Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Central nervous system antiretroviral efficacy in HIV infection: a qualitative and quantitative review and implications for future research

Abstract

Background

There is conflicting information as to whether antiretroviral drugs with better central nervous system (CNS) penetration (neuroHAART) assist in improving neurocognitive function and suppressing cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) HIV RNA. The current review aims to better synthesise existing literature by using an innovative two-phase review approach (qualitative and quantitative) to overcome methodological differences between studies.

Methods

Sixteen studies, all observational, were identified using a standard citation search. They fulfilled the following inclusion criteria: conducted in the HAART era; sample size > 10; treatment effect involved more than one antiretroviral and none had a retrospective design. The qualitative phase of review of these studies consisted of (i) a blind assessment rating studies on features such as sample size, statistical methods and definitions of neuroHAART, and (ii) a non-blind assessment of the sensitivity of the neuropsychological methods to HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder (HAND). During quantitative evaluation we assessed the statistical power of studies, which achieved a high rating in the qualitative analysis. The objective of the power analysis was to determine the studies ability to assess their proposed research aims.

Results

After studies with at least three limitations were excluded in the qualitative phase, six studies remained. All six found a positive effect of neuroHAART on neurocognitive function or CSF HIV suppression. Of these six studies, only two had statistical power of at least 80%.

Conclusions

Studies assessed as using more rigorous methods found that neuroHAART was effective in improving neurocognitive function and decreasing CSF viral load, but only two of those studies were adequately statistically powered. Because all of these studies were observational, they represent a less compelling evidence base than randomised control trials for assessing treatment effect. Therefore, large randomised trials are needed to determine the robustness of any neuroHAART effect. However, such trials must be longitudinal, include the full spectrum of HAND, ideally carefully control for co-morbidities, and be based on optimal neuropsychology methods.

Peer Review reports

Background

The possibility that some antiretroviral drugs with more efficient Central Nervous System (CNS) penetration as part of Highly Active Antiretroviral Therapy (HAART) may be associated with better neurocognitive (NC) functioning and more efficient cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) HIV RNA suppression than other ARVs has important clinical and therapeutic implications [1]. In this paper, we will refer to more efficient CNS HAART as neuroHAART.

First, if true, it indeed means that a non-negligible number of individuals with HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) are not receiving optimal treatment. Without proactive assessment of HAND, the individuals with asymptomatic neurocognitive impairment (ANI) and Mild Neurocognitive Impairment (MND) [2], which now represent the greatest proportion of HAND (as opposed to HIV-associated dementia; HAD in the pre-HAART era) are likely not to be considered for a specific therapeutic strategy. A French study [3] brings support to this potential sub-optimal treatment scenario. This study found that in the pre-HAART era, individuals with HAD were preferentially treated with antiretrovirals with greater CNS penetration. However, this was still sub-optimal as it used pre-HAART drugs. They then found that in the HAART era, the treatment strategy as assessed retrospectively, did not favour neuroHAART for individuals with HAD, while ANI and MND were not considered at all.

Second, and perhaps most importantly, there are currently no HAART guidelines for HAND [4]. While this topic is hotly debated [5], still an informative review of the literature has been missing.

Lastly, there is the prospect that some individuals at risk for HAND may benefit from preventative treatment. This question is currently being studied in an international large-scale trial: http://insight.ccbr.umn.edu/start/

The possible superior efficacy of neuroHAART, however, remains highly controversial in the HIV research community because studies have provided conflicting results. More importantly, the definitions of what constitutes neuroHAART vary (see [1] for review of neuroHAART definitions). The underlying premise that the brain is an HIV sanctuary site less amenable to effective systemic treatment is itself at the centre of a scientific debate [6]. However, evidence for the central nervous system (CNS) being a virological sanctuary site can be drawn from clinical [7], CSF [8–10] and human neuropathological studies [11, 12].

