Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Major depression epidemiology from a diathesis-stress conceptualization

Abstract

Background

Major depression is a widely used diagnostic category but there is increasing dissatisfaction with its performance. The diathesis-stress model is an alternative approach that does not require the (sometimes arbitrary) imposition of categories onto the spectrum of depressive morbidity. However, application of this model has not been well explored and its consistency with available epidemiologic data is uncertain.

Methods

Simulation provides an opportunity to explore these issues. In this study, a simulation model based on an intuitive representation of diathesis-stress interaction was developed. Both diathesis and stress were represented using continuous distributions, without categorization. A diagnostic threshold was then applied to the simulation output to create nominal categories and to explore their consistency with available information.

Results

An apparently complex epidemiologic pattern emerged from the diathesis-stress interaction when thresholds were applied: incidence was time dependent, recurrence depended on the number of past episodes, baseline symptoms were associated with an increased risk of subsequent episodes and the remission rate declined with increasing episode duration.

Conclusions

A diathesis-stress conceptualization coupled with application of a threshold-based diagnostic definition may explain several of the apparent complexities of major depression epidemiology. Some of these complexities may be artifacts of the nominal diagnostic approach. These observations should encourage an empirical exploration of whether diathesis-stress interactions provide a more parsimonious framework for understanding depression than current approaches.

Peer Review reports

Background

Depressive symptoms can be measured using rating scales, which provide an assessment of symptom severity on an ordinal or continuous scale. However, such ratings do not capture important aspects of the concept of a depressive disorder, as this is currently understood. Disorder definitions, and hence most available epidemiologic data, derive from nominal classifications (e.g. major depression (MD) as a named category rather than a scaled rating) that incorporate symptom severity but also thresholds for duration and severity of symptoms and require features such as dysfunction, distress or danger [1]. Diagnostic categories typically play a larger role than symptom ratings in medical practice because they align more closely with clinical decision-making. Since publication of third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III) in 1980 [2], epidemiologic studies have embraced this nominal framework, as have subsequent editions of the manual [1] and the International Classification of Disease [3]. This nominal framework implicitly adopts a particular theoretical orientation towards the epidemiology: an incidence-prevalence-duration framework. In this orientation, there is a “prevalence pool” of depressed people within the population, incidence is an inflow into this pool and the outflow from the pool is due to recovery, remission or mortality. A central aspect of this way of thinking is that the clinical course is characterized by discrete and identifiable transitions between disease states. This framework leads to many familiar epidemiologic concepts (e.g. that the prevalence odds is the product of an incidence rate and mean duration of illness) and is the basis for most disease-modeling approaches. An example of its implementation is the DisMod II program which was until recently used by the Global Burden of Disease project [4].

Categorization of an ordinal or continuous variable into nominal groups can create arbitrary distinctions and runs the risk of obscuring clinically meaningful features. Perhaps in recognition of this, the depression literature has seen the emergence of a complex terminology such as: partial remission [1], residual symptoms [5] and sub-syndromal or sub-threshold episodes [6, 7]. Epidemiologically, episodes of the latter type may be difficult to clearly distinguish from non-pathological fluctuations in mood status and adjustment disorders [8].

These difficulties are also evident in the small literature of simulation studies of this condition. All such studies to date have represented the epidemiology from a nominal perspective, employing the general epidemiologic paradigm described above [912]. Simulation studies have made an important contribution to the literature about MD epidemiology by indicating, starting with Giuffra and Risch [9] that epidemiologic estimates of lifetime prevalence for MD (which have generally fallen between 10% and 20%) may be underestimates due to recall bias [10, 11]. This result has subsequently been confirmed empirically [13, 14]. However, the impact of these simulation studies has perhaps been diminished by the complexity of their modeling strategies, e.g. [15].

