Skip to main content
  • Study protocol
  • Open access
  • Published:

Baby Business: a randomised controlled trial of a universal parenting program that aims to prevent early infant sleep and cry problems and associated parental depression

Abstract

Background

Infant crying and sleep problems (e.g. frequent night waking, difficulties settling to sleep) each affect up to 30% of infants and often co-exist. They are costly to manage and associated with adverse outcomes including postnatal depression symptoms, early weaning from breast milk, and later child behaviour problems. Preventing such problems could improve these adverse outcomes and reduce costs to families and the health care system. Anticipatory guidance-i.e. providing parents with information about normal infant sleep and cry patterns, ways to encourage self-settling in infants, and ways to develop feeding and settling routines before the onset of problems-could prevent such problems. This paper outlines the protocol for our study which aims to test an anticipatory guidance approach.

Methods/Design

750 families from four Local Government Areas in Melbourne, Australia have been randomised to receive the Baby Business program (intervention group) or usual care (control group) offered by health services. The Baby Business program provides parents with information about infant sleep and crying via a DVD and booklet (mailed soon after birth), telephone consultation (at infant age 6-8 weeks) and parent group session (at infant age 12 weeks). All English speaking parents of healthy newborn infants born at > 32 weeks gestation and referred by their maternal and child health nurse at their first post partum home visit (day 7-10 postpartum), are eligible. The primary outcome is parent report of infant night time sleep as a problem at four months of age and secondary outcomes include parent report of infant daytime sleep or crying as a problem, mean duration of infant sleep and crying/24 hours, parental depression symptoms, parent sleep quality and quantity and health service use. Data will be collected at two weeks (baseline), four months and six months of age. An economic evaluation using a cost-consequences approach will, from a societal perspective, compare costs and health outcomes between the intervention and control groups.

Discussion

To our knowledge this is the first randomised controlled trial of a program which aims to prevent both infant sleeping and crying problems and associated postnatal depression symptoms. If effective, it could offer an important public health prevention approach to these common, distressing problems.

Trial registration number

ISRCTN: ISRCTN63834603

Peer Review reports

Background

In the first six months of life, between 15-35% of parents report a problem with their infant's sleep [1–3] including difficulties settling their infant to sleep at the start of the night and re-settling them overnight. Such problems disturb parental sleep leading to parental fatigue [4], reduced ability to care effectively for the infant, and parental depression symptoms [5]. Persistent infant sleep problems are associated with later child behaviour problems [6–8].

Similarly, around 14-28% of parents report infant crying as a problem in the first few months of life and sleep and crying problems often co-exist. Crying duration exceeding 3 hours/24 hours for at least 3 days for at least 3 weeks is typically known as 'colic' and affects between 9-12% of infants from community samples [9, 10]. Parents of crying infants may experience frustration and anger towards their infant and parental perception of crying as a problem is the most common proximal risk factor for Shaken Baby Syndrome [11]. Problem crying is associated with early weaning from breast milk, frequent changes of formulae and parent depression symptoms. Parental perception that their infant's crying is a problem therefore merits attention, regardless of whether the actual duration of crying meets criteria for 'colic'.

Sleep problems in infants

Approximately two-thirds of infants learn to 'sleep through the night' i.e. experience unbroken sleep between the hours of midnight and 5 am, by 12 weeks of age [12, 13]. However, up to a third do not [14] and may also have problems initiating sleep [15]. Such problems have been consistently linked to parental mood disorder [16, 17] and when infant sleep problems are treated parental depressive symptoms decrease [18, 19]. Preventing infant sleep problems may therefore be an acceptable way to reduce rates of parental depression, particularly given that breast feeding mothers are often reluctant to accept pharmacologic treatment for depression [20].

Infant sleep problems tend to arise when parents actively help their infant to fall asleep [21, 22]. Infants, like adults, tend to wake briefly several times during the night, but parents are only aware of this if the infant cries out (signals) and wakes the parents. Parents who rock, feed, or remain with their infant while the infant falls asleep, are more likely to report frequent night awakenings and settling problems [21], as their infants tend to cry out and demand the same attention each time they wake during the night. Conversely, parents who allow their infant to settle to sleep independently with little caregiver interaction, report fewer immediate and long term sleep problems [23, 24]. Their infants tend to be more successful at falling back to sleep without the caregiver's help throughout the night [15].

Maternal cognitions about infant sleep are also strongly related to infant sleep problems [25]. Mothers who have problems resisting their infant's demands, who feel anger and helplessness when faced with their infant's demands, or who feel doubt regarding the adequacy of their parenting, are significantly more likely to report infant sleep disturbance [25]. In order to cope with these feelings, parents may become 'over intrusive' at bedtime. This may result in the infant learning caregiver dependent sleep associations that in turn lead to more frequent night signaling [21].

Crying in infants

Around 14-28% of parents report infant crying as a problem [9, 26, 10]. Infant crying duration peaks at around 6-8 weeks of age at around 2.5 hours per 24 hours, then gradually declines [27, 28]. 'Colic' refers to crying in excess of 3 hours per day for at least 3 days per week, over a three week period that resists soothing. The cause(s) of colic are unknown. Although long attributed to gastrointestinal upset (e.g. flatulence and gastro-oesophageal reflux) [29, 30] and organic disturbances such as food intolerance or allergy [31], medical causes are thought to affect only one in ten infants [32]. Yet many parents change their own diet (if breast feeding) or the infant's formula, in the belief that a food intolerance underlies the crying problem [33, 34]. Others seek over the counter medications for reflux or alternative therapists such as chiropractors, both of which have been shown in rigorous trials to have no impact on crying [35, 36]. Other medications are either ineffective (e.g. simethicone) [37, 38], associated with serious side effects (e.g. dicyclomine and apnoeas) or may predispose the infant to harm (e.g. acid suppressive medications and eosinophilic oesophagitis)[39].

