Skip to main content

Randomized controlled trial of computer-based treatment of social cognition in schizophrenia: the TRuSST trial protocol

Abstract

Background

Schizophrenia is a severe and chronic medical condition, characterized by positive and negative symptoms, as well as pervasive social cognitive deficits. Despite the functional significance of the social cognition deficits affecting many aspects of daily living, such as social relationships, occupational status, and independent living, there is still no effective treatment option for these deficits, which is applied as standard of care. To address this need, we developed a novel, internet-based training program that targets social cognition deficits in schizophrenia (SocialVille). Preliminary studies demonstrate the feasibility and initial efficacy of Socialville in schizophrenia patients (Nahum et al., 2014). The purpose of the current trial (referred to as the TReatment of Social cognition in Schizophrenia Trial or TRuSST) is to compare SocialVille to an active control training condition, include a larger sample of patients, and assess both social cognitive functioning, and functional outcomes.

Methods/Design

We will employ a multi-site, longitudinal, blinded, randomized controlled trial (RCT) design with a target sample of 128 patients with schizophrenia. Patients will perform, at their home or in clinic, 40 sessions of either the SocialVille training program or an active control computer game condition. Each session will last for 40–45 minutes/day, performed 3–5 days a week, over 10–12 weeks, totaling to 30 hours of training. Patients will be assessed on a battery of social cognitive, social functioning and functional outcomes immediately before training, mid-way through training (after 20 training sessions) and at the completion of the 40 training sessions.

Discussion

The strengths of this protocol are that it tests an innovative, internet-based treatment that targets fundamental social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, employs a highly sensitive and extensive battery of functional outcome measures, and incorporates a large sample size in an RCT design.

Trial Registration

ClinicalTrials.gov NCT02246426Registered 16 September 2014

Peer Review reports

Background

Schizophrenia is a severe, chronic mental illness that affects more than two million individuals in the U.S. [1]. Individuals with schizophrenia have both positive and negative symptoms, including hallucinations, delusions, disorganized speech and behavior, alogia (poverty of speech), affect flattening, and avolition (inability to initiate and persist in goal-directed behaviors) [2]. These clinical symptoms are often accompanied by severe cognitive deficits, in speed of processing, attention, working memory, verbal and visual learning and memory, and executive function [3]. Moreover, individuals with schizophrenia often exhibit difficulties in social functioning, namely in their ability to navigate through the social world, create meaningful interactions, and correctly interpret relevant social context [4]. This poor social functioning has been attributable to pervasive and enduring impairments insocial cognition [510]: the perception, interpretation and processing of socially-relevant information [1116]. Individuals with schizophrenia exhibit deficits in all core domains of social cognition [17, 18]: emotion perception (the recognition of facial and vocal affect) [1923], social cue perception (the ability to detect and comprehend cues in a social context) [2426], theory of mind (the mental capacity to infer one’s own and others’ mental states) [1, 2729], attributional style (attribution of causes of events to the self, to others, or to factors in the environment) [30, 31], and empathy (the ability to share, understand and appropriately react to the emotional states of others) [32]. Recent studies have shown that these social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia are rooted in anatomical and functional abnormalities within a complex brain network collectively termed “the social brain” [3339], and include the superior temporal sulcus (STS), anterior insula, amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and to the cingulate cortex [4044]. These fundamental, multi-domain social cognition impairments are not only directly linked with poor social functioning, but also underlie most critical factors of daily living in schizophrenia, such as low occupational status, poor social and community functioning, reduced capabilities for independent living, high relapse rate, and reduced quality of life [2, 4, 4551]. Moreover, the degree of social cognition impairment is a stronger predictor of the level of everyday functional ability than are cognitive abilities or the severity of positive symptoms [52, 53]. This makes social cognition an important treatment target in schizophrenia: as the ultimate goal of therapeutic interventions is to improve life outcomes for patients, it is now clear that recovery of these individuals to the broader society is crucially dependent upon the recovery of their social cognitive abilities. The fact that social cognition deficits persist throughout the course of the illness [22, 54, 55], are seen in prodromal patients [22], and are even present in unaffected relatives of patients, further stresses their central role in schizophrenia and fuels the need for an effective, scalable treatment for social cognitive deficits (see recent review in [44]).

Despite the importance of social cognition as a primary source of impairment, there are currently no well-accepted or even broadly administered treatment methods for improving social cognitive function in schizophrenia patients. Social cognitive deficits are resistant to pharmacological treatments including second-generation antipsychotic medications [5660] that are effective for controlling positive symptom levels [58]. Perhaps more surprisingly, new and demonstrably effective interventions for treating cognitive deficits in schizophrenia have been shown to have only limited impacts on social functioning [61] - presumably because social cognition deficits are associated with impaired function of neural networks that are largely distinct from, and parallel to, those subserving general neurocognition [18].

Several experimental, therapist-delivered approaches targeting social skills or social cognition have been developed over the last decade (e.g. [6266]), and initial studies have shown some promising results (see recent reviews in [52, 67, 68]). These interventions are offered in only a few clinics nationwide and are usually administered by trained professionals individually or in small groups over the course of several months. The therapist-administered options (e.g. Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) [66, 6971]; Social Cognitive Skills Training (SCST) [72]; Emotion and ToM Imitation Training (ETIT) [73]; Social Cognition Enhancement Training (SCET) [74]) usually focus on emotion management and social skills building, and require multiple in-person visits to the clinic in the course of a few months. Recently, several computer-aided interventions (e.g. Tackling Affect Recognition (TAR) [75]; MicroExpression Training Tool (METT) [65]) have been created. These interventions are limited in scope (mainly target a single social cognitive domain in isolation, such as facial affect recognition), have undergone only initial testing in schizophrenia [52, 65, 75], and are not used, to the best of our knowledge, in any clinical treatment programs. While, collectively, these approaches show promise for social cognitive treatment in schizophrenia, to date, no single treatment has been widely adopted, and there is no standard of care for social cognitive treatment in schizophrenia. This is potentially due to the fact that these interventions are not scalable and cost-effective, as they require highly-trained personnel and necessitate frequent visits to the clinic, limiting their scalability and significantly increasing their associated costs.

To address the need for a scalable and effective treatment for social cognition deficits in schizophrenia, that considers these deficits from their neurological core (see [44, 76, 77] for recent reviews on this topic), we have developed SocialVille, an internet-based treatment program designed to specifically address the core social cognitive domains of deficit in individuals with schizophrenia. While the use of computer-based strategies to strengthen social behaviors may seem paradoxical or counter-intuitive, the goal of this form of training does not entail strengthening explicit social skills, but rather strengthening the brain basis that comprises these skills, for which the use of a computer is a substantial advantage. The 27 different exercises of SocialVille collectively encompass the five social cognitive domains (see Table 1 for a complete list). The user is required to make hundreds of speeded, accurate, and increasingly more challenging discriminations of socially-relevant information (e.g., emotional faces, eye gazes, prosody, social situations). During training, the user is systematically exposed to socially-relevant stimuli, starting from very basic-level stimuli and gradually involving more complex, multi-modal, and ecologically-valid stimuli. Trial-by-trial difficulty is adaptively set using either up-down [78] or Bayesian [79] algorithms, maintaining individual success rate at 70-80 % success level, allowing for progression through training based on the user’s individual performance level. Finally, a secure online clinician portal allows the treating clinician to track user performance and treatment compliance.

