Skip to main content

Complex Genotype by Environment interactions and changing genetic architectures across thermal environments in the Australian field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus

Abstract

Background

Biologists studying adaptation under sexual selection have spent considerable effort assessing the relative importance of two groups of models, which hinge on the idea that females gain indirect benefits via mate discrimination. These are the good genes and genetic compatibility models. Quantitative genetic studies have advanced our understanding of these models by enabling assessment of whether the genetic architectures underlying focal phenotypes are congruent with either model. In this context, good genes models require underlying additive genetic variance, while compatibility models require non-additive variance. Currently, we know very little about how the expression of genotypes comprised of distinct parental haplotypes, or how levels and types of genetic variance underlying key phenotypes, change across environments. Such knowledge is important, however, because genotype-environment interactions can have major implications on the potential for evolutionary responses to selection.

Results

We used a full diallel breeding design to screen for complex genotype-environment interactions, and genetic architectures underlying key morphological traits, across two thermal environments (the lab standard 27°C, and the cooler 23°C) in the Australian field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus. In males, complex three-way interactions between sire and dam parental haplotypes and the rearing environment accounted for up to 23 per cent of the scaled phenotypic variance in the traits we measured (body mass, pronotum width and testes mass), and each trait harboured significant additive genetic variance in the standard temperature (27°C) only. In females, these three-way interactions were less important, with interactions between the paternal haplotype and rearing environment accounting for about ten per cent of the phenotypic variance (in body mass, pronotum width and ovary mass). Of the female traits measured, only ovary mass for crickets reared at the cooler temperature (23°C), exhibited significant levels of additive genetic variance.

Conclusions

Our results show that the genetics underlying phenotypic expression can be complex, context-dependent and different in each of the sexes. We discuss the implications of these results, particularly in terms of the evolutionary processes that hinge on good and compatible genes models.

Background

There has been a recent surge in interest among evolutionary biologists in elucidating the genetic determinants of phenotypic quality [1, 2]. This has been driven in part by researchers working in the field of sexual selection, who have sought to address whether females might acquire indirect genetic benefits for their offspring by mating with males of particular genotypes. As such, much attention has focussed on two groups of models that centre on the genetic benefits that might be gained from exhibiting mate discrimination: good genes and genetic compatibility [3–12]. In short, good genes models are based on the premise that genetic variation, acting additively, underlies the phenotypes that contribute to fitness (for instance, those upon which females might base a mating preference) [5, 7]. Thus, it is possible to rank males, according to their underlying genotypes, by their effect on the phenotype. In contrast, genetic compatibility models are based on the idea that no male genotype within a population is superior to others. Rather, mothers of certain genotypes will match better to sires of particular compatible genotypes, and thus, some male and female combinations will produce better phenotypes than others [8, 9].

The contribution and relevance of good versus compatible genes in determining offspring genetic quality has been ardently debated for over a decade, but remains unclear [3–12]. This lack of clarity stems partly from historical confusion as to a robust theoretical and empirical framework with which to discern each process [5, 13–15]. For instance, many tests of these models have relied on documenting phenotypic relationships between male sexual signals, mate preferences and subsequent offspring fitness [4]. However, it is clear that such phenotypic relationships will often provide misleading information as to the underlying genetic associations because of variation imposed by environmental factors [16, 17].

In 2005, Neff and Pitcher outlined a quantitative genetic framework that enables a simultaneous assessment of the potential for good and compatible genes processes to operate within a given population. They pointed out [7], reinforced by others [5, 6], that additive genetic variance for a given trait is consistent with the potential for good genes processes to operate, whereas non-additive genetic variance (encompassing dominance and epistatic variance) is consistent with the potential for genetic compatibility processes to operate. That is to say, that a particular allele that acts additively in effect on a given phenotype, acts independently of the other alleles with in the genome; whereas a particular allele that acts non-additively confers a given phenotypic value only in the presence of particular interacting alleles, either at the same locus (i.e. dominance) or at another locus (i.e. epistasis). Thus, if a particular phenotype possesses a strong underlying non-additive genetic component, then the expression of some key alleles that are transmitted from the father to the offspring will presumably be contingent on an associated set of alleles that are transmitted from the mother.

Several studies have now applied this quantitative genetic framework to assessing the potential for good and compatible genetic models simultaneously [18–28]. These studies have generally demonstrated that the genetic architecture of one or more key life history traits was consistent with the capacity to sustain either both of the models [19–25, 27], or the compatibility model in particular [18, 21, 26]. Note however, that all of these studies have measured the focal trait in a common garden setting. Therefore, it remains unclear whether the underlying additive to non-additive genetic variances are robust to heterogeneity in environmental conditions of the type likely to be experienced by natural populations. That is to say, natural populations do not exist in common gardens, but have distributions that cover a range of environmental conditions that vary in space and time.

It is well known that the prevailing environment has a pervasive influence on the link between the genotype and the phenotype. This has been reinforced repeatedly across taxa and trait types (both morphological and life history), via studies showing that trait values associated with particular genes or genotypes frequently vary across environments [1, 29–32]. Thus, for any given trait within any given species, we typically find variation in the slope of reaction norms that are associated with different genotypes [30]. The fact that genotype - environment interactions (GEIs) are a near ubiquitous phenomenon within populations also suggests that the amount, and perhaps type, of genetic variance underlying quantitative traits will vary across environments. Indeed, the available empirical evidence largely supports this suggestion, showing changes across environments in levels of additive and/or non-additive (dominance or epistasis) genetic variance for traits shaped by natural selection (e.g. body mass, wing size, development time and viability) [33–39]. For example, some studies have documented increases in genetic variance for traits expressed in stressful relative to control (standard rearing) environments [33, 38, 40], while others have showed no change [34, 36], decreases [34, 39], or variable patterns that change according to ontogenetic stage [41] or type of genetic variance [36]. Changes in the levels of genetic variance across environments have usually been attributed to novel gene expression in the new or stressful environment, or due to a historical absence of stabilizing selection on genetic polymorphisms when expressed at the novel environmental condition [38, 42].