The aim of this review is to better synthesise the results of existing studies conducted to address the issue of the potential superior efficacy of neuroHAART on brain functions and CSF HIVRNA suppression. This review is based on an innovative staged review strategy which was designed to overcome study design and neuroHAART definition variations. First, each study was reviewed using a blind quality scoring for the presence or absence of major design, methodological and analysis features (adapted from [13]) combined with a non-blind assessment of appropriate use of demographic and longitudinal (practice effect) corrections on neuropsychological data, as well as test sensitivity to HAND. Studies with at least three methodological limitations were excluded (incidentally retaining studies within the upper confidence interval limits of the quality scoring distribution). Remaining studies were then evaluated for statistical power, that is their ability to detect a significant difference based on their stated aims. The implications of the findings are discussed for future research.

Methods

Study selection and data extraction

Individual studies were retrieved via a search of Pubmed with the following keywords here presented in alphabetical order: antiretroviral, CSF, cognitive functions, CNS, HAART, HIV RNA, HIV-associated dementia, HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders, HIV/AIDS, index, neuropsychological functions, penetrance, penetration.

The following combinations were used: 1. HIV-associated dementia and antiretroviral, CNS, penetrance, penetration, index, HAART, CSF, HIV RNA. 2. HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders and antiretroviral, CNS, penetrance, penetration, index, HAART, CSF, HIV RNA. 3. Neuropsychological functions, HIV/AIDS, and antiretroviral, CNS, penetrance, penetration, index, HAART, CSF, HIV RNA. 4. Cognitive functions, and antiretroviral, CNS, penetrance, penetration, index, HAART, CSF, HIV RNA.

The following criteria were then used to select studies:

  1. 1.

    Reports had to be conducted in the HAART era (that is after 1996).

  2. 2.

    Group comparisons had to have subject numbers of 10 or more; lower numbers provide unstable effect sizes.

  3. 3.

    Reports had to investigate the effect of more than one single drug on an existing HAART regimen because the current review was focused on multiple ARV agents' effect.

  4. 4.

    Studies had to not be based on retrospective data analyses. Some of those studies not only included a sub-optimal definition of HAND, but also were prone to systemic biases in the baseline clinical status of individuals starting a more or less efficient neuroHAART regimen [14].

Using these criteria 16 studies were identified and they are detailed in Table 1. Seven were excluded and their references are included in additional file 1.

Table 1 Review of studies that have assessed the effect of CNS penetrating ARTs on NP performance and/or on CSF HIV RNA

Qualitative analysis

Blinded and non-blinded review for quality scoring

Because the quality (as assessed by statistical power, sample size, reliability of assessment of HAND, definitions of CNS penetration efficiency (CPE) score, and neuropsychological methods) of studies varied substantially, we computed a score to rank each study on these factors. This scoring form was adapted from [13]. The statistical methods and design used in each study were reviewed blindly by E.K.W who was provided with a printed copy of the studies (after removal of authorship identifiers and without the title, the abstract and the discussion) and a scoring form (additional file 2). In brief, we used 15 quality criteria that fell into 5 categories (Blinded: design, outcomes, subjects, controls, and un-blinded outcomes). A not-applicable option was provided for criteria that might not apply for all studies (e.g., it made no sense to examine whether there were demographic differences between cases and controls in observational studies with no control arm). Importantly, a non-blinded review of the presence or absence of neuropsychological cross-sectional norms, longitudinal norms (correction for practice effect) and validity and sensitivity of tests used to assess NC performance in HAND (including size of the neuropsychological battery) was then performed by a neuropsychologist (L.A.C.). This review was non-blinded because it sometimes required the exploration of previous papers by the same research team. Therefore a choice was made that all papers would be treated equally with a non-blind assessment.

As mentioned above, in some instances, quality criteria were "not applicable" to some included studies. To account for these cases and still rank the study on a similar scale, a total percentage score was developed. This total percentage score was computed based on the aforementioned criteria and studies were ranked (see Table 1). A score less than or equal to 80% meant that a study presented at least three or more significant methodological limitations. This continuous score was developed to be more transparent than assessing quality categories such as "poor", "medium" or "high" quality (additional file 3).