Table 1 SocialVille training exercise

Two recently-published studies using SocialVille show promising initial results in both adults with schizophrenia [80] and in young adults at high clinical risk for psychosis [81]. Nahum et al. found that following 24 hours of SocialVille training, participants showed improvements in proximal measures of social cognition (e.g. facial memory, prosody identification), as well as in more remote measures of social functioning and motivation [80]. These preliminary results of SocialVille demonstrate some transfer of training benefits to more general skills. The current multi-site clinical trial extends this and other studies with a longer duration of SocialVille training (30 hours total), the inclusion of an active control training condition (computer games, see Table 2), and the inclusion of additional social and functional outcome measures.

Table 2 Active cotrol training games

Two additional innovations in the current protocol are worth mentioning. First, to the best of our knowledge, this trial is the first to include a fully-Internet-based treatment for social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, designed to be completed entirely remotely (from home), with minimal supervision and clinic visits only for assessments (see recent review of training studies in [82]). Some studies employing social cognition training in schizophrenia have used computerized interventions, but these were usually applied as part of a larger, instruction-based therapy [66, 6972, 83] or in small groups of participants in the clinic (e.g. [84]). The advantage of using a fully online training program is that it can be adaptively tailored to patient’s individual abilities to provide the appropriate level of training, and is easily scalable to support many users.

The second innovation in our current protocol is the use of a large battery of outcome measures, including novel, computerized measures (e.g. Virtual Reality Functional Capacity Assessment Tool (VRFCAT) [85]). As the ultimate goal of clinical intervention is to improve functional outcomes by improving social abilities, it is important to document the degree of improvement in various aspects, encompassing social and global functioning, quality of life, functional capacity, motivation, and symptom severity level. Nonetheless, previous studies generally employed limited batteries of outcome measures (see [67, 68]) or lacked adequate controls that are matched for intensity and experimenter contact [86].

Aims and hypotheses

The aim of the current study is to test the effectiveness of 30 hours of computer-based SocialVille training to improve social cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia, compared with an active control intervention. Based on our previous findings, we predict that the SocialVille group will show statistically greater gains on co-primary outcome measures of social cognitive performance and functional performance as well as secondary measures of social cognition, functional capacity, functional outcome, motivation, and quality of life, indicating that SocialVille drives generalized social cognitive improvements as well as real-world functional improvements. Secondary aims include determining which patients best respond to SocialVille, and evaluating the effects of SocialVille training on low-level vs. high-level social cognition factors. For treatment response, we will examine predictors of social cognitive gains based on baseline participant demographic, symptom severity level, prior computer use, and functional measures, as well as on learning rate and plateau performance measures derived over the course of SocialVille use. Based on our previous studies, we hypothesize that most baseline measures will not define responder/non-responder groups effectively (except potentially for symptom severity), while baseline exercise performance, learning rate and plateau performance during SocialVille use will predict overall gains. Finally, we will separately examine the effects of training on the independent social cognitive factors of low-level social cue detection and high-level inferential process. We hypothesize that both low-level and high-level social cognition factors will be significantly correlated with functional capacity and real-world social and role functioning.

Methods

Ethics Statement

The Western International Review Board (WIRB) is designated to review and provide continuing oversight of ethical standards involving human subjects research (WIRB Pro Number 20141695). Research is conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and monitored by the WIRB. Participants interested in the study will meet with qualified study staff for the consenting process, during which the participant is informed of the nature of the trial, purpose of research, trial procedures, risks and benefits, confidentiality, etc. Following consent, the participant will be assessed for eligibility and potential enrollment in the trial. Minors are excluded from this study and will not undergo the consenting process.

Overall Design and Timeline

The current study will employ a multi-site, longitudinal, blinded randomized controlled trial (RCT) design with a target sample of 128 patients with schizophrenia (see inclusion criteria below). This trial will follow the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) guidelines [87] for the design, execution, and reporting of clinical trials, with the Non-Pharmacologic Treatment Interventions extensions to reflect the use of computerized social cognitive remediation as a treatment intervention.

Sixty-four patients with schizophrenia completing SocialVille will be compared to sixty-four patients with schizophrenia completing the active control condition (online computer-based games; see Fig. 1). Total participation time is approximately 12–14 weeks and includes seven (7) in-person assessment sessions. The first assessment session (V0) involves screening for eligibility (see inclusion/exclusion below). If the participant is eligible, they next perform two baseline assessment visit (V1-V2) to provide baseline level of their symptoms, social cognition, social functioning, functional capacity and motivation before training. After the baseline assessments, patients are randomized to either the SocialVille or control training program, and complete a set-up visit (V3) in which they are provided with a study laptop with their corresponding training program. Participants are then requested to complete 40 training sessions (about 10–12 weeks) of in-home training, while monitored remotely by a research assistant (cognitive remediation coach). Participants will be assessed again at the completion of 20 sessions (mid-intervention assessments; V4-V5) and at the completion of the entire training of 40 sessions (post-intervention assessments, V6-V7), to measure potential training-related improvements. After this visit (V7), participant activities are completed and trial participation ends. All assessments will be done by assessors that are blind to group affiliation.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study Outline. Following screening and baseline visits, participants are randomized into the experimental intervention (SocialVille) or the active control intervention (AC), in which they complete 40 sessions of training, with assessment visits conducted after 20 sessions and after training completion

Study Population

The study population is comprised of individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia. All participants must be clinically stable and be stable on the doses of the psychiatric medications they are taking. For the purposes of this trial, we will focus on treating core symptoms rather than examining etiology, and only enroll those individuals that meet inclusion/exclusion criteria (see below).

We will employ a multi-site study, which is used to ensure that the results are not particular to the participant population or study operations at a single site. We have ongoing research collaborations with all site PIs (who served as consultants and collaborators on the Phase I grant). Study participants will be recruited with established and proven mechanisms developed at each site. All Site PIs, internationally-recognized experts in the field, have extensive expertise in this population as well as access to large cohorts of patients with schizophrenia.

We do not anticipate any specific barriers to the accrual of participants, nor are we aware of a large number of competing clinical trials that would limit enrollment. The study population is limited to those 18 years of age or older. We chose a minimum age of 18, to match the minimum age for participation without parental consent, and since schizophrenia is usually seen in adults above this age. The study is open to all races, ethnicities, and genders. Selection of participants is based on psychiatric condition and is not based on gender or ethnic considerations, although these are expected to reflect the diverse population of San Francisco Bay Area, Los Angeles, and Boston. Ethnic minorities will be included when available and recruiting efforts will target a balanced enrollment.

The following inclusion/exclusion criteria will be determined through our screening procedures during V0, which includes structured interviews, as well as computerized and standardized neuropsychological assessments of attention, cognition and functional abilities.

Inclusion Criteria

  1. 1.

    Subjects must be between 18 and 65 years old at the time of study screening

  2. 2.

    Subjects must have a diagnosis of schizophrenia as defined by DSM-V criteria and confirmed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-P [88]). The SCID-P is a semi-structured clinical interview used to determine major mental disorders and personality disorders and will be used to confirm diagnosis of schizophrenia.

  3. 3.

    Subjects must demonstrate adequate decisional capacity, in the judgment of the consenting study staff member, to make a choice about participating in this research study.

  4. 4.

    Subjects must have been clinically stable (non-acute) for 8 weeks prior to consent; in the judgment of the Site Principal Investigator.

  5. 5.

    Subjects must have been maintained on a stable treatment of antipsychotics and/or other concomitant psychotropic treatment for at least 6 weeks prior to consent.

  6. 6.

    Subjects must have learned English before the age of 12 to ensure valid neuropsychological results.

  7. 7.

    Subjects must have the visual, auditory, and motor capacity to use the computerized intervention in the judgment of the consenting study staff person.

  8. 8.