In this study we explore GEIs, and changes in genetic architecture across environments, in the expression of some classic fitness-related morphological traits in male and female Australian field crickets (Teleogryllus oceanicus). We take a quantitative genetic approach, applying an incomplete full diallel breeding design to eight inbred lines, generating 38 outbred F1 offspring genotypes consisting of various reciprocal combinations of parental haplotypes. F1 offspring of each genotype were then reared at two different temperatures until adulthood, when measures of body mass and size and gonad mass were obtained. This approach enables us to assess the reaction norms of each genotype across environments, examining whether the effect of the paternal haplotype, maternal haplotype, or the interaction between parental haplotypes, on each trait changes across environments. The design also enables us to partition the genetic variance underlying each trait into additive and non-additive contributions (separating them from the residual and environmental variance), and to examine the magnitude of any maternal and paternal effects across environments. We discuss the results in light of their potential to impact on good or compatible gene processes that might be operating within the studied population.

Methods

Study population and construction of inbred lines

The crickets used in the experiment were the immediate descendents of a sample (n > 100) of wild type adults collected from a banana plantation in Carnarvon, north-west Western Australia in 2006. The founder population was maintained in the laboratory at 27°C on a 12 h light: 12 h dark photoperiod in multiple replicate 10 L plastic containers, with ad libitum access to dry cat food and water. Ten females were randomly-sampled and used to create 10 separate isofemale lines. Each of these lines was then immediately subjected to a full-sibling inbreeding protocol, in which a single virgin daughter was mated to a full-sib brother each generation, over five sequential generations. The estimated inbreeding coefficient (F) after five generations of such inbreeding is 0.672. All inbred lines were reared across two separate 5 L containers for most of their development. At their penultimate moult into adulthood, they were sorted by sex (one container per sex).

Diallel crosses

Inbreeding apparently affected productivity, with each line producing an average of only 14 pairs of adult offspring following five generations of inbreeding. This number was only sufficient to sustain an incomplete 8 × 8 full diallel reciprocal crossing scheme, performed in duplicate (the two weakest lines had to be discarded). Thus, for each of the remaining eight lines, a virgin adult dam was mated once to a virgin sire from one of the other seven lines, in all possible dam × sire line combinations. When possible (depending on available numbers of crickets), each line combination was replicated. We omitted inbred combinations from the crossing scheme - that is, we did not cross females from a given inbred line to males from the same line. After mating, each female was placed in a plastic container (16 × 12 × 5 cm) for two weeks (or until death), which contained food and a moist cotton pad for oviposition (replaced every 7 days).

The resultant nymphs of each replicate per line combination were distributed in equal densities across separate 5 L containers with ad libitum access to food and water. To mitigate potential density-dependent effects, we added two egg cartons to each container, and capped the maximum density of crickets at 30 per container. Nymphs were then exposed to one of two temperature treatments. Half of the nymphs per replicate line combination were placed at 27°C (standard rearing conditions for our laboratory culture) and the other half at 23°C. These temperature treatments fall well within the range of temperatures that T. oceanicus crickets experience within their native Australian distribution [43].

We lost a number of cells from the diallel crossing scheme due to reproductive failure and mortality of the inbred parents involved in the crosses. This resulted in some crosses in the diallel not being represented at all, and others being represented by only one of the two replicates (Figure 1). Roff and Sokolovska [44] experienced similar problems when conducting diallel crosses in the sand cricket, Gryllus firmus (15 of 42 cells lost). Missing cells might affect the variance component estimates through a general loss of statistical power, or if the missing cells were distributed throughout the diallel in a non-random way. However, unless the missing cells consistently involved lines with intermediate breeding values (an unlikely scenario since it is likely that the weaker line combinations will have been the ones that failed to produce F1 offspring), then any bias in the genetic variance estimates will be in a downward direction, and our estimates can therefore be considered to be conservative [22, 45].

Figure 1
figure 1

The diallel crossing scheme represented at each thermal treatment: a) 27°C and b) 23°C. Eight lines were crossed to each other in all combinations, barring the inbred combinations (female lines vertically, male lines horizontally). Cells shaded grey indicate those crosses that were duplicated, and hatched cells represent crosses without replication. The number of females versus males subsequently scored for fitness traits per cross are indicated within each cell. For example, in cross Dam 4A × Sire 1B at 27°C, 7 females and 9 males were sampled, with this cross not replicated (represented by the hatched cell).

Measuring traits

As the F1 offspring reached their final moult, we sorted both focal sons and daughters into individual holding containers (7 × 7 × 5 cm), with ad libitum access to food and water. Focal individuals spent nine to 13 days in these containers, after which point they were frozen at - 20°C. Each focal daughter was provided with a male from the stock population with which to mate freely over six hours, and then frozen. We then measured a number of fitness-related morphological traits in up to 10 individuals of each sex per line combination and replicate (Figure 1). Using a Mettler Toledo AG245 micro-balance, we measured body mass, and ovary mass in females, and testes mass in males. We also measured pronotum width using digital callipers. Each of these traits is very likely to be linked to fitness in T. oceanicus, and in males these traits are potential targets of sexual selection. For instance, in the family Gryllidae crickets in general, male body size is frequently assumed to indicate male quality, given the links between body size and call frequency [46]. Furthermore, in the congeneric T. commodus, male body size determines fighting ability [47] and is under sexual selection [48]. While little is known about the genetic association between testes size and male fitness in T. oceanicus [49], studies employing experimental evolution in other insects have found that increasing the force of postcopulatory sexual selection in males (via an enforced polyandrous mating system relative to a monandrous system) can result in an evolutionary increase in testes size [50, 51]. Finally, in female T. oceanicus, ovary mass is strongly associated with fecundity in females [49], but exhibits a negative genetic correlation with egg viability [52].

Data analysis

We took a two-step approach to partitioning the phenotypic variance of each trait, allowing us to examine 1) complex GEIs in the phenotypic expression of each trait in the F1 offspring, and 2) the underlying genetic architecture of these traits when reared in the different thermal environments.

In all analyses, we tested male and female data separately, even for traits that are shared across sexes (i.e. body mass and pronotum width). This is because these traits are sexually dimorphic (body mass F1,1042 = 11.92 p = 0.0006; pronotum width F1,1043 = 358.91 p = < 0.0001, Table 1), and the inclusion of both sexes into the one model introduced complex higher order (4-way) interactions, which resulted in model overfitting. Moreover, previous studies of body size [53], life span [54, 55] and thermal tolerance traits [56] in D. melanogaster indicate that the genetic architecture of each of these traits differs across the sexes. These findings reinforce our decision to analyze pronotum width and body mass separately by sex in this study.