Whilst our qualitative analysis necessarily involved subjectivity, when presented with studies of poor or variable quality some subjective assessment is needed to ensure that like is compared with like; a well designed RCT should ideally not be grouped with a retrospective observational study in such an analysis. Whilst Finney argued in a seminal paper on meta-analysis that an assessment can be purely qualitative when studies are heterogeneous in nature or provide low quality evidence, [15] our scoring method enabled us to transparently identify qualitatively similar studies and analyse them further to provide some quantitative conclusions.

Quantitative analysis

Power computations

The objective of the power analysis was to determine the study's ability to assess its proposed research aims. Accordingly, we developed the following strategy: the power computations were conducted [16] using GPower version 3.1 [17] for the studies with a quality scoring greater than 80%. The power projections were made for conventionally small, medium and large effect sizes (namely, 0.20; 0.50 and 0.80) [18]; and were made separately for cross-sectional and longitudinal designs and univariate and multivariate designs (see Figure 1). The studies were then checked against an "acceptable" criterion: power of 80%; two-tailed with a p-value less than 0.05. Using this criterion for a medium effect size (d = 0.50), we selected which studies in the review were conducted with acceptable power, though it should be acknowledged that the definition of acceptable power is inherently subjective [19].

Figure 1
figure 1

Power projections. The figure provides power projections for four different study designs in order to assess NeuroHAART effect on neurocognitive or CSF HIVRNA suppression (i.e., panel 1 illustrates a cross-sectional design; panel 2 a longitudinal design; panel 3 an univariate regression model design, and panel 4 a multi-variable regression model design). The first part of the power projection is dedicated to sample's comparisons and the second part to the testing of the magnitude of the neuroHAART effect on neurocognitive functions or CSF HIV RNA. For each panel, power projections were computed to detect a small, medium and large effect size. In each panel, the six studies found to be of "appropriate quality" were rated against the power's projection for their relevant design and arrows indicates the N enrolled for each study. In addition a red circle in each power panel indicates the criterion against which studies were defined as having "appropriate versus non-appropriate power" and this was selected for the medium effect size.

Results

The quality scores from the 16 studies were normally distributed with a mean of 76.6% and a standard deviation of 12.8% (additional file 3). Of the 16 studies analysed, six had less than three methodological limitations, and none obtained a quality of 100% which would have reflected an optimal design for addressing their research aims (see Table 1). Four of the six studies retained were longitudinal; all included individuals who had been well characterised neuropsychologically and clinically to determine the full HAND spectrum, while using appropriate normative data when required. The four retained longitudinal studies included statistical methods taking into account the potential practice effect associated with repeated neuropsychological testing. All these studies found a positive effect of NeuroHAART on NC yielding small to large effect sizes (additional file 4). One cross-sectional study and one longitudinal study also found that CSF HIVRNA was more effectively suppressed as a function of a higher CPE.

Studies' quality scoring highlights (see also Table 1)

Heterogeneity of study samples

A number of studies included individuals with various HIV-associated neurological conditions. Others did not carefully report inclusion or exclusion criteria. A few studies did not provide enough information to assess the baseline rate of neuropsychological impairment in their cohort. Lastly, convenience comparisons were sometimes used between a sample on and off NeuroHAART while these samples differed on a number of important clinical characteristics.

Insensitive assessment tools in the case of neurocognitive studies

Among the studies investigating NC functions, five (out of 11) included either a small number of tests or clinical scales rather than standard neuropsychological assessment. Others used non-cognitive endpoints such as time to HAND and survival time, which do not directly address potential change in NC functions, but only represent surrogates that are less sensitive to neuropsychological change.

Lack of norms and practice effect correction

One study did not use adequate normative data correction for demographic factors potentially misclassifying impairment rate at baseline. Among the neuropsychological longitudinal studies, 50% did not correct for practice.

Assessment time points

One major point in the heterogeneity of the published study designs was the variation in test retest intervals. Among studies with higher quality scoring, only one used a short test retest at 3 months. The four others used at least a 12 month retest interval.