    Subjects must have no more than a moderate severity rating on hallucinations and unusual thought content as shown by a score of  ≤ 4 on the Positive and Negative Symptoms Scale (PANSS [89]).

Exclusion criteria

  1. 1.

    Subjects should not have had a psychiatric hospitalization in the 8 weeks prior to consent.

  2. 2.

    Subjects who appear to be intoxicated or under the influence of a controlled substance on any day of assessment must be rescheduled or discontinued based upon the discretion of the site staff evaluator.

  3. 3.

    Subjects should not have a history of mental retardation (IQ < 70 based on The Wechsler Test of Adult Reading, WTAR [90]) or pervasive developmental disorder; or other neurological disorder (e.g., Traumatic Brain Injury, epilepsy, Parkinson’s Disease)

  4. 4.

    Subjects should not have been treated within 5 years of the date of consent with a computer-based cognitive training program manufactured by Posit Science.

  5. 5.

    Subjects should not be participating in a concurrent clinical trial that, in the judgment of the Site Principal Investigator, could affect the outcome of this one.

  6. 6.

    Subjects should not be prescribed more than two anti-psychotics. Subjects should not be treated with medication(s) with a total Cogentin equivalent greater than 4.5 mg (known anti-cholinergic side effects)

  7. 7.

    Subjects who have answered ‘yes’ to Question 5 (Active Suicidal Ideation with Specific Plan and Intent) on the Columbia-Suicide Severity Rating Scale (C-SSRS [91]), or who have answered ‘yes’ to any of the suicide-related behaviors (actual attempt, interrupted attempt, aborted attempt, preparatory act or behavior) on the C-SSRS “Suicidal Behavior” portion shall be excluded from the study if ideation or behavior occurred within two months of consent. Subjects excluded for this reason will be referred for appropriate treatment.

Repeated Assessment Battery (Outcome Measures)

Once a participant is deemed eligible for participation based on their V0 results, they are next scheduled for their baseline sessions on the repeated assessment battery (V1-V2), which take approximately four hours total. After completing these baseline assessments, participants are randomly assigned to either experimental or control training conditions (see below). Then, after half of the training is completed (20 sessions), participants are given the repeated assessment battery (V4-V5), and again at the completion of the entire training protocol (20 additional sessions for a total of 40 sessions; V6-V7).

We will employ a battery of neuropsychological and functional assessments (see Table 3), measuring a range of parameters from proximal to very distal from program use (e.g., assessment of functional abilities and quality of life). This structure will allow us to determine the degree of transfer of benefit to untrained modalities, including the extent to which improvements generalize to untrained functional ability and real-world experience (e.g., quality of life). Assessments will be administered to all participants, including those enrolled in treatment and active control groups; alternate forms of the assessments will be used when available to mitigate test-retest effects. Site psychometricians conducting the assessments will be blinded to group allocation and will receive training by a member of the coordinating center staff regarding appropriate administration of all measures, scoring and data reporting to ensure data quality. Performance on all measures will be scored, submitted by the participating site into the study database, and monitored for accuracy and integrity by the Coordinating Center.

Table 3 TRuSST Study primary and secondary outcome measures

Primary Outcome Measures

The FDA has indicated through the MATRICS (Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia) guidelines that it requires two co-primary endpoints for trials of cognitive enhancement in schizophrenia, a cognitive endpoint and a functional endpoint. We will follow these recommendations and include two co-primary outcome measures, a social cognitive outcome measure and a functional capacity measure. Our selections also adhere to the NIH toolbox recommendations regarding assessments, as this trial is sponsored by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH).

Social Cognitive Outcome Measure

The a priori co-primary social cognitive outcome measure will be a composite score comprised of a standardized, validated set of assessments that tap into low-level and high-level SC (social cognition) abilities (see [92]). A composite score will be derived from all five assessments to provide a social cognitive co-primary measure which encompasses several SC abilities. All assessments have been validated and used in outcome studies [68].

The low-level social cognitive assessments are:

  1. 1.

    ER40 (The Penn Emotional Recognition Test)[93] is a computerized test to assess categorical identification of facial emotions. The tasks consists of 40 digital pictures of faces. The photos are presented individually and subjects are required to choose the most appropriate emotion label from five possible emotion choices (happiness, sadness, anger, fear, or no emotion). The 40 photos are of 8 actors, and there are 8 photos of neutral expressions, 4 low intensity and 4 high intensity photos for each emotion. The index of accuracy is the total number of correct items and response time. This measure was found to exhibit 70% accuracy of identifying emotions, with concurrent validity of .83, convergent validity of .79 and divergent validity of .09.

  2. 2.

    PROID (Prosody Identification)[94] is a computerized vocal identification task used to assess a subject’s ability to perceive and discriminate emotion in the speech of others. All stimuli have been validated through trials with healthy controls.

  3. 3.

    PFMT (Penn Faces Memory Test)[93] consists of 20 target faces and 40 foil faces; Stimuli are black and white photographs of faces, balanced for gender and age. All faces are of neutral expression. Participants are requested to view the pictures, and are then tested immediately and after a delay on the pictures they have seen, to determine if they were in the set they memorized or not.

The high-level social cognitive measures are:

  1. 1.

    MSCEIT (Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test)[95] managing emotions subscale has two subtests that assess how participants manage the emotions of others (Social Management) and how a person would regulate his or her own emotions (Emotion Management). The test-retest reliability has been found to be .86 over a three-week interval and the full-test split-half reliabilities range from .91-.93. Discriminant and convergent validity has been demonstrated.

  2. 2.

    The EA Task (Empathic Accuracy) [32] is an empathy measure, in which subjects are shown multimodal video stimuli and are required to make continuous inferences about a target’s specific thoughts and feelings, which are later compared to the targets’ reported actual thoughts and feelings in order to compute an index of the perceiver’s accuracy. Empathic accuracy has been shown to demonstrate adequate test-retest reliability (.72).

Functional Capacity Outcome Measure

The a priori co-primary functional measure will be the functional capacity measure of The UCSD Performance-based Skills Assessment (UPSA-2)[96].The UPSA is a well-validated measure frequently used in cognitive and SC training studies [68]; In addition, it measures functional capacity, which is expected to be affected by social cognitive change. The UPSA-2 is designed to assess skills in five areas (Household Chores, Communication, Finance, Transportation, and Planning Recreational Activities) that reflect general abilities that are important components of independent living. Test-retest reliability ranged from .63-.80 over follow-up periods up to 36 months in patients with schizophrenia. Among patients, the UPSA performance correlated significantly with severity of negative symptoms and of cognitive impairment but not with that of positive or depressive symptoms.

Secondary Outcome Measures

The following validated and normed assessments will be used as secondary outcome measures in the study:

Clinical Status and Symptom Severity

We will use the PANSS (Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale) [89] to assess clinical status and severity of symptoms. The PANSS is a clinically-administered exam used for measuring symptom severity of patients with schizophrenia of 30 different symptoms, divided into Positive, Negative, and General Psychopathology scales. These scales have been found by coefficient alpha, split-half method, and test-retest reliability testing to be internally consistent and highly reliable [97].

Functioning

The following set of outcome measures will be used to assess global and social functioning:

  1. 1.

    GFS (Global Functioning Scale)[98, 99] is a clinician-administered questionnaire which is used to assess general psychosocial functioning. It has two subscales: role and social.

  2. 2.

    SFS (Social Functioning Scale)[100] is a self-report questionnaire designed to assess social functioning in individuals with schizophrenia. To be completed by both the subject and a relative, the scale is made up of 54 questions divided into seven sections. Results from three samples show the SFS is reliable, valid, sensitive and responsive to change.

  3. 3.

