Skip to main content

Brains, tools, innovation and biogeography in crows and ravens

Abstract

Background

Crows and ravens (Passeriformes: Corvus) are large-brained birds with enhanced cognitive abilities relative to other birds. They are among the few non-hominid organisms on Earth to be considered intelligent and well-known examples exist of several crow species having evolved innovative strategies and even use of tools in their search for food. The 40 Corvus species have also been successful dispersers and are distributed on most continents and in remote archipelagos.

Results

This study presents the first molecular phylogeny including all species and a number of subspecies within the genus Corvus. We date the phylogeny and determine ancestral areas to investigate historical biogeographical patterns of the crows. Additionally, we use data on brain size and a large database on innovative behaviour and tool use to test whether brain size (i) explains innovative behaviour and success in applying tools when foraging and (ii) has some correlative role in the success of colonization of islands. Our results demonstrate that crows originated in the Palaearctic in the Miocene from where they dispersed to North America and the Caribbean, Africa and Australasia. We find that relative brain size alone does not explain tool use, innovative feeding strategies and dispersal success within crows.

Conclusions

Our study supports monophyly of the genus Corvus and further demonstrates the direction and timing of colonization from the area of origin in the Palaearctic to other continents and archipelagos. The Caribbean was probably colonized from North America, although some North American ancestor may have gone extinct, and the Pacific was colonized multiple times from Asia and Australia. We did not find a correlation between relative brain size, tool use, innovative feeding strategies and dispersal success. Hence, we propose that all crows and ravens have relatively large brains compared to other birds and thus the potential to be innovative if conditions and circumstances are right.

Background

Crows are large passerine birds that are considered intelligent because of their flexible behaviour, problem-solving abilities and social learning [1, 2]. Several species show a number of fascinating innovations including tool use in their foraging, the best-known example being that of the New Caledonian crow (C. moneduloides) [35]. Such innovations in foraging are not only unique but are also expected to require increased cognitive abilities, which have been shown to be related to a brain size relatively larger than that of other birds [1, 4, 6]. Thus, the combination of opportunistic behaviour and intelligence should make corvids highly adaptable, competitive and potentially good colonizers of new environments [7, 8].

The family Corvidae (crows, jays, magpies and allies) contains 117 species [9] distributed across most continents except Antarctica. Within the family, crows (genus Corvus) make up about one third of the species diversity (40 species) and they occur on all continents except South America and Antarctica as well as in remote archipelagos such as Hawaii, Micronesia and Melanesia [10]. The Corvidae is part of the core Corvoidea radiation that contains more than 750 species. Recent studies have argued that the core Corvoidea originated in an archipelago environment north of Australia in the late Oligocene/early Miocene and dispersed via the historically complex Indo-Pacific archipelagos to the rest of the world [11]. Thus, we may expect that some or all of the core Corvoidea’s member groups could have been preadapted for dispersal and colonization and in the case of Corvus, it can be expected that this combined with large brains and the increased associated cognitive abilities would make them ideal dispersers and colonizers across the planet [8].

Species-level systematics within Corvus has been based largely on morphological data [12] or very sparse sampling for molecular phylogenies e.g. [1315] and even vocalizations have been used to infer phylogeny e.g. C. enca and C. mellori, [16, 17]. A molecular phylogeny based on extensive taxon sampling is required to establish systematic relationships within Corvus so that questions pertaining to historical biogeography, brain size and the evolution of innovative foraging habits and tool use might be addressed. Additionally, a robust and densely sampled phylogeny will provide a framework for future work on plumage evolution and various aspects of macroecology and macroevolution.

In the present study, we present a molecular phylogeny including all extant crow species and a number of subspecies sometimes assigned species rank [10]. We use the phylogeny to assess systematic relationships and to elucidate historical biogeographical patterns by dating the phylogeny and estimating ancestral areas across the tree. Furthermore, taking into account the Corvus phylogeny, we test whether (i) brain size is correlated with the ability to disperse to and colonize islands and (ii) brain size correlates with innovative feeding behaviour and tool use within crows.

Methods

Taxon sampling and laboratory procedures

We sampled all forty extant species of Corvus[9] (Table 1). Where possible we included multiple individuals and, for widespread species, multiple subspecies (e.g. Corvus enca Corvus macrorhynchos Corvus coronoides and Corvus frugilegus). We also included some well-documented closely related genera to test for monophyly of Corvus: Garrulus Pica Nucifraga[14]. Lanius was used to root the tree.

