cabrera G, Baur LA: Soft drink consumption and excess weight gain in Australian school students: results from the Nepean study. Int J Obesity. 2006, 30 (7): 1091-1093. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803328.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Hodge A, Patterson AJ, Brown WJ, Ireland P, Giles G: The Anti Cancer Council of Victoria FFQ: relative validity of nutrient intakes compared with weighed food records in young to middle-aged women in a study of iron supplementation. Aust NZ J Public Health. 2000, 24 (6): 576-583. 10.1111/j.1467-842X.2000.tb00520.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Marks GC, Webb K, Rutishauser IH, Riley M: Monitoring food habits in the Australian population using short questions. National Food and Nutrition Monitoring and Surveillance Project. 2001, Canberra: Commonwealth Department of Health and Aged Care

    Google Scholar 

  43. McLennan W, Podger A, Australian Bureau of Statistics: National Nutrition Survey: selected highlights, Australia, 1995. 1998, Canberra, ACT: ABS

    Google Scholar 

  44. Cohen S: Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States. The social psychology of health. edn. Edited by: Spacapan S, Oskamp S. 1988, Thousand Oaks, CA US: Sage Publications, Inc, 31-67.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R: A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983, 24 (4): 385-396. 10.2307/2136404.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Smith AW, Baum A, Wing RR: Stress and weight gain in parents of cancer patients. Int J Obesity. 2005, 29 (2): 244-250. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802835.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Steptoe A, Wardle J, Pollard TM, Canaan L, Davies GJ: Stress, social support and health-related behavior: a study of smoking, alcohol consumption and physical exercise. J Psychosom Res. 1996, 41 (2): 171-180. 10.1016/0022-3999(96)00095-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Steptoe A, Lipsey Z, Wardle J: Stress, hassles and variations in alcohol consumption, food choice and physical exercise: a diary study. Br J Health Psychol. 1998, 3 (1): 51-63. 10.1111/j.2044-8287.1998.tb00555.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Long BC, van Stavel R: Effects of exercise training on anxiety: a meta-analysis. J Appl Sport Psychol. 1995, 7 (2): 167-189. 10.1080/10413209508406963.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Buckworth J, Nigg C: Physical activity, exercise, and sedentary behavior in college students. J Am Coll Health. 2004, 53 (1): 28-34. 10.3200/JACH.53.1.28-34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Zillmann D, Bryant J: Affect, mood, and emotion as determinants of selective exposure. Selective Exposure to Communication. Edited by: Zillman D, Bryant J. 1985, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 157-190.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Dallman MF, Pecoraro N, Akana SF, La Fleur SE, Gomez F, Houshyar H, Bell M, Bhatnagar S, Laugero KD, Manalo S: Chronic stress and obesity: a new view of comfort food. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2003, 100 (20): 11696-11701. 10.1073/pnas.1934666100.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Burton NW, Brown W, Dobson A: Accuracy of body mass index estimated from self-reported height and weight in mid-aged Australian women. Aust NZ J Pub Heal. 2010, 34 (6): 620-623. 10.1111/j.1753-6405.2010.00618.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Galea S, Tracy M: Participation rates in epidemiologic studies. Ann Epidemiol. 2007, 17 (9): 643-653. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2007.03.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Sheikh K, Mattingly S: Investigating non-response bias in mail surveys. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1981, 35 (4): 293-296. 10.1136/jech.35.4.293.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Faith M, Butryn M, Wadden T, Fabricatore A, Nguyen A, Heymsfield S: Evidence for prospective associations among depression and obesity in population-based studies. Obes Rev. 2011, 12 (5): e438-e453. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2010.00843.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Roberts RE, Deleger S, Strawbridge WJ, Kaplan GA: Prospective association between obesity and depression: evidence from the Alameda County Study. Int J Obesity. 2003, 27 (4): 514-521. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802204.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Herva A, Laitinen J, Miettunen J, Veijola J, Karvonen J, Läksy K, Joukamaa M: Obesity and depression: results from the longitudinal Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort Study. Int J Obesity. 2005, 30 (3): 520-527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

The READI study was supported by an Australian National Health and Medical Research Council Strategic Award (ID 374241) and a Deakin University Faculty of Health, Medicine, Nursing and Behavioural Sciences Research Development Grant. The authors gratefully acknowledge the contributions of Project Manager Michelle Jackson, field staff, and the study participants. KB is supported by a NHMRC Senior Research Fellowship, ID 479513.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kylie Ball.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declared that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

JM carried out background research and drafted the manuscript. GA and KB assisted JM in performing statistical analysis and helped draft the manuscript. KB conceived the idea for and implemented the READI study, and developed the measures and methods. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mouchacca, J., Abbott, G.R. & Ball, K. Associations between psychological stress, eating, physical activity, sedentary behaviours and body weight among women: a longitudinal study. BMC Public Health 13, 828 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-828

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-828

Keywords