    SLOF (Specific Levels of Functioning)[101] is a 43-item scale designed to assess in detail an individual’s basic living skills and level of independent functioning. Item reliabilities are .62 in community programs and 0.42 in state hospital; internal consistency > =.91.

  4. 4.

    VRFCAT (Virtual Reality Functional Capacity Assessment)[85] is a virtual reality measure mimicking a real-life scenario of a shopping trip. The test has several alternate forms and it records number of errors and RT for task completion.

  5. 5.

    The QLS (Quality of Life Scale)[102] will be used as a secondary outcome measure to assess quality of life. QLS is a 16-item instrument used to measure five conceptual domains of quality of life. It has low to moderate correlations with physical health status and disease measures; however, content validity analysis indicates that the instrument measures domains that diverse patient groups with chronic illness define as quality of life.

Motivation

The following secondary outcome measures will be used to assess motivation:

  1. 1.

    BIS/BAS (Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Activation Scale)[103] is a 24-item self-report questionnaire designed to assess the two general motivational systems that underlie behavior and affect, i.e. sensitivity to anticipated punishment or reward. Test-retest correlations were found to range from .59-.69.

  2. 2.

    TEPS (Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale)[104] is a measure specifically designed to capture the anticipatory and consummatory facets of pleasure. The 10-item anticipatory pleasure scale and 8-item consummatory pleasure scale were found to be internally consistent, temporally stable and moderately, positively correlated with each other.

Social Cognition

The following social cognition measures will be used as secondary outcome measures, in addition to the primary social cognitive outcome measures:

  1. 1.

    TASIT (The Awareness of Social Interaction Test)[105], part 3. The TASIT is a social perception test in which subjects watch scenes which involve lie/sarcasm and are asked questions about them; We will use the 3rd part of the test, the Social Interference Enriched test. Test-retest reliability has been found to range from .74-.88 and alternative forms reliability ranges from .62-.83.

  2. 2.

    The Morphed Faces task [106] is a computerized emotion perception task, in which participants are presented with faces that are morphed between a neutral expression and an emotional expression: happy, disgusted, angry, or fearful. All faces are morphed between a neutral expression and either 20, 30, 40, 50, or 60 of the emotional expression, and are created from 1 male, and 2 female targets, resulting in a total of 60 face stimuli (3 targets, 5 levels of morph, 4 emotions).

  3. 3.

    The Faux Pas test [107] will be used as a Theory of Mind (ToM) measure. The test is comprised of 20 short stories, incidents of faux pas (someone mistakenly saying something they shouldn't have). Stories are read to the individual, who is then asked questions to determine whether or not they recognized the faux pas.

  4. 4.

    The Source Memory Test [31] is a measure of memory for the source of self-generated, and experimenter-provided word items that shows strong associations to social cognition[108]

  5. 5.

    AIHQ (The Ambiguous Intentions Hostility Questionnaire)[109] is a measure of attributional style, and specifically of hostile social-cognitive biases comprised of a variety of negative situations that differ in terms of intentionality. This measure has demonstrated good levels of internal consistency and inter-rater reliability, and is positively correlated with paranoia and hostility but not correlated with measures of psychosis proneness (convergent and discriminant validity).

Randomization

Participants will be randomized after the last baseline visit (V2) and before the planned program set-up visit (V3), which is the first day of program use. All V0-V2 data for each participant must be fully monitored, with all queries resolved, before randomization may take place.

Given the potential importance of cognitive abilities on the response to social cognitive training, participants will be stratified by gender, education (<13 years, >13 years) and age (18–40 years, 41–65 years) and randomly assigned to either treatment (SocialVille) or active control group at each site with an allocation ratio of 1:1. We will employ a minimization method of adaptive stratified randomization (referred to as the ‘platinum standard’ of randomization methods when stratification is required) to minimize the imbalance between the number of participants in each group over these factors.

For this trial, we will use a secure randomization server (Sealed Envelope) that implements the specified procedure and an unblinded study member Site Coordinating Center will issue a randomization assignment at the appropriate time. This approach represents a best practice approach to randomization, implementing an automated centralized group assignment procedure with allocation concealment, and effective separation of sequence generation and allocation concealment.

Blinding

Un-blinded Site Roles

At each site, Cognitive Remediation Coaches are un-blinded in order to provide support for participants using their assigned programs. They will be distinct from staff administering and scoring assessments. Additionally, Site Sub-Investigators authorized to register participants within the TRuSST system will remain un-blinded and may not participate in the assessment, evaluation, or follow-up of study participants.

Blinded Site Roles

All site staff responsible for the administration and scoring of participant assessments will remain blinded to participant treatment. Site Principal Investigators will be required to complete a Delegation of Authority Form prior to the start of the study, indicating which activities individual site research team members will be authorized to complete. Site Principal Investigators will also remain blinded.

Depending upon the extent to which they are responsible for data collection and/or entry, Clinical Research Coordinators may or may not remain un-blinded to participant treatment. This will be clarified on a site-by-site basis and will be noted on the Site Principal Investigator Delegation of Authority Form.

To prevent un-blinding, the following safeguards will be instituted at each site:

  1. 1.

    The treatment condition and the control condition will be identified as “Treatment A” and “Treatment B”;

  2. 2.

    Participants will be reminded not to discuss details related to treatment with psychometricians and/or clinical evaluators during the informed consent process as well as prior to initiation and at the conclusion of each assessment visit;

  3. 3.

    Site personnel will be instructed to not discuss details of either treatment arm during open participant groups or forums;

  4. 4.

    Sites will be required to execute the protocol in a manner that minimizes the possibility of accidental un-blinding of psychometricians or clinical evaluators (e.g. unintended viewing of treatment sessions);

  5. 5.

    Sites will be asked to post signage in appropriate areas throughout the facility reminding staff and participants to not discuss treatment details in open locations.

At the end of the trial, psychometricians will be asked questions designed to evaluate the integrity of the blinding procedures employed throughout the trial.

Description of Treatment Programs

Experimental Treatment Program (SocialVille)

The Experimental Treatment Program (SocialVille) is a computerized social cognitive remediation program consisting of a set of specific social cognitive exercises. To use the experimental treatment program, a participant opens a standard web browser on a broadband connected computer and goes to the experimental treatment program study web site. The participant then logs into the experimental treatment program server (using a study provided login that contains no personally identifiable information). The participant completes 7 cognitive exercise blocks scheduled for the day, and performs each exercise for about 6 minutes (see Fig. 2). Participants perform tens to hundreds of trials over the course of a session, with auditory and visual feedback and rewards to indicate if the trial was performed correctly or incorrectly. After each session, the difficulty of the next session is updated (e.g., more distractors in the response array) to ensure that each participant is appropriately challenged. Summary screens including game metrics (points, levels) and exercise metrics (usage, progress) are shown to the participant at the end of each session.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Examples of SocialVille Training Exercises. a. The daily schedule consists of 7 exercises of 6 minutes each, for a total of 42 minutes. b. Match that Feeling exercise example. In this exercise, participants are required to match the emotion depicted by the person in the target image to the emotion of a different person from a group of faces. C. Face Facts exercise example. In this exercise, participants must remember the given facts associated with a specific person

All usage and progress data are encrypted then transmitted to a central server. In a research study such as this one, no personally identifiable information is stored on the server (including internet protocol addresses). On the server, the data are available for review by the un-blinded Cognitive Remediation Coach or Site Coordinator through a secure web portal. Only data from participants at a particular Site can be viewed by that Site’s staff. The Cognitive Remediation Coach in particular will use the secure web portal to regularly check on usage and progress of each active participant to customize their weekly phone/in-person discussions to provide helpful guidance and coaching.