Studies quantitative analysis highlights (see also Table 1)

Power

Using our criterion for "appropriate power" we found that two of the six studies with higher quality scoring were conducted with appropriate power (medium effect size that is d = .50; 80%, 2-tailed with p = .05). Figure 1 also illustrates which criterion was used for the four different study designs).

Other major issues in all studies

Non-reported p-values or data to compute effect sizes

The majority of studies reported p-values or data allowing effect sizes' conversion when appropriate. Two studies did not provide exact p-values or data for their non-significant findings (see legend for Table 1).

Various definitions of neuroHAART

Studies used various definitions of neuroHAART (see Table 1) sometimes including less than the conventional definition for HAART without good rationale (i.e., less than three ARVs in some comparisons). Seven studies used a simple continuous aggregate of ARVs with good penetration. Moreover, five of 16 studies (excluding the CNS Penetration Effectiveness (CPE) validation study) have used what has been termed the CPE score. This is an empirically derived score that is a summed aggregate of the individual scores for each ARV in a HAART regimen.

Discussion

Our search criteria identified 16 studies that addressed the issue of neuroHAART efficacy.

Qualitative analysis of these studies for the presence of less than three major methodological limitations (which incidentally corresponded to selecting studies within the 5% upper bound of the Student t distribution) showed that 37.5% (6 out of the 16) met the criterion for "higher quality". The main methodological limitations, which are relatively common to the field of clinical research in NeuroAIDS, were clinical heterogeneity of the sample studied or compared, unclear inclusion/exclusion criteria, insensitivity/brevity of the neuropsychological battery in 45% of studies, and lack of correction for practice effect (this was found in 50% of longitudinal studies). Other less common limitations were lack of normative correction for baseline demographic factors, and the definition of neuroHAART not fully described to allow direct replication.

Among the studies that scored above the quality scoring cut-off, it should be noted that none achieved a 100% score. This reflects the practical and scientific constraints of clinical research studies. One study did not provide clear enough inclusion/exclusion criteria; one did not provide a clear neuroHAART definition for direct replication; one used a medium-size neuropsychological battery and one a very brief neuropsychological battery; one included comparison groups with a lack of clinical homogeneity, and one did not include correction for practice effect. It should be noted here that the blind review allowed applying the same strictness to the studies' evaluation for all studies.

Moreover, our quantitative analysis on the remaining six studies which met an adequate quality scoring showed that two met the criterion for 80% a priori power. The advantage of our staged strategy was that only studies with a higher quality, hence greater comparability could be assessed in the quantitative phase.

Altogether, this review clearly demonstrates that methodological limitations and in most studies, lack of power render the literature difficult to readily interpret without both the qualitative and quantitative approach outlined in this paper. In favour of a positive NeuroHAART effect are the six studies with higher quality scoring, however most are underpowered and none were randomised. Nonetheless, all six controlled for factors that may have been affected by non-randomization using multivariate analyses and still yielded results in favour of a positive neuroHAART effect on NC functions or CSF HIVRNA suppression.

While these results are not definitive they may assist the clinician in decision making as no negative effect of NeuroHAART was predominant. It seems reasonable, therefore, to consider neuroHAART whenever possible in a patient with HAND, notwithstanding issues of adherence and resistance to particular ARVs. However, the most definitive answer to the issue of the potential superior efficacy of neuroHAART remains randomised controlled clinical trials conducted in different regions of the world to provide cumulative evidence.

Limitations of the current qualitative and quantitative review are that it was based only on published studies and did not account for any publication bias towards only reporting positive effects. However, because the existence of any neuroHAART effect is an area of great uncertainty in NeuroHIV and HIV/AIDS research, we contend that a negative finding is of substantial interest and as likely to lead to a publication as a positive finding. Finally it could be argued that our qualitative phase should have selected a stricter level of quality. However, as discussed above, a stricter level of quality would have excluded almost all studies form the quantitative analysis. Moreover, despite methodological limitations, observational studies are an important phase of clinical research, providing preliminary evidence regarding treatment efficacy.