Table 1 List of taxa included in the study

Two nuclear gene regions, ornithine decarboxylase (ODC) introns 6 to 7 (chromosome 3), and glyceraldehyde-3-phosphodehydrogenase (GAPDH) intron-11 (chromosome 1), and two mitochondrial markers NADH dehydrogenase subunit 2 (ND2) and subunit 3 (ND3) were sequenced and used to estimate phylogenetic relationships. Primer pairs used for amplification were: ND2: Lmet [18]/H6312 [19]; ND3: ND3-L10755/ND3-H11151 [20]; ODC: OD6/OD8 [21], G3P13/G3P14b [22]. For the old museum specimens we only sequenced the mitochondrial genes. Corresponding laboratory procedures for study skins are detailed in Irestedt et al. [23]. Additional internal primers were designed for this study, ND3-corvR1: GTCAAATAGTAGAAACAGGATTGC; ND3-CorvF1: TTTTCAATTCGATTCTTCCTAGT; ND2-CorvR1: CTTGAACTAGAAAGTATTTGGTTGC; ND2-CorvF2:CCCCTAATCTCAAAATCTCACCA; ND2-CorvR2: CCTTGTAGGACTTCTGGGAATC; ND2-CorvF3: CTAGGACTAGTGCCATTTCACTT; ND2-CorvR3: AGATAGAGGAGAAGGCCATAATT; ND2-CorvF4: CTGAATAGGACTAAACCAAACACAA; ND2-CorvR4: AGTGTTAGTAGGAGGATTGTGCT; ND2-CorvF5: CCACACTAATAACTGCATGAACAAA; ND2-CorvR5: TGTGGGGTGGAAGTGTGATTGT; ND2-CorvF6: TCACTACTGGGCCTCTTCTTCTA. Purified PCR products were cycle-sequenced using the Big Dye terminator chemistry (ABI, Applied Biosystems) in both directions with the same primers used for PCR amplification and run on an automated AB 3100 DNA sequencer. Sequences were assembled with SeqMan II (DNASTAR). Positions where the nucleotide could not be determined with certainty were coded with the appropriate IUPAC code. GenBank accession numbers are provided in Table 1.

Alignment and phylogenetic analyses

Sequence alignment was performed using MegAlign. The concatenated alignment consisted of 2346 base pairs (bp) and the lengths of the individual alignments were GAPDH: 299 bp, ODC intron-6 and 7: 611 bp, NADH dehydrogenase subunit 2: 1041 and NADH dehydrogenase subunit 3: 395 bp. Coding genes (ND2 and ND3) were checked for the presence of stop codons or insertion/deletion events that would have disrupted the reading frame. We used Bayesian inference [24, 25], as implemented in MrBayes 3.1.2 [26, 27] to estimate phylogenetic relationships. The most appropriate substitution models were determined with MrModeltest 2.0 [28], using the Akaike information criterion [29, 30]. Bayesian analyses for the concatenated data set were performed allowing the different parameters (base frequencies, rate matrix or transition/transversion ratio, shape parameter, proportion of invariable sites) to vary between the six partitions (GAPDH, ODC, 1st, 2nd, 3 rd codon positions for mtDNA and tRNA), i.e. mixed-models analyses [27, 28]. Two independent runs initiated from random starting trees were performed for each data set, and in all MrBayes analyses, the Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) was run using Metropolis-coupling, with one cold and three heated chains, for 10 million (individual analyses) to 20 million (combined analysis) iterations with trees sampled every 100 iterations. The number of iterations discarded before the chains had reached their apparent target distributions (i.e. the length of the “burn-in” period) was graphically estimated using AWTY [31, 32] by monitoring the change in cumulative split frequencies, and by the loglikelihood values and posterior probabilities for splits and model parameters. We used GARLI 0.95 [33] to perform maximum likelihood analyses on the concatenated data set. Five independent analyses of 50 million generations were performed. Nodal support was evaluated with 100 nonparametric bootstrap pseudoreplications.

Dating analyses

To estimate the relative divergence times within Corvus, we used beast v.1.6 [3436] and assigned the best fitting model, as estimated by mrmodeltest 2.0 [28], to each of the four partitions. We assumed a Yule Speciation Process for the tree prior and an uncorrelated lognormal distribution for the molecular clock model [35, 37]. We used default prior distributions for all other parameters and ran MC3 chains for 50 million generations. The program Tracer [38] was used to assess convergence diagnostics. To obtain absolute date estimates we calibrated the tree using secondary calibration points derived from Barker et al. [39] who used various approaches to date the all Passeriformes tree. Thus we used the age of Acanthisittidae versus other passerines at 76 ± 8 My SD (age within 95% confidence intervals = 62.8–89.2 My) and the split between Menura noveahollandiae and all other oscines 63 ± 2 My SD (confidence intervals = 59.7–66.3 My). In order to apply these calibration points, some additional taxa were included in the dating analyses (see Table 1). We also compared our age estimates with the classic mitochondrial 2% rule [40].

Biogeographical analysis

We used Bayes-lagrange[41] to assess ancestral patterns within Corvus. In a Maximum-Likelihood biogeographical analysis [42, 43] as implemented in the software lagrange [[42]], ancestral areas are optimized onto internal nodes. lagrange enables maximum likelihood estimation of the ancestral states (range inheritance scenarios) at speciation events by modelling transitions between discrete states (biogeographical ranges) along phylogenetic branches as a function of time. With the Bayes-lagrange approach it is possible to optimize on multiple trees whereby topological uncertainty is taken into account. We sampled 2000 trees (by thinning the chain stochastically) from the MCMC BEAST output, and ran lagrange on all of them. The frequency of ancestral areas for clades was then recorded and plotted as marginal distributions on the majority-rule consensus tree derived from the MCMC. The major advantage of the Bayes-Lagrange method is that the marginal distributions for the alternative ancestral areas at each node in the tree are the product of both the phylogenetic uncertainty in the rest of the tree and the uncertainty in the biogeographical reconstruction of the node of interest.

We assigned species distributions to one or more of nine geographical areas for the Bayes-lagrange analysis basing these on evidence of historical relationships of tectonic plates and terranes in the Indo-Pacific [44, 45]: Nearctic, Caribbean, Palaearctic, Africa, Indomalaya (including the Philippines), Wallacea, Australo-papua and the Pacific. The analysis was carried out using the maxareas (= 2) option in lagrange. However, we also ran additional analyses exploring the importance of changing the maxareas (setting maxareas = 3 and 4).