Parenting style may also affect the amount of fussy crying (crying that can be soothed) as well as colicky crying (crying that is unsoothable). 'Proximal care' describes a parenting style that typically involves feeding an infant in excess of 14 times per day, holding them for greater than 80% of the day time, and cosleeping (i.e. parent sharing a sleeping surface with the infant). One study compared the impact of this approach on infant sleep and crying with a more structured approach (i.e. feeding infant every 3 to 4 hours, placing them in a cot for sleep and a delayed response to infant demands) and an approach somewhere between the two styles [40]. Comparison of approaches revealed that proximal care parents had infants with less fussy crying but the same amount of colicky crying as the more structured approach. Proximal care resulted in more frequent waking and crying at night at 12 weeks of age. The authors concluded that a proximal care approach throughout the first few weeks of life may be useful in reducing overall amounts of non-colicky crying, but changing to a structured approach after this time may result in less night waking at 12 weeks of age [40]. Any program which aims to prevent early infant sleep and cry problems could incorporate this approach.

Interventions for existing infant sleep/cry problems

Behavioural strategies such as graduated extinction (where the parent returns to check on their crying child at increasing time intervals with brief parental reassurance) and positive bedtime routines [41, 42] have been shown to be the most effective strategies for managing sleep problems in children aged 6 months or older. Despite the effectiveness of such interventions [43, 44], some parents find techniques involving extinction unacceptable, as they dislike leaving their infant to cry [25, 42]. Given that these parents may not follow through with strategies, it may instead be preferable to aim to prevent sleep problems. Prevention may have additional advantages including greater efficacy (as problem is less entrenched) and prevention of associated parental distress.

Few randomised controlled trials (RCTs) have evaluated interventions for colicky infant crying. Most have involved changes to diet or use of medication, with results mostly suggesting no effect over and above placebo and additionally, the methodological rigor of these trials has been questioned [45]. Increased carrying of the infant has been shown in one trial to prevent crying [46] but did not reduce established crying in another trial [47]. Keefe [48] has proposed a model to explain infant colic as a psychobiological disturbance in infant behaviour regulation due to increased sensitivity to the environment. Disruptions or inconsistencies in parenting or the surrounding environment overstimulate the infant resulting in crying that the infant does not yet have the maturity to regulate. With this in mind Keefe and colleagues [48] conducted a RCT of an intervention for colicky infants aged two to six weeks. In the intervention group (n = 64), nurses visited families four times over one month to provide support to parents, make modifications to infant care (with an emphasis on consistency of routines) and educate parents on reducing overstimulation in their infant. The control group (n = 57) received usual care. Compared with the control group, intervention infants had a significantly higher number of resolved crying problems (61.8% vs. 28.8%, p = 0.03) as well as shorter total crying time/day (1.29 vs. 2.94 hours, p = 0.02) at approximately 13 weeks of age (exact mean age not given in manuscript). This suggests that intensive parental support, modification of environment and provision of structured care can reduce crying.

Can we prevent infant sleep and cry problems?

Only four RCTs have examined the impact of prevention programs on infant sleep problems. No RCTs have aimed to prevent both infant sleep and cry problems. In a small RCT of middle-class first time parent couples (intervention n = 29, control n = 31), Wolfson and colleagues [24] provided parents with two prenatal and two postnatal group sessions that taught parents about normal infant sleep/wake patterns and the importance of establishing an independent sleep routine so that the infant can self-settle. Infants of parents who attended these group sessions slept for longer amounts of time, and demonstrated better sleep patterns than infants of control group parents at one, two and three weeks of age.

Kerr, Jowett & Smith's [49] RCT aimed to prevent infant sleep problems by providing parents with information on settling methods and the importance of routine via a booklet and a home visit (intervention n = 86 and control n = 83). Information was provided at three months of age and follow up data were collected at nine months. Compared with control group infants, intervention group infants had significantly fewer settling difficulties (21% vs. 39%, p = 0.03), significantly fewer night awakenings (23% woke two or more times per night vs. 46%, p = 0.02) and significantly better cumulative sleep scores overall (22% met criteria for a severe sleep problem vs. 39%, p = 0.03).

In a three-armed RCT that aimed to prevent infant sleep problems in infants aged 8 to 14 days [23], families were allocated to receive either (1) a structured behavioural program (n = 205), (2) an education oriented group (n = 202), or (3) the usual care provided by UK health services (n = 203). Parents in the behavioural group were asked to allow their infant to settle to sleep independently, to only respond to the infant when genuinely crying (as opposed to fussing or fretting), to keep stimulation low during the night, and after three months of age, to gradually increase the time between night feeds. Parents received the information via a flyer which a researcher also discussed with the parents. Parents in the education oriented group received the same information in a ten page booklet and the information was suggestive rather than prescriptive. By 12 weeks of age, significantly more behavioural group infants were sleeping through the night (having uninterrupted sleep from 12 am-5 am) than the other two groups. Behavioural but not education group parents tended to access significantly fewer health services for their infant's sleep in the following six months. In a post hoc analysis [50], infants receiving in excess of 11 feeds per day at one week of age were more likely to wake during the night at 12 weeks of age. The data of infants who met this criterion, from both the behaviour group, and the control group, were then compared. By 12 weeks of age, 80% of these 'at risk' infants in the behaviour program, compared to 60% in the control group, were sleeping through the night. Thus a behavioural program may be particularly useful for preventing sleep problems in infants who feed frequently in the first week of life.