From our analysis of existing studies there are several key aspects that should be considered in the design of a potential future trial.

  1. 1.

    A priori power analyses should be conducted for the ability to detect at least a medium effect size. This implies that a future trial should include at least 100 subjects in each arm (see also Figure 1; multivariate graph). This also takes into account the full HAND spectrum including at least mild neurocognitive disorder (MND) [2] and that adequate power is needed to detect a relatively small effect (the treatment effect) in individuals who are not demented, but do have neurocognitive difficulties.

  2. 2.

    Adequate outcome measures should be selected. Even if a brief assessment is selected, it should be targeted to include tests of psychomotor speed, working memory as well as learning and memory to have a wider range of potential benefit from neuroHAART [20]. Related to this issue, and because of the high complexity of the neuropsychological data in this type of study, the inclusion of a senior neuropsychologist in the research team is essential. Moreover, the use of neuro/psychology graduates for a high quality and standard administration of neuropsychological instruments including computerised battery is highly recommended. Because the NC measure represents the main outcome of those studies, special care in the data collection and data management should be a basic requirement. This will reduce measurement error and systematic biases that are likely with a poorly trained staff or staff who do not have the basic clinical skills to examine patients with cognitive impairment.

  3. 3.

    Adequate sampling: the study should carefully consider what types of patients are to be included with clear inclusion and exclusion criteria. Principally, the current HAART status and HAART duration should be considered, (See [21] for extensive consideration on this issue). Lastly, if the study includes a test of HIV RNA change, a baseline level of detection may be set as an inclusion criterion to improve the homogeneity in each arm on this aspect. Other aspects that may be considered to improve arm homogeneity are HIV duration, nadir CD4, and previous HAART history.

  4. 4.

    Adequate study time points: the study should be longitudinal and select an early follow-up to detect HIV RNA changes in the CSF and plasma as well as NC change (between 4 and 7 weeks after treatment initiation) and, a longer follow-up ideally at about 48 weeks, to detect long-term NC change. The risk of a shorter term trial is to produce negative or neutral findings when actually a positive effect is at play [21].

  5. 5.

    Adequate analytical strategies: in a randomised trial, which by design minimize systematic biases between treatment arms on the outcome measures, extra consideration in the use of neuropsychological normative data to determine baseline impairment rate is needed. In some instances adaptive randomization may offer a flexible solution (see [22] for further discussion on this issue).

  6. 5.

    We would recommend the use of the CPE score pending improved definitions and over older definitions. This would allow planning preliminary analyses with the version of a current CPE at the time of enrolment in future clinical trials. Uniform use of the CPE score would make direct comparisons of the regimens used in different studies easier and enable a larger evidence base to define the most effective neuroHAART regimens to be compiled. In the future definitions, the role of a potentially impaired BBB should also be considered [23]. Also, the adverse effect of some ARVs on the cardio-vascular system would need to be taken into account in the new version of a CPE score as they have been associated with NC impairment in the HIV population [24].

Conclusions

Studies assessed as using more rigorous methods found that neuroHAART was effective in improving neurocognitive function and decreasing CSF viral load, but only two of those studies were adequately statistically powered. Because all of these studies were observational, they represent a less compelling evidence base than randomised control trials for assessing treatment effect. Therefore, large randomised trials are needed to determine the robustness of neuroHAART effect. However, such trials must be longitudinal, include the full spectrum of HAND, ideally carefully control for co-morbidities and be based on optimal neuropsychology methods.

Abbreviations

CNS:

Central nervous system

HAND:

HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder

HAART:

Highly Active Antiretroviral Therapy

CSF:

cerebrospinal fluid

CPE:

CNS penetrance efficiency

ANI:

asymptomatic neurocognitive impairment

MND:

Mild Neurocognitive Impairment

NC:

neurocognitive

RCT:

randomized control trial

BBB:

blood brain barrier.