Brain size, tool use and innovation

Data on brain size, which are considered a good proxy for cognition and intelligence [46], and body mass, were taken from Mlikovsky [47] and Iwanuik & Nelson [48]. Although the data are drawn from two sources, the data have been converted to reflect inner brain case volumes and are therefore directly comparable. These two datasets together include brain size data for 29 Corvus species [47, 48]. By comparing the brain sizes for those species that are represented in both datasets it is clear that most discrepancies between the two datasets are explained by the size of the bird individuals measured. Therefore we believe that the measurements from the two datasets can be analysed combined. For a few species information on body mass was lacking in which case we used data from the CRC Handbook [49]. We compared the data on body mass used in our analyses [4749] with body mass data provided in Handbook of Birds of the World [10] and found the data to be in agreement. After ln transforming the data we regressed brain size against body mass for 30 out of 40 species of Corvus. We also ran separate analyses based on the two datasets from Mlikovsky [47] and Iwanuik & Nelson [48] to account for potential differences in measuring body mass and brain size. We compiled data on tool use and innovative foraging behaviour from studies by Lefebvre et al. [4], Overington et al. [50] and Bentley-Condit & Smith [51]. Additionally, we searched the Handbook of Australian, New Zealand and Antarctic birds [52] for data on Australian crows. Altogether we found that 10 out of 40 Corvus species have a documented record of using tools and that 17 out 40 species use innovative foraging strategies. We note that opinions differ on what it means to be a “real” tool user and that some species are only known to use tools in captivity (Corvus frugilegus). However, this only underscores the high plasticity of this behaviour among Corvus species.

To investigate whether tool use, innovative foraging strategy and colonization of islands was associated with relative larger brain size across Corvus species, we ran a phylogenetic generalized least squares model (PGLS) in a phylogenetic framework using R version 2.10.1 [53] and the caper R package [54, 55] as well as the ape package [56]. This statistical approach fits a linear model, taking into account phylogenetic non-independence. We tested the correlation of ln transformed brain size and ln transformed body mass as explanatory variables, with potential effect of tool use, innovation or island/continent distribution.

Results

Molecular phylogenetics and dating

Model based analyses performed on the concatenated dataset (six partitions: GAPDH, ODC, 1st, 2nd, 3 rd codon positions for mtDNA and tRNA; maximum likelihood (ML): –ln 16528.5601, Bayesian inference (BI) harmonic mean: –ln 15872.64) yielded a 50% majority-rule consensus tree (BI) that was topologically congruent with the Maximum Likelihood tree (Figure 1), (for well-supported nodes receiving posterior probabilities >0.95 or bootstrap values >70%). Scores of the best likelihood trees were within 0.05 likelihood units of the best tree recovered in each of the other four garli runs, suggesting that the five runs had converged.

Figure 1
figure 1

The 50% majority-rule consensus tree of the Corvus obtained from the Bayesian analysis of the combined dataset (GAPDH, ODC, ND2 and ND3). Above the branch is the posterior probability (only values above 0.95 are shown, asterisks indicate 1.00 posterior probabilities). Below the branch is the maximum likelihood bootstrap value (only values above 70% are shown) from 100 pseudoreplicates. Clades I-VIII are discussed in the text.

We find that the genus Corvus is monophyletic and furthermore recovered eight well-supported sub-clades that contain members more or less restricted to biogeographical regions. The basal members (Clades I-III) are distributed across the Holarctic region, the Caribbean and Africa. The Caribbean members are found in two well-supported separate clades (Clades II and III) but resolution between the clades and Corvus capensis remain unresolved. Clade IV consists of the Eurasian Corvus frugilegus and Corvus hawaiiensis. The western and eastern subspecies of C. frugilegus represent a deep split in concordance with a previous study on this species complex [15]. Clade V consists of all the African Corvus species (except C. capensis) and the Holarctic C. corax. Clade VI consists of Holarctic species that are separated in two distinct well-supported clades, one clade of Neararctic species (C. caurinus and C. brachyrhynchos) and one clade of Palaearctic species (C. corone and C. pectoralis). Clade VII contains all the Australo-Papuan and Wallacean taxa except the Wallacean Corvus florensis. The latter species remains unresolved relative to Clades VII and VIII. Within Clade VII we also find some Pacific taxa. One subclade within Clade VII contains all Australian taxa and we also recover a well-supported Australo-Papuan clade. The relationships of the Wallacean species, however, remain unresolved at the base of Clade VII except that there is good support for a sister relationship between C. unicolor and C. typicus. Clade VIII includes the widespread C. macrorhynchos, the Indo-Malayan C. splendens and the Micronesian C. kubaryi.

Our BEAST dating analysis supports a mid-Miocene origin of Corvus dating to around 17.5 Mya (age within 95% HPD confidence intervals = 14.05–21.19 My). Our chronogram was consistent with the “2% rule” (uncorrected pairwise distances) for the rate of mitochondrial DNA sequence divergence per million years for young nodes (Pliocene to present) but suggested somewhat younger diversification times than those that BEAST determined as of Miocene age, which could be expected due to saturation in the mitochondrial genes [57]. According to the 2% rule the origin of Corvus dates to about 11 Mya. Knowledge of a Corvus fossil from North America dating back to the late Miocene [58] does not add much further insight because the author was unable to assign a systematic position. However, assuming that the fossil is closely related to the other North American taxa it supports our age estimates based on secondary calibration points.