When viewed through the lens of the general epidemiologic disease model described above, MD epidemiology is indeed complex. For example, there is no simple incidence rate. Incidence appears to decline over time with age [16, 17]. Also, there is no single recovery rate. The epidemiologic data indicate that the slope of the cumulative recovery curve for MD episodes is steeper in the early weeks of an episode than in the later weeks [18]. This has required the application of various strategies for modeling such as Markov “tunnels” [19], or the use of lognormal [18] or Weibull [12] distributions to depict a time-varying probability of remission. Various other complexities also emerge, such as the observation that sub-threshold episodes or elevated symptoms occur on a continuum with threshold-defined episodes [20] and are also associated with an increased risk of subsequent major depressive episodes [21]. Finally, the occurrence of depressive episodes in the past predicts their occurrence in the future such that respondents with a large number of recurrences are considered candidates for long-term treatment [22]. Representation of these characteristics, either conceptually in clinical practice or mathematically in a simulation model, involves consideration of complex time-dependent patterns of incidence and recovery and a multiplicity of MD-related health states. However, an interesting possibility is that some of the apparent complexity of MD epidemiology may arise merely as an artifact of forcing a presumably continuous phenomenon into nominal categories.

In order to explore this, it is necessary to initially adopt a non-categorical perspective, treating relevant variables as continuous ones, and then to apply a threshold-based definition to the data. A leading candidate for such a model is the diathesis-stress model, originally formulated (with mixed success) as an interaction between a risk-associated cognitive style and life events, see review by Joiner and Wagner [23], and elsewhere in a social model (including “vulnerability” and “provoking” factors) by Brown and Harris [24]. The concept is now more often applied (although usually without invocation of the “diathesis” and “stress” terminology, e.g. see [25] and [26]) with reference to genetic inheritance, epigenetic modification of gene expression and life events. A depressive diathesis may arise, for example, from multiple genes and from exposure to adversity during development [25]. Neither polygenetic inheritance nor psychosocial adversity fit into “yes” or “no” categories. Some of the depressive diathesis may arise through epigenetic mechanisms due to the ability of early life adversity to reduce (through methylation) expression of a glucocorticoid receptor gene promoter in the hippocampus, producing a longstanding reduction to glucocorticoid-mediated negative feedback inhibition of stress responses [2729]. Consistent with this idea, adults having a history of childhood adversity show increased reactivity in stressful circumstances and may therefore be at higher risk of MD, see reviews by Taylor et al. [26, 30].

The goal of this study was to explore whether a simple diathesis-stress model could reproduce some of the complex patterns typically seen in epidemiologic studies when a nominal diagnostic definition was superimposed on the (continuous) output. An affirmative finding would support, at least in theory, two aspects of the diathesis-stress conception of depressive disorders: (1) its consistency with some of the available epidemiologic data and (2) its parsimony in the sense of its greater simplicity than the incidence-prevalence-mortality framework.

Four aspects of major depression epidemiology that are the focus of the study are: (1) that incidence diminishes over time (with increasing age), (2) that recurrence risk increases with a higher number of past episodes, (3) that elevated symptom levels increase the risk of major depression and (4) that recovery rates decline as the duration of episodes increase.

Methods

The simulation model used in this study was intended to provide a simple representation of a presumed underlying diathesis-stress interaction producing continuous depressive-symptom output, without attempting to specify whether the diathesis represents genetic, epigenetic, socially or cognitively determined vulnerability. The model was developed in the freely available software NetLogo [31]. In brief, simulated individuals (the model “agents”) were assigned a diathesis value and their exposure to stress was represented by movement of those agents across an environment characterized by different levels of stress. In some of the models presented below (models 2 and 3), values for the stress variable were assigned by random generation from a lognormal distribution with a mean of zero and a user-assigned standard deviation. This resulted in assignment of values for diathesis and stress that were always positive and that had an approximately bell-shaped distribution when the standard deviation was small and was right skewed when the standard deviation was large [32].

Depressive symptom levels were conceptualized as arising from prolonged activation of stress-response systems of the agent. The model used three variables to depict this process: stress activation, stress adaptation and stress burden. The values of each variable were updated in each day of the simulation as the agent moved across the environment, each day coming into contact with an area characterized by a specified stress level. This is depicted schematically in Figure 1, where the yellow polygon is the agent moving over stress regions (shaded squares). In Figure 1 three severities of stress are depicted but this was treated as a continuous variable in the modeling. The burden of stress for a particular agent was determined both by activation and adaptation. On each day activation was an addition to the burden and adaptation as a subtraction from the burden. The equations used to depict activation and adaptation were:

Stress activation t = stress t * diathesis Stress adaptation t = stress burden t 1 *
(1)
1 / 1 + adaption constant * diathesis
(2)
Figure 1
figure 1

Schematic depicting an individual (agent) moving over patches. * see equation 2. ** see equation 1.