References

  1. Wright E: Neurocognitive impairment and neuroCART. Curr Opin HIV AIDS. 2011, 2011: 5-

    Google Scholar 

  2. Antinori A, Arendt G, Becker JT, Brew BJ, Byrd DA, Cherner M, Clifford DB, Cinque P, Epstein LG, Goodkin K, et al: Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology. 2007, 69 (18): 1789-1799. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000287431.88658.8b.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Lanoy E, Guiguet M, Bentata M, Rouveix E, Dhiver C, Poizot-Martin I, Costagliola D, Gasnault J: Survival after neuroAIDS: association with antiretroviral CNS Penetration-Effectiveness score. Neurology. 2011, 76 (7): 644-651. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31820c3089.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Antiretroviral therapy. [http://www.who.int/hiv/topics/treatment/en/]

  5. ASHM: Proceedings Report - Antiretroviral Guidelines Consensus Discussion (2010). ASHM: 2010; Sydney, Australia. 2010, ASHM, [http://www.ashm.org.au/default2.asp?active_page_id=252]

    Google Scholar 

  6. Dahl V, Josefsson L, Palmer S: HIV reservoirs, latency, and reactivation: Prospects for eradication. Antiviral Res. 2009, 4: 4-

    Google Scholar 

  7. Robertson KR, Smurzynski M, Parsons TD, Wu K, Bosch RJ, Wu J, McArthur JC, Collier AC, Evans SR, Ellis RJ: The prevalence and incidence of neurocognitive impairment in the HAART era. AIDS. 2007, 21 (14): 1915-1921. 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32828e4e27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Cunningham P, Smith D, Satchell C, Cooper DA, Brew B: Evidence for independant development of resistance to HIV-1 reverse transcriptase inhibitors in the cerebrospinal fluid. AIDS. 2000, 14: 1949-1954. 10.1097/00002030-200009080-00010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Staprans S, Marlowe N, Glidden D, Novakovic-Agopian T, Grant RM, Heyes M, Aweeka F, Deeks S, Price RW: Time course of cerebrospinal fluid responses to antiretroviral therapy: evidence for variable compartmentalization of infection. AIDS. 1999, 13: 1051-1061. 10.1097/00002030-199906180-00008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Strain MC, Letendre S, Pillai SK, Russell T, Ignacio CC, Gunthard HF, Good B, Smith DM, Wolinsky SM, Furtado M, et al: Genetic composition of human immunodeficiency virus type 1 in cerebrospinal fluid and blood without treatment and during failing antiretroviral therapy. Journal of Virology. 2005, 79 (3): 1772-1788. 10.1128/JVI.79.3.1772-1788.2005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Smit TK, Brew BJ, Tourtellotte W, Morgello S, Gelman BB, Saksena NK: Independent evolution of human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) drug resistance mutations in diverse areas of the brain in HIV-infected patients, with and without dementia, on antiretroviral treatment. Journal of Virology. 2004, 78 (18): 10133-10148. 10.1128/JVI.78.18.10133-10148.2004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Wong JK, Ignacio CC, Torriani F, Havlir D, Fitch NJ, Richman DD: In vivo compartmentalization of human immunodeficiency virus: evidence from the examination of pol sequences from autopsy tissues. J Virol. 1997, 71 (3): 2059-2071.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Berman NG, Parker RA: Meta-analysis: neither quick nor easy. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2002, 2 (10): 10-

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Garvey L, Winston A, Walsh J, Post F, Porter K, Gazzard B, Fisher M, Leen C, Pillay D, Hill T, et al: Antiretroviral therapy CNS penetration and HIV-1-associated CNS disease. Neurology. 2011, 76 (8): 693-700. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31820d8b0b.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Finney DJ: A statistician looks at met-analysis. J Clin Epidemiol. 1995, 48 (1): 87-103; discussion 105-108. 10.1016/0895-4356(94)00096-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Hoenig JM, Heisey DM: The Abuse of Power: The Pervasive Fallacy of Power Calculations for Data Analysis. The American Statistician. 2001, 55 (1): 1-6. 10.1198/000313001300339860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G: Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods. 2009, 41 (4): 1149-1160. 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Cohen J: Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 1988, New York: Academic Press, 2