Biogeographical analysis

The Bayes-lagrange analysis (Figure 2) finds the origin of Corvus and its closest relatives (Pica, Nucifraga, Garrulus) to be within the Holarctic region. The origin of Corvus, however, is Palaearctic, although some deep branches lead to taxa distributed in North America and the Caribbean. Colonization of Africa took place in the Pliocene, and colonization of Wallacea took place in the late Miocene and led to further colonization of Australo-Papua around 5 Mya. We find evidence for four colonization events of the Pacific from Asia and Australia. The Caribbean was colonized twice. One Caribbean clade is sister to a clade of North American taxa (Clade III) indicative of colonization from there. However, the ancestral area analysis postulates a Palaearctic/Caribbean origin for the other Caribbean clade (II).

Figure 2
figure 2

A summary of the BAYES-LAGRANGE ancestral area analysis for the genus Corvus.The tree is a chronogram (pruned to include one individual per species) based on the BEAST dating analysis of a combined data set of mitochondrial (ND2 and ND3) and nuclear (GAPDH and ODC) DNA sequences. Pie charts at internal nodes indicate the probability of a given area of origin. The inset map indicates the regions demarcated for the ancestral area analyses and colours to the right of the taxon names indicate present distributions (Nearctic, Palaeearctic, Caribbean, Africa, Indomalaya, Wallacea, Australo-Papua and Pacific) and thus coding for the ancestral area analyses. Black parts of the pie charts indicate a mixture of other areas.

Brain size and tool use

We find a highly significant correlation between body mass and brain size in Corvus (P < 0.001 Figure 3) both for the combined dataset and when analyzing the individual datasets from Mlikovsky [47] and Iwanuik & Nelson [48]. This suggests that there are no significant differences in relative brain size between large and small Corvus species. Our analysis (Phylogenetic Generalized Least Squares) taking covariance between taxa into account, finds no correlation between brain size, tool use (P = 0.67) and innovative behaviour (P = 0.69), and no correlation between brain size and the ability to colonize islands (P = 0.46) (Figure 3C). Seemingly, all members of Corvus have the same relative brain size and species of all sizes have innovative foraging strategies/use tools and have been able to colonize islands.

Figure 3
figure 3

A) Phylogeny showing the taxa used in the comparative brain size analyses. Numbers in parentheses indicate the number of innovations followed by the diversity of innovations. Symbols indicate whether the taxon applies tools (upwards pointing triangle), innovative strategies (downwards pointing triangle) or both (star combining the two triangles) in its search for food. Distributions are indicated for islands (blue), continents (red) or both (grey). Island taxa are indicated in blue, continental taxa in red and combinations in grey. Residual brain size and relative brain size for the taxa are indicated to the right of the phylogeny B) Linear regression between brain and body mass. C) Box-plot displaying the difference (median, 25% and 75% percentiles and sample minimum and maximum) in relative brain size between Corvus species that use tools/no tools, Corvus species that apply innovation/no innovation and Corvus species that occur on islands/continents. Relative brain size represents residual values obtained from a linear regression between ln-transformed brain size and ln-transformed body mass.

Discussion

Systematics and biogeography

The early history of the classification of the family Corvidae has been summarized by Goodwin [12] but is restricted to morphology. We present the first complete molecular species level phylogeny for the crows and ravens (Corvus spp) including several subspecies of widespread species (Figure 1). Most notably, we demonstrate that what is currently classified as the Australian Raven C. coronoides, comprises two clades, one in the west (C. c. perplexus) and one in the east (C. c. coronoides). They are geographically isolated from each other only by approximately 100 km of apparently unsuitable habitat across the continent’s south coast at the Great Australian Bight. We propose that these two taxa be elevated to species rank. In contrast, populations of C. tasmanicus, geographically isolated from each other by >500 km, were not reciprocally monophyletic. Currently recognized as two subspecies, their isolation and divergence is presumably very recent. In accordance with a previous study including samples from throughout the Palaearctic, we show that C. frugilegus may also represent two distinct species, one in the western Palaearctic and one in the eastern Palaearctic [15]. C. macrorhynchos is found to be paraphyletic such that the Philippine C. macrorhynchos philippinus is sister to a clade comprising all other C. macrorhyncos, which occur in East Asia, and C. kubaryi. However, denser taxon sampling for C. macrorhynchos and population sampling for other widespread species (e.g. Corvus enca and Corvus orru) is needed to properly revise taxonomic issues at species and subspecies levels.