In another RCT that aimed to prevent infant sleep problems, parents recruited via birth notices in the local newspaper were randomly allocated to receive either a 45 minute consultation with a nurse accompanied by written information at infant age three weeks (intervention group, n = 137), or usual care (control group, n = 131) [51]. Intervention group parents were taught about the cyclical nature of sleep and the benefits of parent-independent sleep cues. Parents were recommended to leave the infant to settle for five minutes before responding if the infant was crying and to extend this response time by five minutes for each subsequent visit. All parents completed a 7 day infant behaviour diary at 12 weeks of age. Intervention infants were significantly more likely to have at least 15 hours of sleep per 24 hours than control infants (62% vs. 36%, respectively, p < 0.001). The sample was predominantly middle-upper class thereby limiting the generalisability of the findings.

Prior research has been limited by the use of population sampling that is self-selected [51, 24], excludes unmarried parents [24] or fails to collect data on compliance with intervention strategies [49, 51, 24]. Despite strong links between infant sleep and crying problems and parental depression [16, 17], parent well being has rarely been included as an outcome [49, 24]. Data from fathers is often lacking despite the increasing role fathers play in infant care [52] and the protective role they can play in prevention of postnatal depression in mothers [53–55]. Therefore, involvement of fathers in trials that have a specific focus on infant and mother wellbeing is paramount.

In summary, an infant demand style of approach in the first few weeks of life may reduce overall crying, but changing to a more structured style of care after these first few weeks, may result in less night waking at 12 weeks of age [40]. A program that implements elements of both approaches and provides parents with information about normal sleep and cry patterns and ways to reduce stimulation and provide a predictable environment in the first few months of life, may be able to prevent both infant sleep and cry problems [14, 48]. Reduction in these problems is likely to have positive flow on benefits for parent wellbeing [18, 19], and could lead to reduced health service use for both parent and infant wellbeing [23]. This paper presents the study protocol of a randomised controlled trial of a universal parenting program designed to prevent both infant sleep and cry problems, and improve parental wellbeing. The recruitment phase of this trial is currently complete and intervention delivery and follow up assessments are ongoing.

Methods/Design

Study Design

Randomised controlled trial.

Setting

Four Melbourne (state of Victoria, Australia) Local Government Areas (LGAs) of Brimbank, Wyndham, Moonee Valley and Yarra. We selected these LGAs based on a Victorian government request to carry out the study in the north-western region of Melbourne and on annual birth rates in excess of 1000 to maximise recruitment.

Participants

Inclusion criteria

All parents of newborn infants seen by their Maternal and Child Health Nurse (MCHN) at their first home visit (day 7-10 postpartum) in the four LGA's. The MCH nursing service is a universal, free service offered to all Victorian families with scheduled visits (covering 93% of all births) post partum, then at 2 weeks of age, and 1, 2, 4, 8, 12, 18, 24 and 42 months.

Exclusion criteria

Parents with insufficient English to complete the questionnaires and take part in the intervention have been excluded. Infants born before 32 weeks gestation or with a serious health concern have been excluded as program material may not be suitable for very premature or ill infants.

Sample Size/Power calculation

In a Victorian survey of 724 mothers, [4] 34% reported sleep problems at mean infant age of 4.6 months (range 3-6 months). In a community survey of Queensland parents [56] 27% reported a problem in their 4-6 month old infants (n = 740). Drawing upon this data, a relative reduction in the prevalence of parent report of an infant sleep problem (sleep problem yes/no) by 30% at infant age four months (i.e. from 30% to 20%) is likely to be clinically significant and to have flow on effects for parent mental health. Assuming this, we aimed to detect a reduction in the proportion of infant sleep problems from 30% to 20% at 4-months with 80% power at the 5% significance level. In a trial that randomises individuals, we required 313 subjects in each arm or a total of 780 families, allowing for a 20% loss to follow up. Due to time and budget restraints, we have been able to recruit 750 families only, giving us 78% power to detect a reduction in the proportion of infant sleep problems from 30% to 20% at 4-months, at the 5% level of significance.

Randomisation

Using a computer generated random number sequence, an independent researcher allocated each consenting family to the intervention or control group. The research team and families remained blind to group allocation at the time of recruitment and consent; however, following this, knowledge of group allocation is unavoidable given the intervention type.

Reducing bias

Despite being at risk of response and subjective bias, parental perceptions of either a sleep or cry problem (yes/no) is our most important outcome measure, given its relation to increased parental depression symptoms, early weaning and increased risk of Shaken Baby Syndrome. To counteract the disadvantages of using a subjective outcome measure, we are also using a validated behaviour diary to obtain measures of sleep and cry duration. In a previous study of 446 infants, we found that diary data including total crying time and number of bouts of crying/24 hours were significantly increased in parents who reported problem crying in their infant vs. those who did not, whilst sleep duration and number of bouts of sleep/24 hours were significantly reduced in infant's whose parents reported a sleep problem vs. those who did not [5].

Intervention

All parents allocated to the intervention group will be posted a study-designed booklet and DVD and will be offered an individual telephone consultation (at infant age 6 weeks) and group session (at infant age 12 weeks). Telephone consultations and parent groups will be facilitated by trained health professionals (nurses, psychologists) with a background in infant care. These facilitators will spend a minimum of two hours completing specific training in how to use the Baby Business telephone and group session manuals. This training will be facilitated by either the Chief Investigator (HH) or the project manager (FC), and all facilitators will observe a minimum of two telephone consultations and two groups before independently delivering program content.