Table 1 Trait means (± SE) and sample sizes for females and males, in each temperature treatment (H = 27°C, L = 23°C).

To examine GEIs on phenotypic expression, we estimated the variance component s associated with the parental Sire and Dam lines, and the Dam × Sire line interaction in both thermal treatments (23°C or 27°C), for each focal trait. This was done using Restricted Maximum Likelihood Analysis (REML) in the MIXED procedure in SAS (v. 9.1, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, USA). Sire and Dam effects were treated as random effects in the model, with individual line combination replicates nested within Sire × Dam combinations. The thermal treatment was treated as a fixed effect, and all interactions as random effects.

To discern the underlying genetic architecture of each trait at each thermal treatment, we applied the Bio model described by Cockerham and Weir [57] and Lynch and Walsh [58] to the data. This enabled us to break down the variance components into additive and non-additive genetic contributions, and separate out maternal and paternal contributions. The model equation for this procedure was:

where Y ijkl is the trait of the l'th offspring from the k'th replicate of the cross between sire i and dam j, and μ = the trait mean of the population. N i and N j are the additive effects of the nuclear genes contributed by i and j, independent of sex; T ij is the interaction of the haploid nuclear contributions; Mj is the maternal genetic and environmental effects of line j when used as dams; Pi is the paternal genetic and environmental effects of line i when used as sires; Kij is the interaction between maternal and paternal effects; Rk(ij) is the effect of k'th replicate cross within the dam line × sire line combination and Wl(k(ij)) is the unique effect of individual l within a replicate cross [22, 23, 59]. These genetic variance components were again estimated by Restricted Maximum Likelihood Analysis (REML), using the MIXED procedure in SAS v.9.1.3 (SAS Institute 2004), and the TYPE = LIN command to model the covariance between families as linear functions of the variances. The analysis was performed on the matrix of between-line crosses (excluding the within-line crosses). We tested the one-sided hypotheses that parameter estimates are larger than 0 with likelihood ratio tests, by comparing models where a given parameter was set to 0 with a model where all parameters were allowed to assume non-negative values [see [59] for details of this model and associated example script]. In addition to the script outlined in Fry (2004), we included a statement that separates the variance among individuals within replicate crosses from the variance among replicate crosses. We then estimated the causal components of the observational variance components using the approach outlined in Fry (2004) and Bilde et al. (2008).

  1. 1)

    σ2 n : nuclear additive variance = 1/2 FVA + 1/4 F2VAA, where VA is the additive genetic variance, and VAA the additive-by-additive epistatic variance, and higher order epistasis is ignored for simplicity [60]. Thus, to enable us to estimate the causal component of additive variation, for simplicity we will assume that VAA is small, and VA can then be estimated as 2σ2 n /F. Consistent with good genes models.

  2. 2)

    σ2 t : nuclear interaction variance = 1/2 F2VAA + F2VD + F3VAD + F4VDD, where VAD and VDD are the additive-by-dominance and dominance- by-dominance epistatic variances. Thus, the term includes epistatic and dominance variation. Again, if we make the assumption that the epistatic terms are small, then we can estimate VD as σ2 t /F2. Consistent with compatible genes models.

  3. 3)

    σ2 m : maternal effect variance VM, including both maternal genotype and environment effects, and possible interactions between maternal nuclear and maternal extra-nuclear effects.

  4. 4)

    σ2 p : paternal effect variance VP, including both paternal genotype and environment effects, and possible interactions between paternal nuclear and paternal extra-nuclear effects. Significant paternal effect variance might in theory be consistent with good genes models under the assumption that the paternal effects are genetic in origin.

  5. 5)

    σ2 k : non-reciprocal interaction variance, VK, of paternal and maternal effects, and of nuclear and extra-nuclear effects. Consistent with compatible genes models under the assumption that the non-reciprocal interaction variance is genetic in origin.

  6. 6)

    σ2 rep : variance among replicate crosses within line combinations.

  7. 7)

    σ2 w : variance among individuals within replicate crosses.

Coefficients of genetic variation (CV) for each variance component estimate were calculated by scaling the causal variance components by the trait mean; e.g. CVA = 100(√VA)/trait mean [61].

All traits exhibited normal distributions of raw data and residuals, with few if any outlying datapoints.

Results

Genotype × Environment Interactions

In sons, all traits measured (pronotum width, body mass & testis mass) were affected by a Sire × Dam × Treatment effect, explaining between 16 and 23 per cent of the phenotypic variance in those traits (Table 2, Figure 2). That is, phenotypic expression in the male F1 offspring was typically determined by complex interactions involving each parental haplotype and the environment in which these haplotypic combinations were expressed. A further analysis of Sire and Dam effects within each of the two thermal environments shows that Sire × Dam interactions abound in each environment, for most of the examined traits (see test statistics presented within Figure 2), involving substantial crossing of reaction norms (Figure 2). In contrast, two-way GEIs (i.e. Sire × thermal treatment, Dam × thermal treatment) did not significantly contribute to phenotypic expression of the son morphologies, nor were there any substantial main effects of Sire or Dam line that were general across both environments. Thus, the expression of each male trait was affected by an interaction between the Sire and the Dam lines used, but this effect was itself contingent in part on whether the traits were assayed at 27°C or at 23°C.

Table 2 Effects of Sire and Dam lines, thermal Treatment, Replicate (Repl), and their interactions, on the expression of the offspring morphological traits.
Figure 2
figure 2

Interaction plots, showing the mean trait values and reaction norms for each Dam × Sire line combination across each thermal environment (27° vs 23°C) for each trait type in a) males and b) females. Thus, each line represents a distinct Dam × Sire genotype. Below each plot, we present the statistics for each environment separately (i.e. Dam and Sire effects within each thermal treatment).

In daughters, three way interactions between parental haplotypes and the rearing environment were not significant. Rather, the female traits measured (pronotum width, body mass and ovary mass), were influenced by Sire × Treatment interactions that explained seven to 12 per cent of the phenotypic variation in those traits (Table 2).