There are multiple social cognitive exercises in SocialVille, collectively targeting the five social cognitive domains identified in the literature: affect perception, social cue perception, theory of mind (ToM), self-referential style and empathy (see Table 2 for a complete list of exercises). All exercises continuously adjust difficulty level to user performance to maintain a 70-80% correct performance rate using adaptive algorithms. The scheduling mechanism ensures that a participant progresses through the exercises in a defined order, generally moving from more simple (e.g., easy to discriminate stimulus types, less response options) exercises to more complex (e.g., greater rule complexity, greater similarity between stimuli, etc.) exercises over the course of the 8–12 weeks experience.

Active Control Program (commercially-available computer games)

The active control group will use conventional, progressive computer games. The 13 computer games (see Table 3 for full list) have been embedded in the training portal; this should greatly facilitate administration, and will allow maintenance of the double-blind procedure of both staff and participants, while controlling for placebo effects, time spent on computer, exposure to study staff, and non-specific effects from attended, rewarded exposure to multi-media stimulation.

This type of control is suitable for an efficacy trial, and is designed to approximate the same level of challenge as in the active training group. Moreover, it is the only possible control in the absence of a standardized social cognitive treatment in schizophrenia: a social skills group control would not control for number of clinic visits, interaction with study staff and other patients, and in addition it is unclear which of the potential group treatments should be chosen; A computerized social cognitive treatment is not conventionally employed, and available experimental ones critically differ from the SocialVille intervention in scope (targeting only a single SC domain) hence may not serve as a true control.

The active control intervention is administered in exactly the same way as the treatment intervention: it is browser-playable (from any browser) and login and password-protected. We randomly select a subset of 7 games subjects play on every session, for 6 minutes each, to equate for the time spent by the treatment group participants (see Fig. 3). The number, availability, and time spent on each game is managed by the same server which manages the treatment group exercises, hence experience is matched between the two groups. We will use games that have been shown to provide face-valid cognitive stimulation and that are rated E (for everyone) by the Entertainment Software Rating Board (ESRB).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Examples of Active Control (AC) Program Training Exercises. a. This daily schedule for the AC program is the same as the SocialVille program, with 7 exercises presented per day for 6 minutes each. b. Some exercises include Tri Peaks Solitaire (3-deck solitaire), War Ship (Battleship), and Bricks Breaking Hex (remove tiles of the same color)

The Cognitive Remediation Coach in particular will use the secure web portal to regularly check on usage of each active participant to customize their weekly phone/email discussions to provide helpful guidance and coaching. This ensures that the experimental treatment and active control groups are matched for social contact and encouragement from the Cognitive Remediation Coach.

Power Calculation for Sample Size

With the aim of having 128 participants complete the study, we have statistically powered it to detect a between-groups Cohen’s d effect size of 0.50 on an outcome measure, calculated as the between-group difference in the treatment effect means (post-assessment score minus pre-assessment score) divided by the pooled standard deviation of the observed test-retest reliability. This effect size translates, for example, in an improvement of 5.8 points (on an IQ-like index score) within the treatment group versus an improvement of 1.0 point within the active control group, with both groups showing a variance of 10 points (2/3 of a standard deviation, as observed in other composite cognitive performance data) from pre-assessment to post-assessment. The results of our feasibility study [80] allowed us to derive training group effect sizes; these ranged from 0.45-1.1 on the SocialVille measures, from 0.53-0.77 for the social cognitive measures and from 0.4-0.67 for the generalization and functional measures. A recent study conducted by our study collaborator, Prof. Vinogradov [61] employed computerized social cognitive training and found effect sizes of 0.53 on the MSCEIT [94] perceiving emotions total subscale. Furthermore, a recent meta-analysis conducted on social cognitive training interventions in schizophrenia [68] found moderate to large effect sizes on facial affect recognition (Cohen’s d between 0.71-1.01) and small to moderate effect sizes on ToM (0.46). Effect size for total symptoms was found to be moderate to large (0.68). We therefore believe this is a reasonable estimate of the plausible effect size in the suggested trial, and that documentation of a 0.5 between-groups effect size in this trial would provide reasonable support for clinical benefit in this population.

Data Analysis

For the main analysis of this study (evaluating the efficacy of SocialVille as a treatment for social cognition deficits) we will define an Intent-To-Treat (ITT) population that includes all participants who have been randomized to either group. We will compare treatment and active control groups in the ITT population to determine if any differences in baseline demographic, characterization, outcomes variables, or total program use time remain after the randomization process.

We will test the following hypotheses of the primary outcomes, in addition to exploratory analyses: (1) Experimental treatment versus active control improves social cognition, and (2) Experimental treatment versus active control improves functional capacity. In addition, we will test the following hypotheses of the secondary outcomes: (1) Experimental treatment versus active control improves symptom severity; (2) Experimental treatment versus active control improves functioning; (3) Experimental treatment versus active control improves social cognition; (4) Experimental treatment versus active control improves motivation, and (5) Experimental treatment versus active control improves quality of life.

To examine each hypothesis, we will examine the data from each outcome measure(s) associated with the Primary or Secondary outcomes using a linear mixed-effects model with group and time as fixed factors, site as a random factor, and additional factors/covariates as required if there are trends towards significant baseline differences (p < 0.1) in the treatment and active control groups. Missing data will be handled with an iterative maximum likelihood procedure to optimally estimate model parameters. The key value for significance will be the group-by-time interaction term. This modeling will be conducted with a Type I error set at 0.025 for each model.

In addition, to identify variables that predict treatment success in the SocialVille group, we will use an individual differences approach [80] and test for associations between not only outcome variables and pre-test scores (raw and composite), but also between outcome variables and improvements in aspects of the training tasks. This will help us determine if specific types of patients benefit from training more than others as well as if particular improvement patterns on training (i.e., training strategies) are predictive of successful outcomes. These analyses will be evaluated after correcting for multiple comparisons (i.e., Bonferroni).

Finally, following the main analyses, we will construct two separate composites, of low-level and high-level SC assessments, respectively, and test the effects of training on each of these composites using a linear mixed-effects model. We will further examine correlations between magnitude of change on each of the constructs and % improvement on SocialVille exercises.

Discussion

Despite the functional significance of social cognition to everyday function, there is currently no effective and widely-adopted treatment approach for the extremely debilitating social cognition deficits seen in schizophrenia. The current TRuSST protocol shall help determine whether SocialVille is an effective treatment method for social cognition deficits in schizophrenia, promoting social and functional benefits that could potentially improve the quality of life of afflicted individuals and their family members. The fact that SocialVille is a highly-scalable intervention, deployed to any number of individuals at minimal cost and does not require additional clinical training, strengthens the notion that a successful TRuSST trial could result in the rapid utilization of SocialVille in schizophrenia treatment, creating a new and improved standard of care for this condition.

Strengths

A major strength of the study is that it deploys simple interventions that can be administered completely remotely, with minimal remote clinical monitoring, planned for a phone call or email once/week to check in on participants and see if they encounter any difficulty completing their training. Deploying interventions remotely and saving the multiple clinical visits should potentially facilitate compliance with study requirements, and allow to mimic real-life deployment of the training program.

In addition, many previous studies and clinical trials testing social cognitive benefits in schizophrenia lacked statistical power due to relatively small sample sizes (e.g. [66, 70, 110113]) or the lack of a control intervention (e.g. [84, 114, 115]). The TRuSST sample size, of 128 individuals, should power it to detect appropriate effect sizes. In fact, TRuSST is one of largest trials, to the best of our knowledge that were conducted in this field of social cognition in schizophrenia. The fact that it is a double-blind study further strengthens the credibility of its potential outcomes, which cannot be attributed to placebo effects or interaction with study staff.