Booklet

The 27-page booklet provides parents with information about normal infant sleep patterns, sleep cycles and therefore, the potential for an infant to wake overnight several times. The benefit of an infant learning to fall asleep independently is discussed. Content highlights the disadvantages of an infant relying on parent dependent cues to fall asleep at the beginning of the night (e.g. by rocking). The dangers of parents sharing their bed with a newborn are also described. Steps to settling an infant are given with emphasis on the importance of putting the infant down for a sleep drowsy but awake, so that the infant can learn to fall asleep independently. Normal infant crying patterns are discussed (including the 'crying curve' showing the natural peak and subsequent decline in infant crying; used with permission from the website: http://www.purplecrying.info [57]) together with strategies for managing infant crying in both checklist and pictorial form. Signs and symptoms of uncommon medical causes of crying are outlined. Information on improving parental wellbeing is provided, as well as information on typical sleep and feeding patterns in Australian children after the first three months of life.

DVD

The DVD covers very similar content to the booklet but also includes footage of parents discussing the methods they use to settle their infant, the tired signs their infant displays, as well as demonstrations of how they wrap and sooth their infant.

Telephone consultation

The telephone consultation expands on the content of the booklet and DVD. During the telephone call, a facilitator helps the parent apply the information in a way that is suitable for their family. Parents are encouraged to discuss topics such as: whether they have noticed their infant's sleep cycles; how their infant behaves when over-tired and how to recognise and avoid over-tiredness; the advantages of teaching an infant to fall asleep without hands on help; the risks of co-sleeping; changes they would like to make to their settling routine as well as strategies they will use when their infant is having trouble settling; where their infant currently lies on the 'crying curve' and what they might expect to happen to crying duration in the coming weeks; colic; and, if appropriate, uncommon medical causes of crying.

Parent Group

Parents are invited to attend a 1.5 hour group session at a local venue. The group aims to troubleshoot any problems parents are having with infant sleep and crying. The group facilitator follows a manual that covers daytime feeding and sleeping patterns at 3 to 4 months of age, day time napping and how to encourage longer daytime sleeps, night time feeding, use of dummies, wrapping, myths around infant care, and the importance of parental self-care.

Usual Care

Families allocated to the 'usual care' (control) group receive no intervention from our research team. These families continue to receive the usual assistance and advice provided to all parents of newborns, via usual contact with MCH nurses and other health professionals.

Measures

Baseline questionnaire

Parent A (the primary caregiver) will complete a baseline questionnaire including infant date of birth, birth weight, birth order, gestation, the type of milk being given, approximate number of feeds during the day and the night, caregiver's date of birth, country of birth, the main language spoken in the home, current marital status and highest level of education completed. Details are also gathered regarding partner date of birth, country of birth and their highest level of education completed. They will also be asked where their infant spends most of the night (parents bed, own cot/bed in other room, own cot/bed in parent's room or 'other') and whether their infant has been diagnosed with or suspected of any major illness.

Parent A will complete the 'doubt' subscale of the Maternal Cognitions about Infant Sleep Questionnaire [25] (see below). Other subscales are not relevant at such a young age. Feelings of parental doubt may already manifest at 2 to 4 weeks of age and such doubts are associated with increased likelihood of infant sleep problems [25]. Parent A will also be asked whether they think they are a relaxed or tense person (0 = relaxed to 10 = tense; Sayers, 2004); being 'tense' (scoring 7 or higher) predicts higher infant irritability at nine weeks of age.

Follow up questionnaires

In order to establish the efficacy of this intervention, we are measuring outcomes at infant age four and six months, since most infants 'sleep through the night' by four months of age [58] and no longer require night time feeding to meet their nutritional needs by six months of age. Measurement at these time points will therefore indicate established sleep patterns of these infants.

Both Parent A and Parent B (secondary caregiver) will complete follow up questionnaires at infant age four and six months. Only Parent A will complete an infant behaviour diary and the feeding questions at these time points.

Primary & secondary outcomes

Our primary outcome is parent report of infant night time sleep problems. Secondary outcomes include parent report of day time sleep problems, infant crying problems and feeding problems. Parents will be asked 'Have any of the following baby behaviours been a problem for you over the last 2 weeks? Daytime sleep (yes/no), Night-time sleep (yes/no), Crying (yes/no)' [59]. If a parent answers 'yes' to any of these, they will rate the severity of the problem on a seven point Likert scale from 1 = 'hardly any problem' to 7 = 'a severe problem' [43].

Parent A will also complete an infant behaviour diary over a 72 hour period, during which they will record whether their infant is sleeping, feeding, awake and content or awake and crying/fussing in 10-minute epochs. This diary has been adapted from the Barr diary which measures such behaviours in 5 minute epochs and has sound psychometric properties [60].

Parental sleep quality and quantity

Two items have been adapted from the validated Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) [61] to measure parent perception of sleep quantity and quality: (1) 'Over the last two weeks, how would you rate your own sleep quantity?' with responses of 'Not nearly enough', 'Not quite enough', 'Enough' or 'More than enough' and (2) 'Over the last two weeks, how would you rate your own sleep quality?' with responses 'Not nearly good enough', 'Not quite good enough', 'Good enough' or 'More than good enough'.

Parents will also report how many times and for how long on average they attended their infant for night waking over the past week.