Genetic architectures

The Biomodels revealed significant additive genetic variance components (σ2 n ) underlying all of the male phenotypes (Table 3) when reared at the laboratory standard (27°C) thermal treatment. Coefficients of additive genetic variance ranged from three to 14 (Table 4). In contrast, we detected less additive genetic variance for these traits when crickets were measured at the cooler thermal treatment (Table 3, 4, Figure 3). Only male testis mass exhibited a relatively stable genetic architecture across environments. Additionally, male body mass and pronotum width exhibited significant non-reciprocal interaction variance components (σ2 k ) in the standard, but not cooler thermal treatment (Table 3 and 4). We found no significant underlying dominance or epistatic genetic variance in any of these traits.

Table 3 Estimates of raw observational variance estimates for male traits, including standard errors (SE).
Table 4 Estimates of raw Causal Variance estimates and the Coefficient of Genetic Variation (CV) for male traits.
Figure 3
figure 3

Coefficients of variation (CVs) for variance components underlying each trait across each thermal environment (H denotes 27°C and L denotes 23°C). Male traits are represented on the top graph, and female traits on the lower graph.

In contrast to males, only one of the female traits (ovary mass) displayed a significant additive genetic variance component (CV = 23.5: Table 5 and 6, Figure 3), but this genetic variance was only detected in the cooler thermal treatment. In addition, female body mass exhibited a significant nuclear interaction variance σ2 t ) in the high temperature treatment. If we assume no epistasis, then we can estimate the coefficient of dominance variance for this trait at 9.2 (Table 6, Figure 3). Furthermore, female pronotum width exhibited paternal variance (σ2 p ), with a related CV of 1.9 (Table 5 and 6, Figure 3), but again, only in the warmer temperature treatment.

Table 5 Estimates of raw observational variance estimates for female traits, including standard errors (SE).
Table 6 Estimates of raw Causal Variance estimates and the Coefficient of Genetic Variation (CV) for female traits.

Discussion

Complex GEIs (haplotype × haplotype × environment) existed for all three of the male traits that we measured. In contrast, female phenotypes were in part shaped by interactions involving only the sire haplotype and environment. Furthermore, the relative contributions of additive and non-additive genetic variance underlying these same traits commonly changed across environments. We suggest that these results are likely to have implications on our understanding of several evolutionary concepts.

Genotype × Environment Interactions

Firstly, our results indicate that trait expression in male and female offspring is influenced by different levels of genetic complexity. In males, all traits were generally shaped by the specific combination of haplotypes that the offspring inherit from their parents in interaction with the environment; whereas in females, all traits were influenced by the sire effect only in interaction with the environment. Notably, across both sexes, the genetic effects were completely contingent on the environment in which the offspring were reared in. That is, in no case did we find Sire × Dam effects on F1trait expression that were general across environments. Likewise, in no case did we find additive main effects of the maternal or paternal haplotype on F1 trait expression that were general across environments.

The general lack of main effects of the parental haplotype on offspring phenotypes, and the prevalence of paternal haplotype × environment interactions for female traits, and the more complex paternal haplotype × maternal haplotype × environment interactions for male traits, adds a previously underappreciated level of complexity to our understanding of evolutionary dynamics within populations. The fact that no particular paternal haplotype resulted in greater phenotypic expression across all maternal haplotypes and environments is consistent with a resolution to the Lek Paradox (i.e. how genetic variation is maintained under directional selection) based on GEIs [62–68], as well as to the general question of whether non-additive gene interactions can contribute to maintaining genetic diversity within populations [69, 70]. A key assumption of the Lek Paradox is that traits subject to strong selection will initially harbour additive genetic variance that is eroded over time - unless there is a process that can counter this erosion. In line with this assumption, and acknowledging that GEIs might provide a resolution to the Paradox, we found that all male traits (likely to be under strong selection) harboured additive genetic variance (at least when measured at the standard thermal treatment), and that they were also all involved in G × E interactions. However, on the other hand, the existence of complex GEIs might well reduce the ability of females to accurately discriminate between males on the basis of acquiring good genes for their offspring, [1, 68, 71], particularly if the rank order in performance of a given set of male genotypes, for a given trait, changes dramatically across environments. This is because the consequences to a female of mating with a male of a particular genotype (in terms of the expression of the offspring traits measured here) will hinge on the genotype of the female involved as well as the thermal environment under which their offspring will develop. This level of complexity will presumably decrease, although not necessarily eliminate, the potential benefits to females of mate discrimination for good genes benefits, especially if there are costs involved in such choice.

Haplotype × haplotype × environment interactions are arguably more consistent with genetic compatibility models of quality, and it is worth asking whether such interactions might promote the evolution of mechanisms that allow females to discriminate between males on the basis of their genetic compatibility. Genetic compatibility models have risen to prominence in recent times [5, 7–10], with many quantitative genetic examples to suggest that Sire × Dam interactions are prevalent [18, 20–26], at least for juvenile life history traits (such as juvenile survival and development). One point worth noting is that there has previously been very little, if any, empirical attention devoted to examining whether Sire × Dam interactions (hence evidence for genetic compatibility) affect the expression of adult life history traits (reproductive parameters, morphologies and survival). In the context of the complex GEIs that we have identified in this study, if genetic compatibility models based on mate choice were to be relevant to this population of crickets in terms of offspring body and gonad size, then females would not only have to assess the quality of a focal male in relation to her own genotype, but also in relation to the environment in which the female will produce her offspring. This might therefore require the females to predict the environment in which the offspring will develop. The likelihood of such a scenario has to be assessed against how well our experimental set up reflects the natural environment of the crickets.

The thermal treatments that we used fall within the thermal range of the native distribution of T. oceanicus [43], as well as within the range of natural seasonal variation at the field collection site of these crickets in Canarvon, Western Australia. Given that T. oceanicus breeds year round [72, 73], it is realistic to expect that adults and developing nymphs in the wild at Carnarvon might experience temperatures within the range of our thermal treatments. Thus, our experiment shows that complex haplotype × environment interactions can indeed affect the expression of offspring phenotypes when thermal environments differ consistently by a few degrees, and such temperature differences may well be represented in the wild. That said, it is highly likely that crickets in the field are able to better offset variation in thermal environments through microhabitat selection than they are when in the lab (the crickets live in cracks in the ground or deep in the leaf litter where the microclimate remains warm). But note however, that we are unable to use this result to predict whether smaller thermal differences, or fluctuating temperatures, will affect the expression of parental haplotypes to the same extent. Further research is thus required to assess how the reaction norms tied to particular parental haplotypes respond to more subtle changes in the prevailing environment.