Equation 1 represents activation as the product of the entity’s diathesis and the stress level encountered on each day. Equation 2 represents adaptation as the proportion of the total stress burden processed or eliminated in each day. Whereas stress activation was proportional to the diathesis, stress adaptation was inversely proportional. A constant was added to the model in order to allow the influence of diathesis to differ between adaptation and activation. Intuitively, if a depressive diathesis means that vulnerable individuals react more strongly to stressful events (activation) then the diathesis may be expected to primarily affect incidence (see review by Taylor [26]), whereas if the diathesis manifests as a lengthening or persistence of the stress response this may have a greater impact on the duration of symptoms (see for example, epigenetic evidence of impaired glucocorticoid feedback resulting from early life events [28]). The denominator in the right-hand side of equation 2 is formulated such that a value of zero for the adaptation constant would translate into a situation where adaptation to 100% of the stress burden of the preceding day would occur. This constant is labeled an “adaptation constant” in the formulas below and in the models presented in the paper. The change in burden was determined by the difference between stress activation (equation 1) and adaptation (equation 2), as summarized in equation 3 (see also Figure 1):

Stress burden t = stress burden t 1 + stressactivation t stressadaptation t
(3)

Since DSM-IV and ICD-10 definitions of depressive episodes are based on the severity and persistence of symptoms over minimum two week intervals, depressive symptom levels were simulated as a moving average of the current and past 13 days of stress burden, as described above. This is summarized in equation 4:

Depressive symptom levels = mean stress burden t 13 = > t
(4)

Finally, the simulation model included a threshold value and a remission stringency value. Entities were classified as entering an episode whenever their symptoms exceeded the threshold value. A depressive episode is clinically usually not considered to be over until symptoms are well below the diagnostic threshold (the concept of remission) [33]. Remission stringency specifies the extent of symptomatic improvement required before the episode can be considered over. For example, if the remission stringency value is 0.5 symptoms must fall to less than 50% of the diagnostic threshold value before the episode is considered over. The relationship between these thresholds and episode duration is presented schematically in Figure 2. It should be emphasized that the threshold and remission stringency variables are not intrinsic to the diathesis-stress interaction that the model represents. Rather, these are variables used to process the output data as if it were epidemiologic data with analogues of threshold and remission-based nominal definitions applied to it. Additional file 1 presents a more structured description of the model using a table format adapted from Railsback and Grimm [34].

Figure 2
figure 2

Typical output from model 2* (screen capture). * model parameters: diathesis = 1.4, stress_sd = 0.75, duration constant = 3, threshold = 15, remission stringency = 0.5. s = start of episode. e = end of episode.

The amount of time that an agent spends with symptoms exceeding the diagnostic threshold before dropping below the remission threshold was the model’s representation of the duration of a nominally classified episode. To illustrate the model, an applet is available through this reference [35]. The model is labeled Model 1. As the entities pass through the stressful intervals depicted by the bars their level of symptoms increases and then subsequently decreases after they pass through the bars into a low stress zone. A screen capture of the applet output is presented in Figure 3.

Figure 3
figure 3

A screen capture from model 1*, near the end of a simulation. * See http://people.ucalgary.ca/~patten/model_1.html.

A second version of this model (Model 2) provides output at the level of an individual agent, simulating the experience of this agent over 2 years (730 days). Again, this is a Java applet and can be examined using a web browser using a link provided in the reference list [36]. In this model, the distribution of the stress variable is assigned randomly to the environment over which the agent passes. The parameter values for the stress distribution can be changed by the user. Note that the button for the stress variable is labeled “stress_sd” because the distribution of stress is represented using the logarithm of a normal distribution with a mean of zero and this value for the standard deviation of the normal distribution. Note that when the model is initialized, the placement of the agent on the y-axis of the output window is random, so that the pattern of exposure to stress is not the same for each simulated agent even if the model parameters are not changed. Running the model several times using the same set parameters may result in episodes occurring in some simulations but not others.

Model 3 builds from the previous ones by randomly assigning diathesis values to each agent, simulating the experience of a series of agents (the number of which can be changed) and calculating output parameters: the incidence proportion (over the specified interval), the mean first episode duration and the mean episode count over the specified simulation interval. This model demonstrates how inter-individual variation in diathesis can be added to the prior model in order to simulate a population sample. Model 3 can be accessed through this reference [37].