Our dating analysis suggests that the radiation of Corvus began in the mid-Miocene and our ancestral area analysis indicates a Palaearctic origin (Figure 2). This is consistent with the two most basal members, Corvus monedula and Corvus dauuricus (Clade I) being Eurasian and with the closest extant relatives of Corvus distributed in Eurasia and North America (Pica and Nucifraga). This results in a signature of a Holarctic origin for Corvus and its closest relatives. We infer that two clades independently colonized the Caribbean islands in the late Miocene (Clades II and III). One Caribbean clade (II) has a long branch that leads to C. leucognaphalus C. jamaicensis and C. nasicus and our analyses suggest a Palaearctic/Caribbean origin. This could be interpreted as evidence for long distance ocean dispersal similar to that inferred in other passerine bird groups that have crossed the Atlantic e.g. Turdus[59]. Two alternative interpretations of the Caribbean having been colonized from North America are possible (i) extinction of a North American ancestor, (ii) an ancestral form was widely distributed in the Holarctic (like Corvus corax) and gave rise to independent colonizations to the Caribbean followed by isolation of the North American population and a second colonization to the Caribbean (Clade III). The other Caribbean species (C. palmarum and C. minutus; Clade III) are sister to three North American species (C. ossifragus C. sinaloae and C. imparatus). This provides evidence for colonization by C. palmarum and C. minutus of the Caribbean from North America.

Relationships of an African species, C. capensis, were difficult to ascertain but it seems to represent a single Miocene colonization of Africa from the Palaearctic. C. capensis does not seem closely related to members of Clade V, which includes all other taxa that have colonized Africa and radiated within the continent. Clade VII of Indo-Pacific species has sequentially colonized Southeast Asia, Wallacea, Australo-Papua and the Pacific islands. However, the Hawaiian crow (C. hawaiiensis) is not a member of this clade, instead it clusters with the Palaearctic C. frugilegus (Clade IV) and so we infer it to have colonized Hawaii from East Asia. This is unexpected because the Hawaiian biota generally evolved through colonization from America whereas that of the rest of the Pacific was mostly colonized from Asia and Australo-Papua [60]. For the Corvus radiation, Asia has been the main source area for colonization of the Pacific as opposed to Australo-Papua, which is only the source area for one out of four Pacific lineages. Overall, the ancestral area analysis provides a rather clear pattern of separate colonizations of all continents except South America from the Palaearctic/(Nearctic).

Brain size, tool use and colonization

Tool use is rare in the animal kingdom and is considered restricted to primates [61], Cetaceans [62], and some birds (e.g. Psittaciformes and Passeriformes) [1]. By far the most well-known tool using bird is the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides) and several studies have demonstrated this island endemic crow’s ingenious abilities to use sticks to probe for larvae [5, 63]. It is well established in the literature that cognitive abilities correlate with larger relative brain size, and that the family Corvidae have unusually large brains compared to other birds [64, 65]. Particularly, the New Caledonian crow’s ability to use tools has been explained by its extraordinary large brain [6]. However, in the study by Cnotka et al. [6] phylogenetic relationships among crows were unknown and they were therefore unable to make appropriate phylogenetic corrections.

Several members of the genus Corvus use a variety of natural tools or advanced innovative strategies when foraging [summarized in 4]. Until now, innovative feeding techniques have been reported for 17 of the 40 species of Corvus and tool use for 10 of the 40 Corvus species ( Additional file 1: Table S1). There is only one tool using species (Corvus ossifragus) that is not known to have foraging innovations and it should be noted that tool-use is usually seen as species typical and often hard-wired as evidenced by experiments on young woodpecker finches [66] and young New Caledonia crows [67], both of which “know” about tools from the start without having to learn about them.

Our analyses on body mass and brain size demonstrate that there is a significant correlation between the two variables, meaning that all crows have the same large relative brain size (similar body mass/brain size ratio). However, our comparative analyses on brain size and innovative feeding/tool use strategies within Corvus, corrected for phylogenetic relationships, reveal no correlation between the variables. This could be interpreted in two ways. Either brain size has little to do with innovative foraging strategies/tool use and thus other factors are more important in determining whether or not crows use tools or innovative feeding strategies. Alternatively, all crow species have large brains relative to other birds and thus have the potential to use tools or other innovative feeding strategies. Given that a number of studies have already demonstrated a link between cognitive abilities and brain size in both birds [3, 4] and mammals [68, 69] and that it is well-established that corvids have larger brains than many other birds [1, 4], it seems the most likely hypothesis that all crows have the potential to develop innovative foraging strategies and to use tools in their search for food and there could be many reasons why this potential is only realised in some species across the Corvus tree. However, it has also been argued that total brain size may not be the ideal proxy for cognition and that measures should be taken to explore particular brain regions to explain innovation and tool-use [70]. We do not consider this study the final word on the topic, but merely a first attempt to combine phylogeny with functional traits associated with cognition and innovation in crows.

The most persistent hypothesis of large brains and corresponding enhanced cognition is that they evolved as an adaptation to handle novel or altered environmental conditions [71]. Island environments may prove particularly challenging as they, depending on the size and nature of the island, may provide fewer available niches, inferior access to food and new unknown dangers. On the other hand, a new island colonizer, could also find itself in an environment free of closely related competitors and free of inhibitors leading to occupancy of a wider range of habitats – ecological release [72, 73]. A combination of these two extreme scenarios, however, could to some extent counteract each other, which may explain the lack of correlation between relative brain size and island colonisations in crows and ravens.

Conclusion

The analyses based on molecular sequence data from all recognized crow and raven species (genus: Corvus) demonstrate that the genus is monophyletic and that it originated in the Palaearctic in the Miocene. From the centre of origin crows dispersed to North America and the Caribbean, to Africa and to Australasia, with several independent colonizations of remote Pacific islands. Our analysis comparing brain size and colonization of islands within Corvus found no correlation and we therefore conclude that colonization of islands by crows cannot be explained by brain size. We did not find a correlation between brain size, tool use and innovative foraging strategies as otherwise suggested by other studies e.g. [6,8]. Thus, there seems no reason to believe that brain size alone has any influence on tool use, innovative foraging stragegies and colonization ability within the crow lineages. Rather it would appear that large brains had already evolved in the ancestor of crows, leading to a generally high cognitive ability to deal with new challenges for crows and other corvid lineages.