Although the traits that we assessed in this study were all morphological, the available evidence suggests that each is under selection and closely tied to fitness in males and females (see Methods). In this regard, these traits therefore provided a good opportunity to assess the relevance of genetic models, such as the good and compatibility genetic models to phenotypic expression, particularly given the paucity of information that currently exists as to the relevance of genetic compatibility models on adult trait expression (almost all previous studies have examined compatibility effects on juvenile fitness components). Models of good genes and genetic compatibility are almost always posed in the context of sexual selection, typically addressing the question of whether females might acquire good or compatible genes through mate choice. Therefore, it is worth asking whether the set of morphological traits that we measured might be used in mate choice or male-male competition for matings. Body and pronotum size are clearly traits whose expression might be readily assessed by the opposite sex, and as previously mentioned, studies in the closely related species T. commodus show that male body size is indeed under sexual selection [47, 48]. On the other hand, there is no evidence for an effect of male body size on fighting ability in T. Oceanicus [74]. Body and pronotum size in females, however, is under natural selection (large females produce larger ovaries, r = 0.37 [49], thus more eggs [49, 52]. On the other hand, gonadal traits in crickets lie embedded within the body cavity where they cannot be directly assessed during mate choice. However, testes size is clearly under sexual selection in several species [50, 51], and its expression is phenotypically correlated to body size in T. oceanicus (r = 0.37, n = 504), thereby providing females with a mechanism by which to choose males on the basis of large ejaculate investment. Moreover, molecules produced by the testes clearly have important downstream effects on female reproductive biology in Teleogryllus. In T. Commodus, the testes generate an enzyme complex, Prostaglandin synthetase, that contributes to the production of the chemical messenger Prostaglandin, which is transferred to the females in the seminal fluid [75]. These prostaglandins exert strong physiological effects on females, for example by stimulating oogenesis and elevating oviposition [76, 77], as well as by accumulating within the female's neural tissue (specifically in the head region [78], and possibly also suppressing the phonotaxis response to sexually signalling males [77]). Hence, we conclude that all of the morphological traits used in this study were relevant candidates for genetic quality models based on mate choice.

Genetic architectures

When it came to assessing the genetic architectures of the traits at hand, we generally found additive genetic variance for each male trait in the lab standard thermal environment (27°C), but not in the cooler environment (23°C). However, in females, we generally detected no such additive variance, with the exception of ovary mass at 23°C. This result is generally encouraging for good genes models based on mate choice, because it suggests that male candidate traits that are under sexual selection also exhibited an underlying genetic architecture consisting of standing additive variance. The disclaimer is that this genetic additively was only detected at 27°C, meaning that females might only draw benefits of discriminative mate choice if they can control or predict the environments in which their offspring are raised.

Previously, Simmons (2003) used a paternal half-sibling analysis to measure the coefficients of additive genetic variance in male testes and pronotum width, and female ovary mass and female pronotum width in T. oceanicus. While Simmons's results for pronotum width were consistent with our results in males and females [49], the measured CVAs for testes and ovary mass were different in magnitude to those measured in the current study. Specifically, at similar temperatures to that of the high temperature treatment in this study, Simmons found lower levels of additive variance for testis mass (CVA = 7.18 compared to 14.23). In contrast, he did find higher additive variance for ovary mass than we did in our study (32.05 vs. 10.06). These discrepancies are not surprising, and can be reconciled by considering that the two studies were separated in time by about a decade, and each study exhibited differences in their respective technical designs (e.g. in the breeding designs employed, the age and mating history of the focal individuals, and in sample size of captured genotypes from the wild type population).

As mentioned above, almost all of the detectable significant genetic variance for the morphological traits was found in the 27°C treatment, and we detected only CVA for ovary mass at 23°C. Although 27°C represented the standard rearing condition for crickets in our study, the crickets had only recently been sourced from the field population where they breed year round [72], and had not had any chance to adapt to the new rearing conditions (fewer than six generations in the lab under a heavy inbreeding protocol that meant that the genotypes were thus shaped under drift, not selection). As outlined within the Introduction, several studies prior to ours have detected differences in genetic architectures across environments [33–39], usually attributing one of the environments as the stressful or novel environment, and the other as the benign or standard environment. Given T. oceanicus breeds all year round in the field, it is clear that neither of the treatments is novel to the crickets, although it is likely that the cooler temperature was more stressful given that peak cricket abundances in the field occur during the warmer seasons [73]. Finally, we point out that the sample sizes differed across treatments, with slightly fewer cells and replicates represented in the diallel cross at 23°C (Figure 1). This might have resulted in a general reduction of statistical power in the cooler treatment.

Reconciling the quantitative genetic and GEI analyses

The presence of haplotype × haplotype × environment interactions, and haplotype × haplotype interactions within each environment (see test statistics in Figure 2) suggested that we should have also found abundant levels of non-additive genetic variance underlying the morphological traits in each environment. This was not the case. Our non-additive genetic variance components were represented by σ2t, an estimate of the reciprocal nuclear interaction variance (dominance or epistatic genetic variance), and σ2k, an estimate of complex non-reciprocal non-additive variance (including possible interactions of paternal and maternal effects, and of nuclear and extra-nuclear effects; see methods). Breaking down the non-additive variance components into these two estimates provided us with a more detailed picture of the genetic architecture of the traits at hand. The consequence of doing this, however, is that it decreases the power of finding significant non-additive variance components (relative to other studies based on North Carolina II breeding designs that have used the Sire term to directly estimate additive genetic variance, and the Dam × Sire interaction to directly estimate the amount of non-additive genetic variance [24, 26–28]. This explains the discord between our GEI and the Biomodel results. In contrast, the Biomodel actually has greater power to detect additive genetic variance than similar models that simply use the Sire effect as an indicator of additive genetic variance (as in the GEI analyses), because it uses information from both sires and dams to calculate the additive variance [59]. Accordingly, while we did not find additive sire effects at 27°C for any of the male traits measured (as indicated by the Sire × Treatment terms), we did detect additive genetic variance for all male traits at 27°C using the Biomodels. This indicates that additive variance is indeed present, but is swamped by more complex Sire × Dam × Treatment interactions in the GEI analyses.