The model that was used to produce the simulated output for data analysis in this study is attached as Additional file 2. This model is the same as Model 3, above, except that the simulation interval is set by default to 10 years (3652 days), the output window on the interface is expanded to accommodate this, and some additional outcome variables are recorded by the model in a data file. For example, the model output records the number of episodes occurring in the first nine years of the simulation in order to evaluate the emergence one of the complexities under investigation: whether the number of episodes in the first nine years would predict whether an episode would occur in the 10th year. A set of parameters was calibrated by trial and error to produce roughly the expected pattern of 10 year incidence (about 13%) [14], mean episode counts over 10 years (between 2 and 3 episodes for each agent having an episode) [12] and a mean first episode duration of about 11 months. This one-year episode duration may seem long for MD, e.g. see [38], but the mean duration of MD episodes have been found to be strongly influenced by a small number of very long episodes such that the distribution of episode durations from the simulation model does follow the familiar pattern, e.g. see [18], with this parameterization. The settings used in the simulation were 0.20 for the diathesis standard deviation, 0.75 for the stress standard deviation, 15 for the threshold value, 0.5 for the remission stringency and 3.0 for the adaptation constant. A trial simulation of 100,000 individuals with this model resulted in: a 15% cumulative incidence, mean episode count (among those with and episode) of 2.8 and a 314 day mean episode duration.

These parameters were then used to simulate 100,000 observations, creating a dataset with which to evaluate the specified epidemiologic patterns. This dataset, in the form of a comma separated values file (Additional file 3), was imported into the statistical software Stata [39] for analysis. Additional file 4 includes the same dataset in Stata format. A Stata “do” file in Additional file 5 was used to generate the results of the study, as reported below.

Additional information on characteristics of the model may be found in Additional file 6. This document presents a series of simulations, examining the effects of altering model parameters on two of the epidemiologic outputs: incidence and mean duration of the first episode.

Results

The first question addressed was whether simulated incidence would decline over time. Figure 4 presents a Kaplan-Meier curve for the onset of episodes. This analysis excludes simulated individuals already depressed at baseline (day 14, the first day when the moving average could be calculated). The slope of the curve flattens from left to right, indicating declining incidence.

Figure 4
figure 4

Incidence* over 10 years of a 3652 day simulation run. * incidence is the slope of the Kaplan-Meier curve.

The second question to be addressed was whether recurrence risk increases with a larger number of past episodes. This was addressed by tabulating episode incidence in the final year of the 10 year simulation interval according to how many episodes had been experienced during the first nine years. There is a dramatic association between the number of episodes in the earlier interval and the risk of having one in the final year of the simulation, see Figure 5.

Figure 5
figure 5

Recurrence in year 10, by number of episodes in years 1–9.

The third question was whether depressive symptoms at baseline or sub-threshold episodes at a baseline time point would predict subsequent incidence. To assess this, baseline scores were calculated at the first possible time (day 14 in the simulation) and Kaplan-Meier curves were generated for both groups. Respondents exceeding the diagnostic threshold value at the baseline time point (depressive symptoms at baseline were calculated at day 14) were excluded from this analysis. Score greater than 6 (approximately half the diagnostic threshold value in the simulations presented) was taken to indicate elevated symptoms. Those with higher baseline symptoms had a much higher subsequent risk, see Figure 6.

Figure 6
figure 6

Incidence over 10 years, by elevated* depressive symptoms at baseline. * scores > 6 (approximately 50% of the diagnostic threshold) were considered elevated.

The fourth question of interest was whether the probability of recovery would diminish with increasing episode duration. Figure 7 shows the cumulative probability of recovery among first episodes as a function of time. The rate of recovery is high in the early months and then diminishes subsequently, as is typically reported for major depression [18]. A fairly large proportion (13%) of the simulated cohort had not recovered after one year.

Figure 7
figure 7

Simulated cumulative recovery from MD episodes, by month.

Discussion

The general idea of a diathesis-stress model is intuitively appealing and not so complex as to be inaccessible to intuition. The simple formulas that comprise this simulation model merely reflect a simplified, intuitive representation of a diathesis-stress interaction. What these models demonstrate is that this simple and basic representation of an intuitive idea produces resemblances of epidemiologic data concerning depressive disorders. As such, the importance of this study is its tentative identification of the diathesis-stress model as a promising alternative framework for understanding these disorders. Given mounting awareness of the limitations of the current approach to diagnosis [40, 41] such tentative exploration of alternative perspectives seem warranted. These results should encourage further exploration of the diathesis-stress relationships that may exist in depressed patients.