Postnatal depression

The Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) [62] is a validated screen of postnatal depression (PND) [63, 64] consisting of 10 items. Clinically significant levels of PND are indicated by scores ≥ 10 and ≥ 9 for mothers and fathers in community samples, respectively.

Parental cognitions around infant sleep

The 20-item Maternal Cognitions about Infant Sleep Questionnaire (MCISQ) [25] has 5 subscales including: limit setting (ability to resist infant demands), anger (anger, regret and helplessness), doubt (uncertainty regarding ability as a parent), feeding (belief about the importance of feeding to settle and concerns about child going hungry) and safety (concerns about cot death). We have not included the 'feeding' subscale as it is not associated with infant sleep problems [25].

Parental perception of infant temperament

Parents will rate their infant's temperament on a six point scale: 'Compared to other babies, I think my baby is:' Much easier than average, Easier than average, Average, More difficult than average, Much more difficult than average or cannot say. This single item from the Australian Temperament Project-a longitudinal study of 2000 children-has a moderate correlation with the Australian version of the Revised Infant Temperament Questionnaire [65].

Sources of alternate help for infant sleep/crying

Parents will be asked if they have received help or advice (professional or otherwise) from outside the program for either their infant's sleep/crying or for their own stress, and if so, how many appointments they attended for each.

Feeding

Parent A will indicate the type of food being offered to their infant at present (breast milk, formula or a combination of these) [66, 67], and if applicable, whether breast feeding has stopped and why, whether their infant's formula has changed at any stage and whether the mother has changed her own diet while breastfeeding. The 6 month questionnaire also asks about timing of introduction and type of solids.

Caregiver support and self-efficacy

At baseline and follow up, parent A will rate the level of support or help they receive from their partner and family and friends living elsewhere (I get enough help, I don't get enough help, I don't get any help, I don't need any help), and how often they feel they need support or help but can't get it from anyone (very often, often, sometimes, never, I don't need it) (LSAC) [68]. Parent A will rate their efficacy as a parent (from 1 = not very good to 5 = a very good parent) [68].

Parental use of intervention materials

Intervention parents will be asked which components of the intervention they received or participated in and usefulness of each, on a study-designed scale ('not at all (useful), a little, quite a bit, a great deal'). Intervention parents will also be asked to rate the helpfulness of specific strategies provided in the intervention materials as either helpful or unhelpful.

Procedures

Throughout the recruitment phase, MCH nurses approached all families of newborn infants at the first home visit and asked permission to pass on to the research team the contact details of families interested in hearing more about the research. The research team contacted interested families to explain the study further and posted a recruitment pack (information statement, consent form and baseline questionnaire) to parent A and an information pack (information statement and consent form) to parent B if requested. Families were randomised upon receipt of their signed consent form and baseline questionnaire.

Families allocated to the usual care group received a letter explaining that they will continue to see their MCH nurse as usual. Those allocated to the Baby Business intervention group were mailed the booklet and DVD.

A member of the study team completes the telephone consultation with intervention group parents one to two weeks after sending the booklet and DVD. Families are then invited to attend a parenting group session when their infant is around 12 weeks of age. Neither the telephone consultation nor the parent group is compulsory, and parents who do not take part in these components will not be removed from the study given that they will already have received most of the program content via the booklet and DVD. Data will be gathered on participation in each component.

In order to minimize study drop-out, we will call parents if they have not returned their follow up questionnaires two weeks after they were mailed to them. If we do not receive the completed questionnaire in the two weeks following this, we will call the parent again and give the parent the option of completing a shortened version of the questionnaire (covering only the main outcome measures of whether infant sleep, crying and feeding are a problem) over the phone.

Hypotheses

We hypothesise that intervention parents compared to control group parents will, at infant age four and six months, report:

  • fewer infant night time sleep problems (primary outcome)

  • fewer infant day time sleep problems

  • fewer crying problems

  • decreased mean infant crying duration and increased mean infant sleep duration/24 hours

  • improved parent sleep quality and quantity,

  • improved mental health with lower mean EPDS scores and lower proportions of mothers scoring > 10 and fathers scoring > 9 on the EPDS

  • less difficult infant temperament,

  • fewer visits to healthcare professionals for their infant's sleep and crying and their own wellbeing

  • higher rates of breastfeeding, fewer formula changes and fewer dietary changes in breastfeeding mothers

Analysis plan

Outcome data at 4 and 6 months will be presented using descriptive statistics. Means and standard deviations will be given for continuous outcomes, as well as medians and inter-quartile ranges where continuous data are skewed. Proportions for categorical data will also be given. The primary outcome comparison will be of the proportion of infants with sleep and cry problems at 4 months between the two trial arms. Logistic regression adjusting for potential confounders identified a priori, and measured at baseline (including child gender and family socioeconomic status), will be used to estimate the treatment effect as an odds ratio and 95% confidence interval. Random effects regression models [69, 70] will be used for further longitudinal analysis examining trends in treatment response, that is, persistence of sleep and cry problems from baseline to 4 and 6 months post intervention. Similar analysis will be carried out for each of the categorical outcomes. We will also compare mean scores for continuous outcomes at the primary endpoint of 4 months (e.g. sleep duration/24 hours) between the two trial arms using t tests, as well as linear regression adjusting for potential confounders. The study sample size is sufficient to enable the use of such techniques when the outcome data are skewed [71], and empirical bootstrap estimates will be examined to confirm the validity of the inferences made. Trends in treatment response will again be examined from baseline to 4 and 6 months post intervention using random effects regression models. All analyses will be conducted on the basis of intention to treat. The frequency and patterns of missing data will be examined and sensitivity analyses will be performed comparing the results of analyses restricted to families with complete data and analyses where missing data are imputed using a conservative approach [72].