References

  1. Emery NJ, Clayton NS: The mentality of crows: Convergent evolution of intelligence in Corvids and Apes. Science. 2004, 306: 1903-1907. 10.1126/science.1098410.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Cornell HN, Marzluff JM, Pecoraro S: Social learning spreads knowledge about dangerous humans among American crows. Proc R Soc London B Biol Sci. 2012, 279: 499-508. 10.1098/rspb.2011.0957.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Lefebvre L, Whittle P, Lascaris E, Finkelstein A: Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds. Anim Behav. 1997, 53: 549-560. 10.1006/anbe.1996.0330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Lefebvre L, Nicolakakis N, Boire D: Tools and brains in birds. Behaviour. 2002, 139: 939-973. 10.1163/156853902320387918.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Rutz C, Bluff LA, Reed N, Toscianko J, Newton J, Inger R, Kacelnik A, Bearhop S: The ecological significance of tool use in New Caledonian crows. Science. 2010, 329: 1523-1526. 10.1126/science.1192053.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Cnotka J, Güntürkün O, Rehkämper G, Gray RD, Hunt GR: Extraordinary large brains in tool-using New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides). Neurosci Lett. 2008, 433: 241-245. 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.01.026.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Sol D, Timmermans S, Lefebvre L: Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds. Animal Behav. 2002, 63: 495-502. 10.1006/anbe.2001.1953.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Sol D, Duncan RP, Blackburn TM, Cassey P: Lefebvre L Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005, 102: 5460-5465. 10.1073/pnas.0408145102.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Dickinson EC: The Howard and Moore complete checklist of the birds of the world. 2003, Princeton University Press, Princeton, 3

    Google Scholar 

  10. Dos Anjos L: Family Corvidae (Crows). Handbook of the Birds of the World Bush-shrikes to Old World Sparrows. Edited by: Hoyo J, Elliot A, Christie DA. 2009, Lynx Edicions, Barcelona, 494-641. vol 14

    Google Scholar 

  11. Jønsson KA, Fabre PH, Ricklefs RE, Fjeldså J: Major global radiation of corvoid birds originated in the proto-Papuan archipelago. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011, 108: 2328-2333. 10.1073/pnas.1018956108.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Goodwin D: Crows of the world. 1976, British Museum (Natural History), London, UK

    Google Scholar 

  13. Omland KE, Tarr CL, Boarman WI, Marzluff JM, Fleischer RC: Cryptic genetic variation and paraphyly in ravens. Proc R Soc London B Biol Sci. 2000, 267: 2475-2482. 10.1098/rspb.2000.1308.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Ericson PGP, Jansén A-L, Johansson US, Ekman J: Inter-generic relationships of the crows, jays, magpies and allied groups (Aves: Corvidae) based on nucleotide sequence data. J Avian Biol. 2005, 36: 222-234. 10.1111/j.0908-8857.2001.03409.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Haring E, Gamauf A, Kryukov A: Phylogeographic patterns in widespread corvid birds. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2007, 45: 840-862. 10.1016/j.ympev.2007.06.016.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Martens J, Böhner J, Hammerschmidt K: Calls of the Jungle Crow (Corvus macrorhynchos s.l.) as a taxonomic character. J Ornithology. 2000, 141: 275-284.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Rowley I: A fourth species of Australian corvid. Emu. 1967, 66: 191-210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Hackett SJ: Molecular phylogenetics and biogeography of tanagers in the genus Ramphocelus (Aves). Mol Phylogenet Evol. 1996, 5: 368-382. 10.1006/mpev.1996.0032.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Cicero C, Johnson NK: Higher level phylogeny of vireos (Aves: Vireonidae) based on sequences of multiple mtDNA genes. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2001, 20: 27-40. 10.1006/mpev.2001.0944.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Chesser RT: Molecular systematics of the rhinocryptid genus Pteroptochos. Condor. 1999, 101: 439-446. 10.2307/1370012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Allen ES, Omland KE: Novel intron phylogeny (ODC) supports plumage convergence in orioles (Icterus). Auk. 2003, 120: 961-969.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Fjeldså J, Zuccon D, Irestedt M, Johansson US, Ericson PGP: Sapayoa aenigma: a New World representative of Old World suboscines. Proc R Soc London B Biol Sci Suppl. 2003, 270: 238-241. 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0075.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Irestedt M, Ohlson JI, Zuccon D, Källersjö M, Ericson PGP: Nuclear DNA from old collections of avian study skins reveals the evolutionary history of the Old World suboscines (Aves, Passeriformes). Zool Script. 2006, 35: 567-580. 10.1111/j.1463-6409.2006.00249.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Holder MT, Lewis PO: Phylogeny estimation: traditional and Bayesian approaches. Nat Rev Genet. 2003, 4: 275-284. 10.1038/nrg1044.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Huelsenbeck JP, Ronquist F: MrBayes: a program for the Bayesian inference of phylogeny Version 3.1.2. 2003, Available from: http://mrbayes.scs.fsu.edu/index.php

    Google Scholar 

  26. Huelsenbeck JP, Ronquist F, Hall B: MrBayes: Bayesian inference of phylogeny. Bioinformatics. 2001, 17: 754-755. 10.1093/bioinformatics/17.8.754.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Ronquist F, Huelsenbeck JP: MrBayes 3: Bayesian phylogenetic inference under mixed models. Bioinformatics. 2003, 19: 1572-1574. 10.1093/bioinformatics/btg180.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Nylander JAA: MrModeltest2 Available from:. ;http://www.abc.se/~nylander/ 2004.