Major depression has been viewed as an episodic condition with a complex epidemiology characterized by age, time and state-dependent rates of transition, with afflicted persons moving in and out of episodes according to these complicated dynamics. Through the lens of a diathesis-stress approach it is also possible to regard this condition as representing interplay between a personal vulnerability (diathesis) to the effects of stress and exposure to stressful life events. Diathesis-stress interactions are expected to result in fluctuating levels of symptoms as individuals with various degrees of vulnerability encounter various levels of stress in their lives. These models show that application of a threshold to a fluctuating level of symptoms can produce the appearance of an episodic disease course, at least in a subset of the population whose diathesis-stress interactions leave them neither always below (never depressed) nor always above (chronic depression) diagnostic thresholds. Whereas major depression has been viewed as an inherently episodic condition, the results presented here are consistent with the possibility that diathesis-stress dynamics may be more fundamentally important to the depressive disorders.

The tendency of the number of prior episodes to predict subsequent recurrence (see question 2, above) might be taken as evidence of kindling [42] or “scar” [4346] related phenomena, but the simulations presented here suggest another possibility: that this and other features may occur as an artifact of applying a threshold-based definition to a fundamentally continuous underlying process. Of note, the use of the term “scar” has recently been invoked to describe heightened depressive diatheses arising from early life adversity [47], a usage differing from the traditional concept of a “scar effect” (an effect of a depressive episode that persists after remission is obtained). This non-traditional usage fits nicely with the conceptualization modeled here. At a physiological level, the diathesis depicted in this study using a probability distribution might partially represent “limbic scars” arising from early life adversity, manifest along a continuum. Among other possible explanations, relevant changes may be related to polygenetic risk [25] or to adversity-induced methylation of promoter regions for hippocampal expression of glucocorticoid receptors – leading to increased stress sensitivity and diminished stress adaptation [48].

The model presented here is not intended to accurately reflect any real-world phenomena or to be used as a decision-support or prediction tool. The model has a conceptual rather than empirical basis. Furthermore, even conceptually, the model does not attempt to account for several potentially important factors. For example, whereas stress has been modeled as interacting passively with a diathesis, in reality people learn from their experiences and can also learn skills to cope with stressful events. The probability distributions selected for use in the model are arbitrary and were selected merely because they seemed to “make sense” intuitively. Conceivably, different patterns of exposure to stress could, in themselves, produce an episodic pattern, even without individual variation in vulnerability (diathesis). Despite these concerns, these results encourage the idea that empirical confirmation of diathesis-stress interactions (a rapidly developing area within the epigenetics literature [48]) may lead to simpler and more useful ways of understanding these conditions than the current approach has provided.

There are additional limitations to this study. Episode duration was examined only for the first episode, as this was the simplest to record from the model. In principle, episode duration (rather than specifically first episode durations) was the targeted output, so the strategy that was adopted in capturing the output is a limitation in this respect. Also, some of the agent’s variables could have been treated as global variables in the models since each models only a single individual at a time. More sophisticated agent-based simulation approaches, better informed by the rapidly developing biological literature, will play a role in advancing knowledge about these disorders.

Conclusions

Whereas major depression is usually characterized as an episodic condition with a very complex epidemiology, it may instead (at least in some cases) represent a much simpler pattern of environmental interaction with heightened vulnerability to stress. Some of the complexity associated with the epidemiology of this condition may arise as an artifact of applying threshold-based diagnostic definitions to an inherently dimensional underlying process. The diathesis-stress concept is a strong candidate framework to supersede the currently dominant threshold-based nominal approach.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR). 2000, Washington: American Psychiatric Association

    Google Scholar 

  2. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 1980, Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association, 3

    Google Scholar 

  3. World Health Organization: ICD-10. The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders. Clinical Descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. 1992, Geneva: World Health Organization