Finally, the study employs a very large battery of outcome measures, encompassing the domains of social cognition, social functioning, quality of life and functional capacity. Specifically, the study employs some novel assessments as secondary outcome measures (e.g. VRFCAT [83]), and should thus help validate and norm these novel measures for schizophrenia, on the path of making them the new gold standard in the field.

Weaknesses

Our protocol has some potential limitations. Due to the relatively long duration and multiple components of our study, attrition rates may pose a potential limitation, making it difficult to reach our recruitment goal. Furthermore, one of the common symptoms of schizophrenia is a lack of motivation [116, 117], which could prevent timely completions of the self-initiated training program. With a standardized protocol, frequent check-ins, and regular feedback from our research assistants, we aim to limit drop-out rate and motivate individuals to continue with training through completion.

Trial Status

The trial is currently in the recruitment phase.

Abbreviations

TRuSST:

Treatment of Social Cognition in Schizophrenia Trial

RCT:

Randomized Controlled Trial

STS:

Superior temporal sulcus

mPFC:

Medial Prefrontal Cortex

SCIT:

Social Cognition and Interaction Training

SCST:

Social Cognitive Skills Training

ETIT:

Emotion and ToM Imitation Training

SCET:

Social Cognition Enhancement Training

TAR:

Tackling Affect Recognition

METT:

Micro Expression Training Tool

VRFCAT:

Virtual Reality Functional Capacity Assessment Tool

AC:

Active Control

SCID-P:

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-V, Patient Edition

PANSS:

Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale

WTAR:

Wechsler Test of Adult Reading

C-SSRS:

Columbia Suicide Severity Rating Scale

ER40:

Penn Emotion Recognition Test

PROID:

Prosody Identification

PFMT:

Penn Facial Memory Test

MSCEIT:

Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test

EA:

Empathic Accuracy

UPSA-2:

UCSD Performance-based Skills Assessment

GFS:

Global Functioning Scale

SFS:

Social Functioning Scale

SLOF:

Specific Levels of Functioning

BIS/BAS:

Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Activation Scale

TEPS:

Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale

QLS:

Quality of Life Scale

TASIT:

The Awareness of Social Interaction Test

ToM:

Theory of Mind

AIHQ:

Ambiguous Intentions Hostility Questionnaire

ESRB:

Entertainment Software Rating Board

ITT:

Intent-To-Treat

WIRB:

Western International Review Board

MATRICS:

Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophreni

NIMH:

National Institute of Mental Health

SC:

Social Cognition

References

  1. Regier DA, Narrow WE, Rae DS, Manderscheid RW, Locke BZ, Goodwin FK. The de facto US mental and addictive disorders service system. Epidemiologic catchment area prospective 1-year prevalence rates of disorders and services. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:85–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Association AP, Association AP, editors. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5, 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM, Goldberg TE, Green MF, Heaton RK. Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:29–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Couture SM, Penn DL, Roberts DL. The Functional Significance of Social Cognition in Schizophrenia: A Review. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32:S44–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Perkins DO, Lieberman J. Implications for the Neural Basis of Social Cognition for the Study of Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:815–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Penn DL, Sanna LJ, Roberts DL. Social Cognition in Schizophrenia: An Overview. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:408–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Savla GN, Vella L, Armstrong CC, Penn DL, Twamley EW. Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39:979–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2009;109:1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E, Hox J, van Engeland H. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci. 2007;191:5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Kohler CG, Walker JB, Martin EA, Healey KM, Moberg PJ. Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36:1009–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Adolphs R. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:165–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Brothers L. The social brain: A project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts Neurosci. 1990;1:27–51.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Fiske ST, Taylor SE (2008) Social Cognition: From Brains to Culture. McGraw-Hill Higher Education, Boston

  14. Kunda Z (1999) Social Cognition: Making Sense of People. MIT Press

  15. Augoustinos M, Walker I, Donaghue N (2014) Social Cognition: An Integrated Introduction, 2nd edition. SAGE Publications Ltd, London

  16. Eliot R, Smith GRS. Socially Situated Cognition: Cognition in its Social Context. ADVAN EXP SOC PSYCHOL. 2004;36:53–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Mueser KT, Bellack AS. Social skills and social functioning in schizophrenia. In: Tarrier N, editor. Mueser KT. Boston, MA, US: Handb. Soc. Funct. Schizophr. Allyn & Bacon; 1998. p. 79–98.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Green MF, Penn DL, Bentall R, Carpenter WT, Gaebel W, Gur RC, et al. Social Cognition in Schizophrenia: An NIMH Workshop on Definitions, Assessment, and Research Opportunities. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:1211–20.

  19. Edwards J, Jackson HJ, Pattison PE. Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: A methodological review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2002;22:789–832.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Mandal MK, Pandey R, Prasad AB. Facial Expressions of Emotions and Schizophrenia: A Review. Schizophr Bull. 1998;24:399–412.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Addington J, Addington D. Facial affect recognition and information processing in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res. 1998;32:171–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Perkins DO, Graham KA, Siegel M. Emotion perception and social skill over the course of psychosis: A comparison of individuals “at-risk” for psychosis and individuals with early and chronic schizophrenia spectrum illness. Cognit Neuropsychiatry. 2007;12:198–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Williams LM, Loughland CM, Green MJ, Harris AWF, Gordon E. Emotion perception in schizophrenia: an eye movement study comparing the effectiveness of risperidone vs. haloperidol. Psychiatry Res. 2003;120:13–27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Hellewell JSE, Whittake JF. Affect perception and social knowledge in schizophrenia. In: Tarrier N, editor. Mueser KT. Needham Heights, MA, US: Handb. Soc. Funct. Schizophr. Allyn & Bacon; 1998. p. 197–212.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Green MJ, Phillips ML. Social threat perception and the evolution of paranoia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;28:333–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Corrigan PW, Green MF. Schizophrenic patients’ sensitivity to social cues: the role of abstraction. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150:589–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Frith CD, Corcoran R. Exploring “theory of mind” in people with schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1996;26:521–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Harrington L, Siegert R, McClure J. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: A critical review. Cognit Neuropsychiatry. 2005;10:249–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Marjoram D, Gardner C, Burns J, Miller P, Lawrie S, Johnstone E. Symptomatology and social inference: A theory of mind study of schizophrenia and psychotic affective disorder. Cognit Neuropsychiatry. 2005;10:347–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Brüne M, Abdel-Hamid M, Lehmkämper C, Sonntag C. Mental state attribution, neurocognitive functioning, and psychopathology: What predicts poor social competence in schizophrenia best? Schizophr Res. 2007;92:151–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Vinogradov S, Willis-Shore J, Poole JH, Marten E, Ober BA, Shenaut GK. Clinical and Neurocognitive Aspects of Source Monitoring Errors in Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:1530–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Lee J, Zaki J, Harvey P-O, Ochsner K, Green MF. Schizophrenia patients are impaired in empathic accuracy. Psychol Med. 2011;41:2297–304.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Adolphs R. The Social Brain: Neural Basis of Social Knowledge. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:693–716.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Adolphs R. Social cognition and the human brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 1999;3:469–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Botvinick M, Jha AP, Bylsma LM, Fabian SA, Solomon PE, Prkachin KM. Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain. NeuroImage. 2005;25:312–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Wicker B, Keysers C, Plailly J, Royet J-P, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G. Both of Us Disgusted in My Insula: The Common Neural Basis of Seeing and Feeling Disgust. Neuron. 2003;40:655–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Lamm C, Batson CD, Decety J. The Neural Substrate of Human Empathy: Effects of Perspective-taking and Cognitive Appraisal. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:42–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Fakra E, Salgado-Pineda P, Delaveau P, Hariri AR, Blin O. Neural bases of different cognitive strategies for facial affect processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2008;100:191–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Stanley DA, Adolphs R. Toward a neural basis for social behavior. Neuron. 2013;80:816–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Pinkham AE, Hopfinger JB, Ruparel K, Penn DL. An Investigation of the Relationship Between Activation of a Social Cognitive Neural Network and Social Functioning. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:688–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Gur RE, McGrath C, Chan RM, et al. An fMRI Study of Facial Emotion Processing in Patients With Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1992–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Schneider F, Weiss U, Kessler C, Salloum JB, Posse S, Grodd W, et al. Differential amygdala activation in schizophrenia during sadness. Schizophr Res. 1998;34:133–42.