  29. Akaike H: Information theory as an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. Second International Symposium on Information Theory. Edited by: Petrov BN, Csaki F. 1973, Hungary: Akademiai Kiado, Budapest, 276-281.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Posada D, Buckley TR: Model selection and model averaging in phylogenetics: advantages of Akaike information criterion and Bayesian approaches over likelihood ratio tests. Syst Biol. 2004, 53: 793-808. 10.1080/10635150490522304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Wilgenbusch JC, Warren DL, Swofford DL: AWTY: a system for graphical exploration of MCMC convergence in Bayesian phylogenetic inference. 2004, Available from: http://ceb.csit.fsu.edu/awt

    Google Scholar 

  32. Nylander JAA, Wilgenbusch JC, Warren DL, Swofford DL: AWTY (are we there yet): a system for graphical exploration of MCMC convergence in Bayesian phylogenetics. Bioinformatics. 2008, 24: 581-583. 10.1093/bioinformatics/btm388.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Zwickl DJ: Genetic algorithm approaches for the phylogenetic analysis of large biological sequence datasets under the maximum likelihood criterion. 2006, PhD thesis, The University of Texas at Austin

    Google Scholar 

  34. Drummond AJ, Nicholls GK, Rodrigo AG, Solomon W: Estimating mutation parameters, population history and genealogy simultaneously from temporally spaced sequence data. Genetics. 2002, 161: 1307-1320.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Drummond AJ, Ho SYW, Phillips MJ, Rambaut A: Relaxed phylogenetics and dating with confidence. PLoS Biol. 2006, 4: e88-10.1371/journal.pbio.0040088.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Drummond AJ, Rambaut A: BEAST v1.4.7. 2007, Available from: http://beast.bio.ed.ac.uk/

    Google Scholar 

  37. Ho SYW: Calibrating molecular estimates of substitution rates and divergence times in birds. J Avian Biol. 2007, 38: 409-414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Rambaut A, Drummond AJ: Tracer v.1.4. 2007, Available from: http://beast.bio.ed.ac.uk/Tracer

    Google Scholar 

  39. Barker FK, Cibois A, Schikler P, Feinstein J, Cracraft J: Phylogeny and diversification of the largest avian radiation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004, 101: 11040-11045. 10.1073/pnas.0401892101.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Weir JT, Schluter D: Calibrating the avian molecular clock. Mol Ecol. 2008, 17: 2321-2328. 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2008.03742.x.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Smith SA, Donoghue MJ: Combining historical biogeography with niche modeling in the Caprifolium clade of Lonicera (Caprifoliaceae, Dipsacales). Syst Biol. 2010, 59: 322-341. 10.1093/sysbio/syq011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Ree RH, Moore BR, Webb CO, Donoghue MJ: A likelihood framework for inferring the evolution of geographic range on phylogenetic trees. Evolution. 2005, 59: 2299-2311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Ree RH, Smith SA: Maximum likelihood inference of geographic range evolution by dispersal, local extinction, and cladogenesis. Syst Biol. 2008, 57: 4-14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Audley-Charles MG: Tectonics of the New Guinea area. Annu Rev Earth Planet Sci. 1991, 19: 17-41. 10.1146/annurev.ea.19.050191.000313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Hall R: Biogeographic implications of the Tertiary palaeogeographic evolution of Sulawesi and Borneo. Biogeography and geological Evolution of SE Asia. Edited by: Hall R, Holloway JD. 1998, Backhuys Publishers, Leiden, the Netherlands, 133-163.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Lefebvre L: Taxonomic counts of cognition in the wild. Biol Lett. 2010, 7: 631-633.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Mlikovsky J: Brain size and forearmen magnum area in crows and allies (Aves: Corvidae). Acta Soc Zool Bohem. 2003, 67: 203-211.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Iwaniuk AN, Nelson JE: Developmental differences are correlated with relative brain size in birds: a comparative analysis. Can J Zool. 2003, 81: 1913-1928. 10.1139/z03-190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Dunning JB: The CRC handbook of avian body masses. 1993, CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL, USA

    Google Scholar 

  50. Overington SE, Morand-Ferron J, Boogert NJ, Lefebvre L: Technical innovations drive the relationship between innovativeness and residual brain size in birds. Animal Behav. 2009, 78: 1001-1010. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Bentley-Condit VK, Smith EO: Animal tool use: current definitions and an updated comprehensive catalog. Behaviour. 2010, 147: 185-221. 10.1163/000579509X12512865686555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Higgins PJ, Peter JM, Cowling SJ: Handbook of the Australian, New Zealand and Antarctic birds. 2006, Oxford University Press, Melbourne, Australia, Vol 7

    Google Scholar 

  53. R Development Core Team: R: a language and environment for statistical computing: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. 2009, Available from: http://www.R-project.org