    Google Scholar 

  19. Cochran WG: Sampling Techniques. 1977, Wiley Inc

    Google Scholar 

  20. Cysique L, Maruff P, Brew B: The neuropsychological profile of symptomatic, AIDS and ADC patients in the pre-HAART era: a meta-analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2006, 12: 1-15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Cysique LA, Brew BJ: Neuropsychological functioning and antiretroviral treatment in HIV/AIDS: a review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2009, 19 (2): 169-185. 10.1007/s11065-009-9092-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. May S, Letendre S, Haubrich R, McCutchan JA, Heaton R, Capparelli E, Ellis R: Meeting practical challenges of a trial involving a multitude of treatment regimens: an example of a multi-center randomized controlled clinical trial in neuroAIDS. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol. 2007, 2 (1): 97-104. 10.1007/s11481-006-9057-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Avison MJ, Nath A, Greene-Avison R, Schmitt FA, Greenberg RN, Berger JR: Neuroimaging correlates of HIV-associated BBB compromise. J Neuroimmunol. 2004, 157 (1-2): 140-146. 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2004.08.025.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Wright EJ, Grund B, Robertson K, Brew BJ, Roediger M, Bain MP, Drummond F, Vjecha MJ, Hoy J, Miller C, et al: Cardiovascular risk factors associated with lower baseline cognitive performance in HIV-positive persons. Neurology. 2010, 75 (10): 864-873. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f11bd8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Antinori A, Giancola ML, Grisetti S, Soldani F, Alba L, Liuzzi G: Factors influencing virological response to antiretroviral drugs in cerebrospinal fluid of advanced HIV-1-infected patients. AIDS. 2002, 16: 1867-1876. 10.1097/00002030-200209270-00003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Chang L, Ernst T, Witt MD, Ames N, Walot I, Jovicich J, DeSilva M, Trivedi N, Speck O, Miller EN: Persistent brain abnormalities in antiretroviral-naive HIV patients 3 months after HAART. Antivir Ther. 2003, 8 (1): 17-26.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Cysique L, Maruff P, Brew B: Antiretroviral therapy in HIV infection: are neurologically active drugs important?. Archives of Neurology. 2004, 61 (11): 1699-1704. 10.1001/archneur.61.11.1699.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Cysique LA, Maruff P, Brew BJ: Variable benefit in neuropsychological function in HIV-infected HAART-treated patients. Neurology. 2006, 66 (9): 1447-1450. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000210477.63851.d3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Cysique LA, Vaida F, Letendre S, Gibson S, Cherner M, Woods SP, McCutchan JA, Heaton RK, Ellis RJ: Dynamics of cognitive change in impaired HIV-positive patients initiating antiretroviral therapy. Neurology. 2009, 73 (5): 342-348. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181ab2b3b.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. De Luca A, Ciancio BC, Larussa D, Murri R, Cingolani A, Rizzo MG, Giancola ML, Ammassari A, Ortona L: Correlates of independent HIV-1 replication in the CNS and of its control by antiretrovirals. Neurology. 2002, 59: 342-347.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Eggers C, Hertogs K, Sturenburg HJ, van Lunzen J, Stellbrink HJ: Delayed central nervous system virus suppression during highly active antiretroviral therapy is associated with HIV encephalopathy, but not with viral drug resistance or poor central nervous system drug penetration. Aids. 2003, 17 (13): 1897-1906. 10.1097/00002030-200309050-00008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Marra CM, Lockhart D, Zunt JR, Perrin M, Coombs RW, Collier AC: Changes in CSF and plasma HIV-1 RNA and cognition after starting potent antiretroviral therapy. Neurology. 2003, 60: 1388-1390.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Marra CM, Zhao Y, Clifford DB, Letendre S, Evans S, Henry K, Ellis RJ, Rodriguez B, Coombs RW, Schifitto G, et al: Impact of combination antiretroviral therapy on cerebrospinal fluid HIV RNA and neurocognitive performance. Aids. 2009, 23 (11): 1359-1366. 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32832c4152.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Letendre S, McCutchan J, Childers M, Woods S, Lazzaretto D, Heaton R, Grant I, Ellis R, Group. H: Enhancing antiretroviral therapy for human immunodeficiency virus cognitive disorders. Annals of Neurology. 2004, 56 (3): 416-423. 10.1002/ana.20198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Letendre S, Marquie-Beck J, Capparelli E, Best B, Clifford D, Collier AC, Gelman BB, McArthur JC, McCutchan JA, Morgello S, et al: Validation of the CNS Penetration-Effectiveness rank for quantifying antiretroviral penetration into the central nervous system. Arch Neurol. 2008, 65 (1): 65-70. 10.1001/archneurol.2007.31.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Patel K, Ming X, Williams PL, Robertson KR, Oleske JM, Seage GR: Impact of HAART and CNS-penetrating antiretroviral regimens on HIV encephalopathy among perinatally infected children and adolescents. Aids. 2009, 23 (14): 1893-1901. 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32832dc041.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Sacktor N, Tarwater PM, Skolasky RL, McArthur J, Selnes OA, Becker J, Cohen B, Miller EN: CSF antiretroviral drug penetrance and the treatment of HIV-associated psychomotor slowing. Neurology. 2001, 57: 542-544.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Sevigny JJ, Albert SM, McDermott MP, McArthur JC, Sacktor N, Conant K, Schifitto G, Selnes OA, Stern Y, McClernon DR, et al: Evaluation of HIV RNA and markers of immune activation as predictors of HIV-associated dementia. Neurology. 2004, 63: 2084-2090.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Smurzynski M, Wu K, Letendre S, Robertson K, Bosch RJ, Clifford DB, Evans S, Collier AC, Taylor M, Ellis R: Effects of central nervous system antiretroviral penetration on cognitive functioning in the ALLRT cohort. AIDS. 2011, 25 (3): 357-365. 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32834171f8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Tozzi V, Balestra P, Salvatori MF, Vlassi C, Liuzzi G, Giancola ML, Giulianelli M, Narciso P, Antinori A: Changes in cognition during antiretroviral therapy: comparison of 2 different ranking systems to measure antiretroviral drug efficacy on HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2009, 52 (1): 56-63. 10.1097/QAI.0b013e3181af83d6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Brain Sciences Institute fellowship at the University of New South Wales and NHMRC Project Grant ID568746 (CIA-Cysique).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lucette A Cysique.