  43. Russell TA, Rubia K, Bullmore ET, Soni W, Suckling J, Brammer MJ, et al. Exploring the Social Brain in Schizophrenia: Left Prefrontal Underactivation During Mental State Attribution. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:2040–2.

  44. Dodell-Feder D, Tully LM, Lincoln SH, Hooker CI. The neural basis of theory of mind and its relationship to social functioning and social anhedonia in individuals with schizophrenia. NeuroImage Clin. 2014;4:154–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Albert N, Bertelsen M, Thorup A, Petersen L, Jeppesen P, Le Quack P, et al. Predictors of recovery from psychosis: Analyses of clinical and social factors associated with recovery among patients with first-episode psychosis after 5 years. Schizophr Res. 2011;125:257–66.

  46. Corrigan PW, Hirschbeck JN, Wolfe M. Memory and vigilance training to improve social perception in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1995;17:257–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Bell M, Tsang HWH, Greig TC, Bryson GJ. Neurocognition, Social Cognition, Perceived Social Discomfort, and Vocational Outcomes in Schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:738–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Fett A-KJ, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez M-G, Penn DL, van Os J, Krabbendam L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:573–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Addington J, Saeedi H, Addington D. Influence of social perception and social knowledge on cognitive and social functioning in early psychosis. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;189:373–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Sergi MJ, Rassovsky Y, Widmark C, Reist C, Erhart S, Braff DL, et al. Social cognition in schizophrenia: Relationships with neurocognition and negative symptoms. Schizophr Res. 2007;90:316–24.

  51. Vauth R, Rüsch N, Wirtz M, Corrigan PW. Does social cognition influence the relation between neurocognitive deficits and vocational functioning in schizophrenia? Psychiatry Res. 2004;128:155–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Choi K-H, Kim J, Lee J, Green MF. Social cognition training for individuals with schizophrenia: A review of targeted interventions. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci. 2009;7:29–38.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Horan WP, Green MF, DeGroot M, et al. Social Cognition in Schizophrenia, Part 2: 12-Month Stability and Prediction of Functional Outcome in First-Episode Patients. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:865–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Edwards J, Pattison PE, Jackson HJ, Wales RJ. Facial affect and affective prosody recognition in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2001;48:235–53.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Green MF, Bearden CE, Cannon TD, et al. Social Cognition in Schizophrenia, Part 1: Performance Across Phase of Illness. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:854–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Mueser KT, Doonan R, Penn DL, Blanchard JJ, Bellack AS, Nishith P, et al. Emotion recognition and social competence in chronic schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1996;105:271–5.

  57. Salem JE, Kring AM, Kerr SL. More evidence for generalized poor performance in facial emotion perception in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1996;105:480–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Green MF. Stimulating the Development of Drug Treatments to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2007;3:159–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Swartz MS, Perkins DO, Stroup TS, et al. Effects of antipsychotic medications on psychosocial functioning in patients with chronic schizophrenia: findings from the NIMH CATIE study. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:428–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Keefe RSE, Bilder RM, Davis SM, et al. Neurocognitive effects of antipsychotic medications in patients with chronic schizophrenia in the CATIE Trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:633–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Sacks S, Fisher M, Garrett C, Alexander P, Holland C, Rose D, et al. Combining Computerized Social Cognitive Training with Neuroplasticity-Based Auditory Training in Schizophrenia. Clin Schizophr Relat Psychoses. 2013;7:78–86A.

  62. Bell MD, Zito W, Greig T, Wexler BE. Neurocognitive enhancement therapy with vocational services: Work outcomes at two-year follow-up. Schizophr Res. 2008;105:18–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Roder V, Mueller DR, Mueser KT, Brenner HD. Integrated Psychological Therapy (IPT) for Schizophrenia: Is It Effective? Schizophr Bull. 2006;32:S81–93.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Hogarty GE, Flesher S, Ulrich R, et al. Cognitive enhancement therapy for schizophrenia: effects of a 2-year randomized trial on cognition and behavior. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:866–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Wölwer W, Frommann N, Halfmann S, Piaszek A, Streit M, Gaebel W. Remediation of impairments in facial affect recognition in schizophrenia: Efficacy and specificity of a new training program. Schizophr Res. 2005;80:295–303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Combs DR, Penn DL, Tiegreen JA, Nelson A, Ledet SN, Basso MR, et al. Stability and generalization of Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for schizophrenia: Six-month follow-up results. Schizophr Res. 2009;112:196–7.

  67. Bartholomeusz CF, Allott K. Neurocognitive and Social Cognitive Approaches for Improving Functional Outcome in Early Psychosis: Theoretical Considerations and Current State of Evidence. Schizophr Res Treat. 2012;2012, e815315.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Kurtz MM, Richardson CL. Social Cognitive Training for Schizophrenia: A Meta-Analytic Investigation of Controlled Research. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:1092–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Penn D, Roberts DL, Munt ED, Silverstein E, Jones N, Sheitman B. A pilot study of social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2005;80:357–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Combs DR, Adams SD, Penn DL, Roberts D, Tiegreen J, Stem P. Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for inpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Preliminary findings. Schizophr Res. 2007;91:112–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Roberts DL, Penn DL. Social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for outpatients with schizophrenia: A preliminary study. Psychiatry Res. 2009;166:141–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Horan WP, Kern RS, Shokat-Fadai K, Sergi MJ, Wynn JK, Green MF. Social cognitive skills training in schizophrenia: An initial efficacy study of stabilized outpatients. Schizophr Res. 2009;107:47–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Mazza M, Lucci G, Pacitti F, Pino MC, Mariano M, Casacchia M, et al. Could schizophrenic subjects improve their social cognition abilities only with observation and imitation of social situations? Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2010;20:675–703.

  74. Choi K-H, Kwon J-H. Social Cognition Enhancement Training for Schizophrenia: A Preliminary Randomized Controlled Trial. Community Ment Health J. 2006;42:177–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Frommann N, Streit M, Wölwer W. Remediation of facial affect recognition impairments in patients with schizophrenia: a new training program. Psychiatry Res. 2003;117:281–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Merzenich MM, Van Vleet TM, Nahum M. Brain plasticity-based therapeutics. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:385.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Cramer SC, Sur M, Dobkin BH, et al. Harnessing neuroplasticity for clinical applications. Brain J Neurol. 2011;134:1591–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Levitt H. Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. J Acoust Soc Am. 1971;49 Suppl 2:467+.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. King-Smith PE, Grigsby SS, Vingrys AJ, Benes SC, Supowit A. Efficient and unbiased modifications of the QUEST threshold method: theory, simulations, experimental evaluation and practical implementation. Vision Res. 1994;34:885–912.

  80. Nahum M, Fisher M, Loewy R, Poelke G, Ventura J, Nuechterlein KH, et al. A novel, online social cognitive training program for young adults with schizophrenia: A pilot study. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2014;1:e11–9.