    Google Scholar 

  4. Barendregt JJ, van Oortmarssen GJ, Vos T, Murray CJL: A generic model for the assessment of disease epidemiology: the computational basis of DisMod II. Population Health Metrics. 2003, 1: 4-10.1186/1478-7954-1-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. McIntyre R, Kennedy S, Bagby M, Bakish D: Assessing full remission. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2002, 27: 235-239.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Judd LL, Paulus MP, Wells KB, Rapaport MH: Socioeconomic burden of subsyndromal depressive symptoms and major depression in a sample of the general population. Am J Psychiatry. 1996, 153: 1411-1417.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Judd LL, Rapaport MH, Paulus MP, Brown JL: Subsyndromal symptomatic depression: a new mood disorder?. J Clin Psychiatry. 1994, 55 (Suppl): 18-28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Casey P, Maracy M, Kelly BD, Lehtinen V, Ayuso-Mateos J-L, Dalgard OS, et al: Can adjustment disorder and depressive episode be distinguished? Results from ODIN. J Affect Disord. 2006, 92: 291-297. 10.1016/j.jad.2006.01.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Giuffra LA, Risch N: Diminished recall and the cohort effect of major depression: a simulation study. Psychol Med. 1994, 24: 375-383. 10.1017/S0033291700027355.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Kruijshaar ME, Barendregt J, Vos T, de Graaf R, Spijker J, Andrews G: Lifetime prevalence estimates of major depression: An indirect estimation method and a quantification of recall bias. Eur J Epidemiol. 2005, 20: 103-111. 10.1007/s10654-004-1009-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Patten SB, Gordon-Brown L, Meadows G: Simulation studies of age-specific lifetime major depression prevalence. BMC Psychiatry. 2010, 10: 85-10.1186/1471-244X-10-85.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Patten SB: A visual depiction of major depression epidemiology. BMC Psychiatry. 2007, 7: 23-10.1186/1471-244X-7-23.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Kokaua J, Milne BJ, Polanczyk G, et al: How common are common mental disorders? Evidence that lifetime prevalence rates are doubled by prospective versus retrospective ascertainment. Psychol Med. 2010, 40: 899-909. 10.1017/S0033291709991036.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Patten SB: Accumulation of major depressive episodes over time in a prospective study indicates that retrospectively assessed lifetime prevalence estimates are too low. BMC Psychiatry. 2009, 9: 19-10.1186/1471-244X-9-19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Patten SB: Problems encountered with the use of simulation in an attempt to enhance interpretation of a secondary data source in epidemiologic mental health research. BMC Res Notes. 2010, 3: 231-10.1186/1756-0500-3-231.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Eaton WW, Kramer M, Anthony JC, Dryman A, Shapiro S, Locke BZ: The incidence of specific DIS/DSM-III mental disorders: data from the NIMH Epidemiological Catchment Area Program. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1989, 79: 163-178. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1989.tb08584.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Patten SB: Incidence of major depression in Canada. CMAJ. 2000, 163: 714-715.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Vos T, Haby MM, Berendregt JJ, Kruijshaar M, Corry J, Andrews G: The burden of major depression avoidable by longer-term treatment strategies. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004, 61: 1097-1103. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1097.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Patten SB, Lee RC: Refining estimates of major depression incidence and episode duration in Canada using a Monte Carlo Markov model. Med Decis Making. 2004, 24: 351-358. 10.1177/0272989X04267008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ayuso-Mateos JL, Nuevo R, Verdes E, Naidoo N, Chatterji S: From depressive symptoms to depressive disorders: the relevance of thresholds. Br J Psychiatry. 2010, 196: 365-371. 10.1192/bjp.bp.109.071191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Patten SB, Williams JV, Lavorato DH, Bulloch AG, Macqueen G: Depressive episode characteristics and subsequent recurrence risk. J Affect Disord. 2012, 140: 277-284. 10.1016/j.jad.2012.02.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kennedy SH, Lam RW, Cohen NL, Ravindran AV, and the CANMAT Depression Work Group: Clinical guidelines for the treatment of depressive disorders. IV. Medications and other biological treatments. Can J Psychiatry. 2001, 46 (1): 38-58.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Joiner TE, Wagner KD: Parental, child-centered attributions and outcome: a meta-analytic review with conceptual and methodological implications. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1996, 24: 37-52. 10.1007/BF01448372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Brown GW, Harris TO: Social origins of depression: a study of psychiatric disorder in women. 1978, London: Tavistock Publications