Relevance to the natural world

Our study offers valuable insights into the likely complexity of gene by environment interactions in natural populations, and the evolutionary implications of these interactions. To generate these insights, we originally captured eight random parental genotypes from a wild population of T. oceanicus, and used these to generate 36 focal genotypes. Although these 36 genotypes represent a random sample of the total genetic variation segregating within the T. oceanicus population in question, it is important to realize that these genotypes nonetheless constitute only a tiny fraction of the total pool of possible genotypes within the population. This has implications. First, it is entirely possible that the parental genotypes that acted additively in our set of diallel lines, may not act additively in the wild population when matched against the complete pool of possible genotypes. Similarly, genotypes acting non-additively in our sample of genotypes, might exhibit additivity among the total pool of genotypes [6]. Although this point is pertinent to most quantitative genetic studies, it is particularly relevant to diallel designs which start with a small set of parental genotypes. Second, in diallel breeding designs, the parents used are inbred (although the F1 offspring genotypes that are subsequently tested are completely outbred). This might mean that presence and magnitude of parental effects are not representative of the magnitude of effects that occur in wild populations, in which the parents are likely to be outbred. Thus, while the results we present are accurate for the specific combinations of haplotypes used here, we must be cautious when scaling these results up to the genetic effects that occur within the wider natural population, let alone the species. Having said that, our motivation was not to calculate the absolute value of each genetic variance component to each trait in this species, but rather to address the potential magnitude by which genetic variances and gene expression might vary across environments.

Conclusions

In sum, we have demonstrated that complex GEIs make a significant contribution to offspring phenotype expression in T. oceanicus, and this result suggests that genetic compatibility models might be relevant to overall genetic quality. However, our quantitative genetic Biomodels also show that there is some additive genetic variance underlying key male traits that are likely shaped by sexual selection. Thus, the combined results suggest that there is at least the potential, genetically, for females to utilize a good and/or a compatible genes process when choosing mates to procure genetic benefits for the offspring, based on these morphological traits, at least at the standard rearing temperature. Whether females actually exhibit such mating preferences for the traits measured here, requires experimental testing. For example, despite the existence of the non-additive genetic variance required for compatibility models to operate, questions must remain about the significance of mate choice based on compatibility in driving trait evolution, at least in the context of sexually dimorphic morphologies, such as body and pronotum size. This is because mate choice for compatibility will exert balancing selection on these traits, whereas sexual dimorphism would seem most likely to evolve under sex-specific directional selection. Finally, we note that the relevance of our findings to natural populations of crickets will also hinge on the effectiveness by which crickets, when developing in their natural habitat, can mitigate the effects of environmental fluctuations of the magnitude encountered in our study via microhabitat selection.

References

  1. Hunt J, Bussiere LF, Jennions MD, Brooks R: What is genetic quality?. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2004, 19 (6): 329-333. 10.1016/j.tree.2004.03.035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Pitcher TE, Mays HL: An introduction to genetic quality in the context of sexual selection. Genetica. 2008, 134 (1): 1-4. 10.1007/s10709-008-9279-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Zeh JA, Zeh DW: The evolution of polyandry.2. Post-copulatory defences against genetic incompatibility. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 1997, 264 (1378): 69-75. 10.1098/rspb.1997.0010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Andersson M: Sexual selection. 1994, Princeton: Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  5. Puurtinen M, Ketola T, Kotiaho JS: Genetic compatibility and sexual selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2005, 20 (4): 157-158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Puurtinen M, Ketola T, Kotiaho JS: The Good-Genes and Compatible-Genes Benefits of Mate Choice. American Naturalist. 2009, 174 (5): 741-752. 10.1086/606024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Neff BD, Pitcher TE: Genetic quality and sexual selection: an integrated framework for good genes and compatible genes. Mol Ecol. 2005, 14 (1): 19-38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Tregenza T, Wedell N: Genetic compatibility, mate choice and patterns of parentage: Invited review. Mol Ecol. 2000, 9 (1013-1027):