  81. Hooker CI, Carol EE, Eisenstein TJ, Yin H, Lincoln SH, Tully LM, et al. A pilot study of cognitive training in clinical high risk for psychosis: initial evidence of cognitive benefit. Schizophr Res. 2014;157:314–6.

  82. Paquin K, Wilson AL, Cellard C, Lecomte T, Potvin S. A systematic review on improving cognition in schizophrenia: which is the more commonly used type of training, practice or strategy learning? BMC Psychiatry. 2014;14:139.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Tas C, Danaci AE, Cubukcuoglu Z, Brüne M. Impact of family involvement on social cognition training in clinically stable outpatients with schizophrenia – a randomized pilot study. Psychiatry Res. 2012;195:32–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Marsh P, Langdon R, McGuire J, Harris A, Polito V, Coltheart M. An open clinical trial assessing a novel training program for social cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Australas Psychiatry Bull R Aust N Z Coll Psychiatr. 2013;21:122–6.

    Google Scholar 

  85. Ruse SA, Harvey PD, Davis VG, Atkins AS, Fox KH, Keefe RSE. Virtual reality functional capacity assessment in schizophrenia: Preliminary data regarding feasibility and correlations with cognitive and functional capacity performance. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2014;1:e21–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Eack S, Greenwald D, Hogarty S, Cooley S, DiBarry AL, Montrose D, et al. Cognitive Enhancement Therapy for Early-Course Schizophrenia: Effects of a Two-Year Randomized Controlled Trial. Psychiatr Serv. 2009. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.60.11.1468.87.  Begg C, Choe M, Eastwood S, et al (1996) Improving the quality of reporting of randomized controlled trials. The CONSORT statement. JAMA 276:637-639

  87. Begg C, Choe M, Eastwood S, et al (1996) Improving the quality of reporting of randomized controlled trials. The CONSORT statement. JAMA 276:637-639

  88. First MB, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition. (SCID-I/P). New York: Biometrics Research, New York Psychiatric Institute; 2002.

  89. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for Schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261–76.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Holdnack J (2001) WTAR: Wechsler Test of Adult Reading manual. San Antonio TX Psychol. Corp.

  91. Posner K, Brown GK, Stanley B, et al. The Columbia–Suicide Severity Rating Scale: Initial Validity and Internal Consistency Findings From Three Multisite Studies With Adolescents and Adults. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:1266–77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  92. Mancuso F, Horan WP, Kern RS, Green MF. Social cognition in psychosis: Multidimensional structure, clinical correlates, and relationship with functional outcome. Schizophr Res. 2011;125:143–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Gur RC, Ragland JD, Moberg PJ, Turner TH, Bilker WB, Kohler C, et al. Computerized Neurocognitive Scanning: I. Methodology and Validation in Healthy People Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;25:766–76.

  94. Russ J. Development of PROID, a computerized emotional prosody identification task. PennScience J. 2008;6:14–20.

    Google Scholar 

  95. Mayer JD, Salovey P, Caruso DR, Sitarenios G. Measuring emotional intelligence with the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion. 2003;3:97–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Patterson TL, Goldman S, McKibbin CL, Hughs T, Jeste DV. UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment: Development of a New Measure of Everyday Functioning for Severely Mentally Ill Adults. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27:235–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Kay SR, Opler LA, Lindenmayer J-P. Reliability and validity of the positive and negative syndrome scale for schizophrenics. Psychiatry Res. 1988;23:99–110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Auther A, Smith C, Cornblatt B. Global Functioning Scale: Social (GFS: Social). Glen Oaks, NY: Zucker-Hillside Hospital; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  99. Niendam T, Bearden C, Johnson J, Cannon T. Global Functioning Scale: Role (GFS:Role). Los Angeles, CA: University of California; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  100. Birchwood M, Smith J, Cochrane R, Wetton S, Copestake S. The Social Functioning Scale. The development and validation of a new scale of social adjustment for use in family intervention programmes with schizophrenic patients. Br J Psychiatry. 1990;157:853–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Schneider LC, Struening EL. SLOF: a behavioral rating scale for assessing the mentally ill. Soc Work Res Abstr. 1983;19:9–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67:319–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Gard DE, Gard MG, Kring AM, John OP. Anticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: A scale development study. J Res Personal. 2006;40:1086–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Heinrichs DW, Hanlon TE, Carpenter WT. The Quality of Life Scale: An Instrument for Rating the Schizophrenic Deficit Syndrome. Schizophr Bull. 1984;10:388–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. McDonald S. Awareness of Social Inference Test: Manual. Thames Valley Test: Company; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  106. Germine LT, Hooker CI. Face emotion recognition is related to individual differences in psychosis-proneness. Psychol Med. 2011;41:937–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Baron-Cohen S, O’Riordan M, Jones R, Stone V, Plaisted K. A new test of social sensitivity: Detection of faux pas in normal children and children with Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29:407–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Fisher M, McCoy K, Poole JH, Vinogradov S. Self and Other in Schizophrenia: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:1465–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Combs DR, Penn DL, Wicher M, Waldheter E. The Ambiguous Intentions Hostility Questionnaire (AIHQ): A new measure for evaluating hostile social-cognitive biases in paranoia. Cognit Neuropsychiatry. 2007;12:128–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Fuentes I, García S, Ruiz JC, Soler MJ, Roder V. Social perception training in schizophrenia: a pilot study. Int. J. Psychol. Psychol. Ther. 2007; 7.1:1-2.

  111. García S, Fuentes I, Ruíz JC, Gallach E, Roder V. Application of the IPT in a spanish sample: evaluation of the “social perception subprogramme”. Int. J. Psychol. Psychol. Ther. 2003; 3.2:299-310.

  112. Hodel B, Kern RS, Brenner HD. Emotion Management Training (EMT) in persons with treatment-resistant schizophrenia: first results. Schizophr Res. 2004;68:107–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Kayser N, Sarfati Y, Besche C, Hardy-Baylé M-C. Elaboration of a rehabilitation method based on a pathogenetic hypothesis of “theory of mind” impairment in schizophrenia. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2006;16:83–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Penn DL, Roberts DL, Combs D, Sterne A. Best practices: The development of the Social Cognition and Interaction Training program for schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Psychiatr Serv Wash DC. 2007;58:449–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  115. Silver H, Goodman C, Knoll G, Isakov V. Brief emotion training improves recognition of facial emotions in chronic schizophrenia. A pilot study. Psychiatry Res. 2004;128:147–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Gard DE, Sanchez AH, Starr J, Cooper S, Fisher M, Rowlands A, et al. Using self-determination theory to understand motivation deficits in schizophrenia: the “why” of motivated behavior. Schizophr Res. 2014;156:217–22.

  117. Gard DE, Fisher M, Garrett C, Genevsky A, Vinogradov S. Motivation and its relationship to neurocognition, social cognition, and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2009;115:74–81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This project is funded by a Phase II SBIR Grant (R44MH091793) from the National Institute of Mental Health.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mor Nahum.

Additional information

Competing interests

MN and AR are paid employees of Posit Science, the developer of the SocialVille program.

Author’s contributions

MN conceived the study and drafted the manuscript. AR and MM helped with manuscript preparation and AR assisted in past and current schizophrenia research studies, study coordination and data management. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access  This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.

The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

To view a copy of this licence, visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rose, A., Vinogradov, S., Fisher, M. et al. Randomized controlled trial of computer-based treatment of social cognition in schizophrenia: the TRuSST trial protocol. BMC Psychiatry 15, 142 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-015-0510-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-015-0510-1

Keywords