Additional information

Competing interests

Dr. Lucette Cysique has received honoraria from ViiV Healthcare and Abbott

Prof. Brew has received honoraria from ViiV Healthcare, Abbott, and Merck.

Authors' contributions

LAC has designed the study, conducted the studies' search, conducted the non-blind review, conducted the statistical analyses under the supervision of EKW, written the first draft of the paper, finalize the manuscript. EKW has conducted the blind review, overseen the statistical analyses, and participate to the writing of the manuscript. BJB has participated in the study design and the writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

12883_2011_551_MOESM1_ESM.PDF

Additional file 1: Excluded studies per our criteria of exclusion. detailed references of the excluded studies. (PDF 8 KB)

12883_2011_551_MOESM2_ESM.PDF

Additional file 2: Quality scoring table and instructions for NeuroHAART studies. This table and instructions are an exact reproduction of those designed for the blind review of studies. (PDF 93 KB)

12883_2011_551_MOESM3_ESM.PDF

Additional file 3: Quality scores distribution in the 16 observational NeuroHAART studies. Figures and details of the quality scores obtained by each study. (PDF 81 KB)

12883_2011_551_MOESM4_ESM.PDF

Additional file 4: Details of the effect sizes' computations. Figure providing the effects sizes in the 6 studies with a quality score > 80%. (PDF 164 KB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cysique, L.A., Waters, E.K. & Brew, B.J. Central nervous system antiretroviral efficacy in HIV infection: a qualitative and quantitative review and implications for future research. BMC Neurol 11, 148 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-11-148

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-11-148

Keywords