Economic evaluation

A cost-consequences analysis will be conducted from a societal perspective. Costs and outcomes will be taken into account and valued in the analysis regardless of who bears the costs, who benefits or who provides the resources. The incremental costs of the intervention (the difference of costs accrued in the intervention group and costs accrued in the control group) will be compared to a range of the incremental primary and secondary outcomes. Both costs of delivering the intervention and costs of families' use of health and other services outside of the study will be considered in economics costing.

The economic evaluation will draw a comprehensive picture of the costs and consequences of the intervention and assist policy makers to make appropriate decisions on resource allocation to such interventions and determine the cost-effectiveness of nationwide roll-out.

Ethical approval

Ethical approval has been obtained from the Royal Children's Hospital Human Research Ethics Committee (HREC 28130) and the Department of Education and Early Childhood Development, Early Childhood Research Committee.

References

  1. France KG: Behavior characteristics and security in sleep-disturbed infants treated with extinction. J Pediatr Psychol. 1992, 17: 467-475. 10.1093/jpepsy/17.4.467.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Goodlin-Jones BL, Burnham MM, Anders TF: Sleep and sleep disturbances: regulatory processes in infancy. Handbook of developmental psychopathology. Edited by: Sameroff A, Lewis M, Miller M. 2000, New York: Kluwer Academic, 309-325.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  3. Messer D, Richards M: The development of sleeping difficulties. Infant crying, feeding and sleeping: development, problems and treatments. Edited by: St James-Roberts I, Harris G, Messer D. 1993, London: Harvester Wheatsheaf, 150-173.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Bayer JK, Hiscock H, Hampton A, Wake M: Sleep problems in young infants and maternal mental and physical health. J Paed Child Health. 2007, 43: 66-73. 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2007.01005.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Wake M, Morton-Allen E, Poulakis Z, Hiscock H, Gallagher S, Oberklaid F: Prevalence, stability and outcomes of cry-fuss and sleep problems in the first two years of life: prospective community-based study. Pediatrics. 2006, 117: 836-842. 10.1542/peds.2005-0775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Gregory AM, O'Connor TG: Sleep problems in childhood: a longitudinal study of developmental change and association with behavioral problems. J Am Acad Child Psy. 2002, 41: 964-971. 10.1097/00004583-200208000-00015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Gruber R, Sadeh A, Raviv A: Instability of Sleep Patterns in Children with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Am Acad Child Psy. 2000, 39: 495-501. 10.1097/00004583-200004000-00019.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Stores G: A clinical guide to sleep disorders in children and adolescents. 2001, Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press, 20-21.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  9. Canivet C, Hagander B, Jakobsson I, Lanke J: Infantile colic--less common than previously estimated?. Acta Paediatrics. 1996, 85: 454-458. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1996.tb14060.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Reijneveld SA, Brugman E, Hirasing RA: Excessive infant crying: The impact of varying definitions. Pediatrics. 2001, 108: 893-897. 10.1542/peds.108.4.893.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Barr RG: Preventing SBS with an educational campaign on infant crying: 'the period of purple crying'. Proceedings of The European Conference on Shaken Baby Syndrome: 19-20. 2003, May ; Edinburgh