    Google Scholar 

  25. Hettema JM: Genetics of depression. Focus. 2010, 8: 316-322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Taylor SE: Mechanisms linking early life stress to adult health outcomes. Proc Nat Acad Sci. 2010, 107: 8507-8512. 10.1073/pnas.1003890107.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Weaver IC, Cervoni N, Champagne FA, D'Alessio AC, Sharma S, Seckl JR, et al: Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2004, 7: 847-854. 10.1038/nn1276.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Meaney MJ, Szyf M, Seckl JR: Epigenetic mechanisms of perinatal programming of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function and health. Trends Mol Med. 2007, 13: 269-277. 10.1016/j.molmed.2007.05.003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Szyf M, McGowan P, Meaney MJ: The social environment and the epigenome. Environ Mol Mutagen. 2008, 49: 46-60. 10.1002/em.20357.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Taylor SE, Lerner JS, Sage RM, Lehman BJ, Seeman TE: Early environment, emotions, responses to stress, and health. J Pers. 2004, 72: 1365-1393. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00300.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Wilensky U: NetLogo. [5.0.3]. 2012. The Center for Connected Learning (CCL) and Computer-based Modeling. 2012, http://ccl.northwestern.edu/netlogo/,

    Google Scholar 

  32. Log-normal distribution. 10-14-2012. Wikipedia. 11-3-2012. 2012, http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Log-normal_distribution,

  33. Keller MB: Past, present, and future directions for defining optimal treatment outcomes for depression. Remission and beyond. JAMA. 2004, 289: 3152-3160.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Railsback SF, Grimm V: Describing and formulating ABMs; the ODD Protocol. Agent-based and individual-based modeling. A practial introduction. 2012, Princeton: Princeton University Press, 35-46.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Patten SB. Model 1. 11-3-2012. 2012, http://people.ucalgary.ca/~patten/model_1.html,

  36. Patten SB. Model 2. 11-3-2012. 2012, http://people.ucalgary.ca/~patten/model_2.html,

  37. Patten SB. Model 3. 11-3-2012. 2012, http://people.ucalgary.ca/~patten/model_3.html,

  38. Üstün TB, Kessler RC: Global burden of depressive disorders: the issue of duration. Br J Psychiatry. 2002, 181: 181-183. 10.1192/bjp.181.3.181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Stata Corporation: Stata, version 11.0. [11.0]. 2009. College Station. 2009, TX: Stata Corporation

    Google Scholar 

  40. Holtzheimer PE, Mayberg HS: Stuck in a rut: rethinking depression and its treatment. Trends Neurosci. 2011, 34: 1-9. 10.1016/j.tins.2010.10.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Patten SB: Major depression prevalence is very high, but the syndrome is a poor proxy for community populations' clinical treatment needs. Can J Psychiatry. 2008, 53: 411-419.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Post RM, Weiss SR: Sensitization and kindling phenomena in mood, anxiety, and obsessive-compulsive disorders: the role of serotonergic mechanisms in illness progression. Biol Psychiatry. 1998, 44: 193-206. 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00144-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Beevers CG, Rohde P, Stice E, Nolen-Hoeksema S: Recovery from major depressive disorder among female adolescents: a prospective test of the scar hypothesis. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2007, 75: 888-900.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Ormel J, Oldehinkel AJ, Nolen WA, Vollebergh W: Psychosocial disability before, during and after a major depressive episode. A 3-wave population-based study of state, scar and trait effects. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004, 61: 387-392. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.4.387.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Rohde P, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR: Are people changed by the experience of having an episode of depression? A further test of the scar hypothesis. J Abn Psychol. 1990, 99: 264-271.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Zeiss AM, Lewinsohn PM: Enduring deficits after remissions of depression: a test of the scar hypothesis. Behav Res Ther. 1988, 26: 151-158. 10.1016/0005-7967(88)90114-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Dannlowski U, Stuhrmann A, Beutelmann V, Zwanzger P, Lenzen T, Grotegerd D, et al: Limbic scars: long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biol Psychiatry. 2012, 71: 286-293. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. McGowan PO: Epigenetic clues to the biological embedding of early life adversity. Biol Psychiatry. 2012, 72: 4-5. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

Dr. Patten is a Senior Health Scholar with Alberta Innovates, Health Solutions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Scott B Patten.

Additional information

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Author’s contributions

Dr. P was responsible for all aspects of the study.

Electronic supplementary material

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Patten, S.B. Major depression epidemiology from a diathesis-stress conceptualization. BMC Psychiatry 13, 19 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-19

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-19

Keywords