    Google Scholar 

  54. Orme D, Freckleton R, Thomas G, Petzoldt T, Fritz SE, Isaac N: 2011, http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/caper/index.html,

  55. Freckleton RP, Harvey PH, Pagel M: Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: A test and review of evidence. Am Nat. 2002, 160: 712-726. 10.1086/343873.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Paradis E, Claude J, Strimmer K: APE: analyses of phylogenetics and evolution in R language. Bioinformatics. 2004, 20: 289-290. 10.1093/bioinformatics/btg412.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  57. Arbogast BS, Edwards SV, Wakely J, Beerli P, Slowinski JB: Estimating divergence times from molecular data on phylogenetic and population genetic timescales. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 2002, 33: 707-740. 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.33.010802.150500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Bickart KJ: Part I: The Birds of the Late Miocene-Early Pliocene Big Sandy Formation, Mohave County, Arizona. Ornithological Monographs, No. 44, Recent Advances in the Study of Neogene Fossil Birds. Edited by: Bickart KJ, Chandler RM. 1990, University of California, USA, 1-72.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Voelker G, Rohwer S, Outlaw DC, Bowie RCK: Repeated trans-Atlantic dispersal catalysed a global songbird radiation. Global Ecol Biogeogr. 2009, 18: 41-49. 10.1111/j.1466-8238.2008.00423.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Gillespie RG, Claridge EM, Goodacre SL: Biogeography of the fauna of French Polynesia: diversification within and between a series of hot spot archipelagos. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2008, 363: 3335-3346. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0124.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Tomasello M, Call J: Primate Cognition. 1997, Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  62. Marino L: Convergence of complex cognitive abilities in cetaceans and primates. Brain Behav Evol. 2002, 59: 21-32. 10.1159/000063731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Hunt GR: Manufacture and use of hook-tools by new Caledonian crows. Nature. 1996, 379: 249-251. 10.1038/379249a0.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  64. Harvey PH, Krebs JR: Comparing brains. Science. 1990, 249: 140-146. 10.1126/science.2196673.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Lefebvre L, Sol D: Brains, lifestyles and cognition: are there general trends?. Brain Behav Evol. 2008, 72: 135-144. 10.1159/000151473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Tebbich S, Taborsky M, Fessl B, Blomqvist D: Do woodpecker finches acquire tool-use by social learning?. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2001, 268: 2189-2193. 10.1098/rspb.2001.1738.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  67. Kenward B, Weir AAS, Rutz C, Kacelnik A: Tool manufacture by naive juvenile crows. Nature. 2005, 433: 121-10.1038/433121a.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  68. Baron G, Stephan H, Frahm HD: Comparative Neurobiology in Chiroptera. 1996, Birkhäuser Verlag, Basel, Switzerland, vol. I–III

    Google Scholar 

  69. Stephan H, Baron G: Frahm HD: Insectivora. 1991, SpringerVerlag, NewYork

    Book  Google Scholar 

  70. Healy SD, Rowe C: A critique of comparative studies of brain size. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2007, 274: 453-464. 10.1098/rspb.2006.3748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Allman J: Evolving Brains. 2000, Scientific American Library, New York

    Google Scholar 

  72. MacArthur RH, Recher H, Cody ML: On the relation between habitat selection and species diversity. Am Nat. 1966, 100: 603-609. 10.1086/282454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Losos JB, De Queiroz K: Evolutionary consequences of ecological release in Caribbean Anolis lizards. Biol J Linn Soc. 1997, 61: 459-483.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank a number of institutions for supplying samples for this study: AM, Australian Museum, Sydney, Australia; AMNH, American Museum of Natural History, USA; ANWC, Australian National Wildlife Collection, Canberra, Australia; FMNH, Field Museum of Natural History, Chicago, USA; NRM, Naturhistoriska Riksmuseet, Stockholm, Sweden; RMNH, Rijksmuseum van Natuurlijke Histoire, Leiden, Netherlands; UWBM, University of Washington, Burke Museum, Seattle, USA; ZMUC, Zoological Museum, University of Copenhagen, Denmark. KAJ and PHF acknowledge the Danish National Research Foundation for support to the Center for Macroecology, Evolution, and Climate. We also thank Leo Joseph, Jon Fjeldså and two anonymous reviewers for comments on the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Knud A Jønsson.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

KAJ designed the study, carried out the lab work, performed the phylogenetic analyses, and drafted the manuscripts. PHF carried out additional comparative analyses. MI assisted with lab work. All authors read, commented upon and approved the manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

12862_2011_2081_MOESM1_ESM.doc

Additional file 1: Table S1. List of taxa used in the comparative analyses Distribution is either insular or continental. Body mass (D) according to Dunning body mass (I) according to Iwanuik & Nelson and body mass (M) according to Mlikovsky. Brain size according to Mlikovsky and Iwanuik & Nelson. Feeding innovation according to Lefebvre et al. Bentley-Condit & Smith and Higgins et al. Feeding innovations according to Lefebvre et al. Bentley-Condit & Smith and Overington et al. [4, 7, 4751]. (DOC 104 KB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Authors’ original file for figure 3

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Jønsson, K.A., Fabre, PH. & Irestedt, M. Brains, tools, innovation and biogeography in crows and ravens. BMC Evol Biol 12, 72 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-12-72

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-12-72

Keywords