    Google Scholar 

  12. Anders TF, Keener M: Developmental course of nighttime sleep-wake patterns in full-term and premature infants during the first year of life. Sleep. 1985, 8: 173-192.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Moore T, Ucko LE: Night Waking in Early Infancy: Part I. Arc Dis Child. 1957, 32: 333-342. 10.1136/adc.32.164.333.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. St James-Roberts I: Helping parents to manage infant crying and sleeping: a review of the evidence and its implications for services. Child Abuse Review. 2007, 16: 47-69. 10.1002/car.968.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Anders TF, Halpern LF, Hua J: Sleeping through the night: a developmental perspective. Pediatrics. 1992, 90: 554-560.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Field T, Diego M, Hernandez-Reif M, Figueiredo B, Schanberg S, Kuhn C: Sleep disturbances in depressed pregnant women and their newborns. Infant Behav Dev. 2007, 30: 127-133. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.08.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. O'Connor TG, Caprariello P, Blackmore ER, Gregory AM, Glover V, Fleming P: Prenatal mood disturbance predicts sleep problems in infancy and toddlerhood. Early Hum Dev. 2007, 83: 451-458. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.08.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Armstrong KL, Van Haeringen AR, Dadds MR, Cash R: Sleep deprivation or postnatal depression in later infancy: separating the chicken from the egg. J Paediatr Child Health. 1998, 34: 260-262. 10.1046/j.1440-1754.1998.00213.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Hiscock H, Bayer J, Gold L, Hampton A, Ukoumunne OC, Wake M: Improving infant sleep and maternal mental health: a cluster randomised trial. Arc Dis Child. 2007, 92: 952-958. 10.1136/adc.2006.099812.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Gentile S: Use of contemporary antidepressants during breastfeeding: a proposal for a specific safety index. Drug Safety. 2007, 30: 107-121. 10.2165/00002018-200730020-00002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Hiscock H: Rock-a-bye baby? Parenting and infant sleep. Sleep Med Rev. 2010, 14: 85-87. 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.10.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Morrell J, Cortina-Borja M: The developmental change in strategies parents employ to settle young children to sleep, and their relationship to infant sleeping problems, as assessed by a new questionnaire: the parental interactive bedtime behaviour scale. Infant Child Dev. 2002, 11: 17-41. 10.1002/icd.251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. St James-Roberts I, Sleep J, Morris S, Owen C, Gillham P: Use of a behavioural programme in the first 3 months to prevent infant crying and sleeping problems. J Paediatr Child Health. 2001, 37: 289-297. 10.1046/j.1440-1754.2001.00699.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Wolfson A, Lacks P, Futterman A: Effects of parent training on infant sleeping patterns, parents' stress, and perceived parental confidence. Child Dev. 1992, 60: 41-48.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Morrell JM: The role of maternal cognitions in infant sleep as assessed by a new instrument, the maternal cognitions about infant sleep questionnaire. Child Psychol Psychiatr. 1999, 40: 247-258. 10.1111/1469-7610.00438.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Rautava P, Helenius H, Lehtonen L: Psychosocial predisposing factors for infantile colic. BMJ. 1993, 307: 600-604. 10.1136/bmj.307.6904.600.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Michelsson K, Rinne A, Paajanen S: Crying, feeding and sleeping patterns in 1 to 12-month-old infants. Child Care Hlth Dev. 1990, 16: 99-111. 10.1111/j.1365-2214.1990.tb00642.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Walker AM, Menaheim S: Intervention of supplementary carrying on normal baby crying patterns: a randomised study. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1994, 15: 174-178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Heine RG, Jaquiery A, Lubitz L, Cameron DJS, Catto-Smith AG: Role of gastro-oesophageal reflux in infant irritability. Arch Dis Child. 1995, 73: 121-125. 10.1136/adc.73.2.121.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Illingworth RS: Infantile colic revisited. Arch Dis Child. 1985, 60: 981-985. 10.1136/adc.60.10.981.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Iacono G, Carroccio A, Montalto G, Cavataio F, Bragion E, Lorello D, Balsamo V, Notarbartolo A: Severe infantile colic and food intolerance: a long-term prospective study. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 1991, 12: 332-335. 10.1097/00005176-199104000-00008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Gormally S: Clinical clues to organic etiologies in infants with colic. New Evidence on Unexplained Early Infant Crying: Its Origins, Nature and Management. Edited by: Barr RG, St James-Roberts I, Keefe MR. 2001, New Jersey, USA: Johnson & Johnson Pediatric Institute, 133-149.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Barr RG, Woolridge J, Hanley J: Effects of formula change on intestinal hydrogen production and crying and fussing behavior. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1991, 12: 248-253.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Forsyth BW, McCarthy PL, Leventhal JM: Problems of early infancy, formula changes, and mothers' beliefs about their infants. J Pediatr. 1985, 106: 1012-1017. 10.1016/S0022-3476(85)80260-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Jordan B, Heine RG, Meehan M, Catto-Smith AG, Lubitz L: Effect of antireflux medication, placebo and infant mental health intervention on persistent crying: a randomized clinical trial. J Paediatr Child H. 2006, 42: 49-58. 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2006.00786.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Olafsdottira E, Forsheib S, Flugea G, Markestada T: Randomised controlled trial of infantile colic treated with chiropractic spinal manipulation. Arch Dis Child. 2001, 84: 138-141. 10.1136/adc.84.2.138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Estep DC, Kulczycki A: Treatment of infant colic with amino acid-based infant formula: a preliminary study. Acta Paediatr. 2000, 89: 22-27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Swadling C, Griffiths P: Is modified cow's milk formula effective in reducing symptoms of infant colic?. Br J Community Nurs. 2003, 8: 24-27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Douglas PS, Hiscock H: The unsettled baby: crying out for an integrated, multidisciplinary primary care approach. Med J Australia. 2010, 193: 533-536.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. St James-Roberts I, Alvarez M, Csipke E, Abramsky T, Goodwin J, Sorgenfrei E: Infant crying and sleeping in London, Copenhagen and when parents adopt a "proximal" form of care. Pediatrics. 2006, 117: 1146-1155. 10.1542/peds.2005-2387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Mindell JA, Kuhn B, Lewin DS, Meltzer LJ, Sadeh A: Behavioral treatment of bedtime problems and night waking in infants and young children. Sleep. 2006, 29: 1263-1276.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Ramchandani P, Wiggs L, Webb V, Stores G: A systematic review of treatments for settling problems and nightwaking in young children. Br Med J. 2000, 320: 209-213. 10.1136/bmj.320.7229.209.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Hiscock H, Bayer JK, Hampton A, Ukoumunne OC, Wake M: Long-term mother and child mental health effects of a population-based infant sleep intervention: cluster-randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2008, 122: 621-627. 10.1542/peds.2007-3783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Hiscock H, Wake M: Randomised controlled trial of behavioural infant sleep intervention to improve infant sleep and maternal mood. Brit Med J. 2002, 324: 1062-10.1136/bmj.324.7345.1062.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Hall B, Cheskers J, Robinson A: Infantile colic: a systematic review of medical and conventional therapies. J Paediatr Child H.

  46. Hunziker UA, Barr RG: Increased carrying reduces infant crying: a randomised controlled trial. Pediatrics. 1986, 77: 641-648.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Barr RG, McMullan SJ, Spiess H, Leduc DG, Yaremko J, Barfield R, Francoeur TE, Hunziker UA: Carrying as colic "therapy": a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics. 1991, 87: 623-630.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Keefe MR, Lobo ML, Froese-Fretz A, Kotzer AM, Barbosa GA, Dudley WN: Effectiveness of an intervention for colic. Clin Pediatr. 2006, 45: 123-133. 10.1177/000992280604500203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Kerr SM, Jowett SA, Smith LN: Preventing sleep problems in infants: a randomised controlled trial. J Advanced Nursing. 1996, 24: 928-942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Nikolopoulou M, St James-Roberts I: Preventing sleep problems in infants who are at risk of developing them. Arch Dis Child. 2002, 88: 108-111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Symon BG, Marley JE, Martin AJ, Norman ER: Effect of a consultation teaching behaviour modification on sleep performance in infants: a randomised controlled trial. Med J Aust. 2005, 182: 215-218.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Bailey WT: A longitudinal study of fathers' involvement with young children: infancy to age 5 years. J Genet Psychol. 1994, 155: 331-339. 10.1080/00221325.1994.9914783.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Dennis CLE, Janssen PA, Singer J: Identifying women at risk for postpartum depression in the immediate postpartum period. Acta Psychiat Scand. 2004, 110: 338-346. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2004.00337.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Dennis C-L, Ross L: Women's perceptions of partner support and conflict in the development of partpartum depressive symptoms. J Adv Nurs. 2006, 56: 588-599. 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2006.04059.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Lemola S, Stadlmayr W, Grob A: Maternal adjustment five months after birth: the impact of the subjective experience of childbirth and emotional support from the partner. J Reprod Infant Psyc. 2007, 25: 190-202. 10.1080/02646830701467231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Armstrong KL, Quin RA, Dadds MR: The sleep patterns of normal children. Med J Aust. 1994, 161: 202-206.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. The Period of Purple Crying. [http://www.purplecrying.info]

  58. Henderson JMT, France KG, Owens JL, Blampied NM: Sleeping through the night: the consolidation of self-regulated sleep across the first year of life. Pediatrics. 2010, 126: 993-994.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Smart J, Hiscock H: Early infant crying and sleeping problems: a pilot study of impact on parental well-being and parent-endorsed strategies for management. J Paediatr Child H. 2007, 43: 284-290. 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2007.01060.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Barr RG, Kramer MS, Boisjoly C, McVey-White L, Pless IB: Parental diary of infant cry and fuss behaviour. Arch Dis Child. 1988, 63: 380-387. 10.1136/adc.63.4.380.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ: The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989, 28: 193-213. 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R: Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987, 150: 782-786. 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Liu X, Liu L, Owens JA, Kaplan DL: Sleep patterns and sleep problems among schoolchildren in the United States and China. Pediatrics. 2005, 115: 241-249. 10.1542/peds.2004-0815F.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Murray L, Carothers AD: The validation of the Edinburgh post-natal depression scale on a community sample. Brit J Psychiat. 1990, 157: 288-290. 10.1192/bjp.157.2.288.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Carey WB, McDevitt SC: Revision of the infant temperament questionnaire. Pediatrics. 1978, 61: 735-739.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Amir LH, Forster DA, Lumley J, McLachlan H: A descriptive study of mastitis in Australian breastfeeding women: incidence and determinants. BMC Public Health. 2007, 7: 62-10.1186/1471-2458-7-62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Forster D, McLachlan H, Lumley J, Beanland C, Waldenström U, Amir L: Two mid-pregnancy interventions to increase the initiation and duration of breastfeeding: a randomized controlled trial. Birth. 2004, 31: 176-182. 10.1111/j.0730-7659.2004.00302.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Sanson A, Nicholson J, Ungerer J, Zubrick S, Wilson K, Ainley J, Berthelsen D, Bittman M, Broom D, Harrison L, Rodgers B, Sawyer M, Silburn S, Strazdins L, Vimpani G, Wake M: Introducing the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. Australian Institute of Family Studies, Melbourne, LSAC discussion Paper No 1 2002

  69. Gibbons RD, Hedeker D: Application of random-effects probit regression models. J Consult Clin Psych. 1994, 62: 285-296.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  70. Hedeker D, Gibbons RD: A Random-Effects Ordinal Regression Model for Multilevel Analysis. Biometrics. 1994, 50: 933-944. 10.2307/2533433.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Lumley T, Diehr P, Emerson S, Chen L: The importance of the normality assumption in large public health data sets. Annu Rev Publ Health. 2002, 23: 151-169. 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.23.100901.140546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Sterne JAC, White IR, Carlin JB, Spratt M, Royston P, Kenward MG, Wood AM, Carpenter JR: Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. BMJ. 2009, 338-

    Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the Maternal and Child Health nurses and coordinators across the LGA's of Wyndham, Brimbank, Yarra City and Moonee Valley for their assistance in the recruitment of families. We would also like to thank all staff involved in the running of this research project: Tracey Kearins, Amy Coe, Zvezdana Bucalo, Jessica Antunovic, Raelene Rosicka, Marisa Baschuk, Anica Risteska, Dr. Gina Sartore and Dianne Ridley. Funding for this research has been provided by the Victorian Government Department of Education and Early Childhood Development (DEECD), the Scobie and Claire MacKinnon Trust and the Population Health Strategic Research Centre, Deakin University. Harriet Hiscock is supported by an Australian National Health & Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Career Development Award (Grant 607351) and Fiona Mensah is supported by the NHMRC Population Health Capacity Building Grant (Grant 436914).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Fallon Cook.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

FC and HH drafted the manuscript, with JB, HL, FM and WC contributing to several revisions. HH and JB are responsible for the study design. All authors have given approval for the final version to be published.

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cook, F., Bayer, J., Le, H.N. et al. Baby Business: a randomised controlled trial of a universal parenting program that aims to prevent early infant sleep and cry problems and associated parental depression. BMC Pediatr 12, 13 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2431-12-13

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2431-12-13

Keywords