  9. Zeh JA, Zeh DW: The evolution of polyandry I: Intragenomic conflict and genetic incompatibility. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 1996, 263 (1377): 1711-1717. 10.1098/rspb.1996.0250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Jennions MD, Petrie M: Why do females mate multiply? A review of the genetic benefits. Biological Reviews. 2000, 75 (1): 21-64. 10.1017/S0006323199005423.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Simmons LW: The evolution of polyandry: Sperm competition, sperm selection, and offspring viability. Annual Review of Ecology Evolution and Systematics. 2005, 36: 125-146. 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.36.102403.112501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Moller AP, Alatalo RV: Good-genes effects in sexual selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 1999, 266 (1414): 85-91. 10.1098/rspb.1999.0607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Mays HL, Hill GE: Choosing mates: good genes versus genes that are a good fit. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2004, 19 (10): 554-559. 10.1016/j.tree.2004.07.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Pialek J, Albrecht T: Choosing mates: complementary versus compatible genes. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2005, 20 (2): 63-63. 10.1016/j.tree.2004.11.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Kempenaers B: Mate choice and genetic quality: A review of the heterozygosity theory. Advances in the Study of Behavior, Vol 37. 2007, 37: 189-278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Reznick D: Costs of reproduction - an evaluation of the empirical evidence. Oikos. 1985, 44 (2): 257-267. 10.2307/3544698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Stearns SC: Trade-Offs in Life-History Evolution. Funct Ecol. 1989, 3 (3): 259-268. 10.2307/2389364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Rodríguez-Muñoz R, Tregenza T: Genetic compatibility and hatching success in the sea lamprey (Petromyzon marinus). Biology Letters. 2009, 5 (2): 286-288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Wedekind C, Evanno G, Urbach D, Jacob A, Müller R: 'Good-genes' and 'compatible-genes' effects in an Alpine whitefish and the information content of breeding tubercles over the course of the spawning season. Genetica. 2008, 132 (2): 199-208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Wedekind C, Müller R, Spicher H: Potential genetic benefits of mate selection in whitefish. J Evol Biol. 2001, 14 (6): 980-986. 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2001.00349.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Pitcher TE, Neff BD: Genetic quality and offspring performance in Chinook salmon: implications for supportive breeding. Conservation Genetics. 2007, 8 (3): 607-616. 10.1007/s10592-006-9204-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Bilde T, Friberg U, Maklakov AA, Fry JD, Arnqvist G: The genetic architecture of fitness in a seed beetle: assessing the potential for indirect genetic benefits of female choice. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 2008, 8: 295-10.1186/1471-2148-8-295.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Ivy TM: Good genes, genetic compatibility and the evolution of polyandry: use of the diallel cross to address competing hypotheses. J Evol Biol. 2007, 20: 479-487. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01269.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Evans JP, García-González F, Marshall DJ: Sources of genetic and phenotypic variance in fertilization rates and larval traits in a sea urchin. Evolution. 2007, 61 (12): 2832-2838. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00227.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Pitcher TE, Neff BD: MHC class IIB alleles contribute to both additive and nonadditive genetic effects on survival in Chinook salmon. Mol Ecol. 2006, 15 (9): 2357-2365. 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2006.02942.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Dziminski MA, Roberts JD, Simmons LW: Fitness consequences of parental compatibility in the frog Crinia georgiana. Evolution. 2008, 62: 879-886. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00328.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Evans ML, Neff BD, Heath DD: Quantitative genetic and translocation experiments reveal genotype-by-environment effects on juvenile life-history traits in two populations of Chinook salmon (Oncorhynchus tshawytscha). J Evol Biol. 2010, 23 (4): 687-698. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.01934.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Marshall DJ, Evans JP: Context-dependent genetic benefits of polyandry in a marine hermaphrodite. Biology Letters. 2007, 3: 685-688. 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0438.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Pigliucci M: Phenotypic integration: studying the ecology and evolution of complex phenotypes. Ecol Lett. 2003, 6: 265-272. 10.1046/j.1461-0248.2003.00428.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Pigliucci M: Evolution of phenotypic plasticity: where are we going now?. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2005, 20 (9): 481-486. 10.1016/j.tree.2005.06.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Flint J, Mackay TFC: Genetic architecture of quantitative traits in mice, flies, and humans. Genome Research. 2009, 19 (5): 723-733. 10.1101/gr.086660.108.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Mackay TFC: The genetic architecture of quantitative traits. Annual Review of Genetics. 2001, 35: 303-339. 10.1146/annurev.genet.35.102401.090633.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Guntrip J, Sibly RM, Holloway GJ: The effect of novel environment and sex on the additive genetic variation and covariation in and between emergence body weight and development period in the cowpea weevil, Callosobruchus maculatus (Coleoptera, Bruchidae). Heredity. 1997, 78: 158-165. 10.1038/hdy.1997.23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Hoffmann AA, Schiffer M: Changes in the heritability of five morphological traits under combined environmental stresses in Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution. 1998, 52 (4): 1207-1212. 10.2307/2411250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Hoffmann AA, Parsons PA: Evolutionary genetics and environmental stress. 1991, Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  36. Blows MW, Sokolowski MB: The Expression of Additive and Nonadditive Genetic Variation Under Stress. Genetics. 1995, 140: 1149-1159.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Jinks J, Perkins J, Pooni H: The incidence of epistasis in normal and extreme environments. Heredity. 1973, 31: 263-269. 10.1038/hdy.1973.81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Holloway GJ, Povey SR, Sibly RM: The effect of new environment on adapted genetic architecture. Heredity. 1990, 64: 323-330. 10.1038/hdy.1990.40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Kawecki TJ: Expression of genetic and environmental variation for life history characters on the usual and novel hosts in Callosobruchus maculatus (Coleoptera: Bruchidae). Heredity. 1995, 75: 70-76. 10.1038/hdy.1995.105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Edvardsson M, Hunt J, Moore AJ, Moore PJ: Quantitative genetic variation in the control of ovarian apoptosis under different environments. Heredity. 2009, 103 (3): 217-222. 10.1038/hdy.2009.44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Dmitriew C, Blows MW, Rowe L: Ontogenetic Change in Genetic Variance in Size Depends on Growth Environment. American Naturalist. 2010, 175 (6): 640-649. 10.1086/652470.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Hoffmann AA, Sgrò CM, Lawler S: Ecological population genetics: The interface between genes and the environment. Annual Review of Genetics. 1995, 29: 349-370. 10.1146/annurev.ge.29.120195.002025.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Otto D, Alexander RD: The Australian Crickets (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). 1983, Philadelphia, USA: Academy of Natural Sciences of Philadelphia

    Google Scholar 

  44. Roff DA, Sokolovska N: Extra-nuclear effects on growth and development in the sand cricket Gryllus firmus. J Evol Biol. 2004, 17 (3): 663-671. 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00673.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Dowling DK, Nystrand M, Simmons LW: Maternal effects, but no good or compatible genes for sperm competitiveness in Australian crickets. Evolution. 2010, 64 (5): 1257-1266.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Verburgt L, Ferguson JWH: Mate choice in field crickets: can females acoustically detect male body size?. J Ethol. 2010, 28 (1): 141-151. 10.1007/s10164-009-0166-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Shackleton MA, Jennions MD, Hunt J: Fighting success and attractiveness as predictors of male mating success in the black field cricket, Teleogryllus commodus: the effectiveness of no-choice tests. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2005, 58 (1): 1-8. 10.1007/s00265-004-0907-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Hall MD, Bussiere LF, Hunt J, Brooks R: Experimental evidence that sexual conflict influences the opportunity, form and intensity of sexual selection. Evolution. 2008, 62 (9): 2305-2315. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00436.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Simmons LW: The evolution of polyandry: patterns of genotypic variation in female mating frequency, male fertilization success and a test of the sexy-sperm hypothesis. J Evol Biol. 2003, 16 (4): 624-634. 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00572.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Gay L, Hosken DJ, Vasudev R, Tregenza T, Eady PE: Sperm competition and maternal effects differentially influence testis and sperm size in Callosobruchus maculatus. J Evol Biol. 2009, 22 (5): 1143-1150. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01724.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Hosken DJ, Ward PI: Experimental evidence for testis size evolution via sperm competition. Ecology Letters. 2001, 4 (1): 10-13. 10.1046/j.1461-0248.2001.00198.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Simmons LW, Garcia-Gonzalez F: Female crickets trade offspring viability for fecundity. J Evol Biol. 2007, 20 (4): 1617-1623. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2007.01320.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Rako L, Anderson AR, Sgro CM, Stocker AJ, Hoffmann AA: The association between inversion In(3R)Payne and clinally varying traits in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetica. 2006, 128 (1-3): 373-384. 10.1007/s10709-006-7375-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Nuzhdin SV, Pasyukova E, Dilda C, Zeng ZB, Mackay TFC: Sex-specific quantitative trait loci affecting longevity in Drosphila melanogaster. Proceedings of the National Acadademy of Science, USA. 1997, 94: 9734-9739. 10.1073/pnas.94.18.9734.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Vieira C, Pasyukova EG, Zeng ZB, Hackett JB, Lyman RF, Mackay TFC: Genotype-environment interaction for quantitative trait loci affecting life span in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics. 2000, 154 (1): 213-227.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Morgan T, Mackay T: Quantitaive trait loci for thermotolerance phenotypes in Drosophila melanogaster. Heredity. 2006, 96: 232-242. 10.1038/sj.hdy.6800786.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Cockerham CC, Weir BS: Quadratic Analyses of Reciprocal Crosses. Biometrics. 1977, 33 (1): 187-203. 10.2307/2529312.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Lynch M, Walsh B: Genetics and analysis of quantitative traits. 1998, Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates

    Google Scholar 

  59. Fry J: Estimation of genetic variances and covariances by restricted maximum likelihood using PROC MIXED. Genetic Analysis of Complex Traits using SAS. Edited by: Saxton A. 2004, Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc, 11-34.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Falconer D, Mackay TFC: Introduction to quantitative genetics. 1996, London: Longman

    Google Scholar 

  61. Houle D: Comparing Evolvability and Variability of Quantitative Traits. Genetics. 1992, 130 (1): 195-204.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Hedrick PW: Genetic polymorphism in heterogenous environments: a decade later. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 1986, 17: 535-566. 10.1146/annurev.es.17.110186.002535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Hedrick PW, Ginevan ME, Ewing EP: Genetic-polymorphism in heterogeneous environments. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 1976, 7: 1-32. 10.1146/annurev.es.07.110176.000245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Kokko H, Heubel K: Condition-dependence, genotype-by-environment interactions and the lek paradox. Genetica. 2008, 132 (2): 209-216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Turelli M, Barton NH: Polygenic variation maintained by balancing selection: Pleiotropy, sex-dependent allelic effects and GxE interactions. Genetics. 2004, 166 (2): 1053-1079. 10.1534/genetics.166.2.1053.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Gillespie JH, Turelli M: Genotype-environment interactions and the maintenance of polygenic variation. Genetics. 1989, 121 (1): 129-138.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Levene H: Genetic equilibrium when more than one ecological niche is available. American Naturalist. 1953, 87 (836): 331-333. 10.1086/281792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Ingleby FC, Hunt J, Hosken DJ: The role of genotype-by-environment interactions in sexual selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2010, 23 (10): 2031-2045. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.02080.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Neff BD, Pitcher TE: Mate choice for nonadditive genetic benefits and the maintenance of genetic diversity in song sparrows. J Evol Biol. 2009, 22 (2): 424-429. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01646.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Fromhage L, Kokko H, Reid JM: Evolution of mate choice for genome-wide heterozygosity. Evolution. 2009, 63 (3): 684-694. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00575.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Bussiere LF, Hunt J, Stolting KN, Jennions MD, Brooks R: Mate choice for genetic quality when environments vary: suggestions for empirical progress. Genetica. 2008, 134 (1): 69-78. 10.1007/s10709-007-9220-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Macfarlane J, Drummond F: Embryonic diapause in a hybrid between two Australian species of field crickets, Teleogryllus (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Australian Journal of Zoology. 1970, 18 (3): 265-272. 10.1071/ZO9700265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Simpson GB, Mayer DG, Robertson LN: Daily trap catches of 2 earwig (Dermaptera) and 3 cricket (Orthoptera) species in central Queensland. Journal of the Australian Entomological Society. 1992, 31: 255-262. 10.1111/j.1440-6055.1992.tb00503.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Thomas ML, Simmons LW: Male dominance influences pheromone expression, ejaculate quality, and fertilization success in the Australian field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus. Behav Ecol. 2009, 20 (5): 1118-1124. 10.1093/beheco/arp105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Stanley-Samuelson DW, Loher W: Arachidonic and other long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in spermatophores and spermathecae of Teleogryllus commodus: Significance in prostaglandin-mediated reproductive behaviour. Journal of Insect Physiology. 1983, 29 (1): 41-45. 10.1016/0022-1910(83)90104-X.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Loher W: The influence of Prostaglandin E2 on oviposition in&lt;/i&gt; Teleogryllus commodus. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata. 1979, 25 (1): 107-109. 10.1111/j.1570-7458.1979.tb02853.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Loher W, Ganjian I, Kubo I, Stanleysamuelson D, Tobe SS: Prostaglandins - Their Role in Egg-Laying of the Cricket Teleogryllus-Commodus. P Natl Acad Sci-Biol. 1981, 78 (12): 7835-7838. 10.1073/pnas.78.12.7835.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  78. Stanley-Samuelson DW, Klocke JA, Kubo I, Loher W: Prostaglandins and arachidonic acid in nervous and reproductive tissue from virgin and mated female crickets Teleogryllus commodus. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata. 1983, 34 (1): 35-39. 10.1111/j.1570-7458.1983.tb03287.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by funding from the Swedish Research Council (VR) to MN, and the Australian Research Council (ARC) to LWS. MN was supported by VR, DKD by the ARC and Monash University, and LWS by the ARC and the University of Western Australia. We thank Maxine Beveridge, Amy Denholm, Jessica Bell, John Trainer, and Paco-Garćia-González for technical assistance during this experiment, James Fry for statistical advice and comments on the manuscript, and Trine Bilde for statistical advice.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Magdalena Nystrand.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

MN, DKD and LWS designed the experiment, MN and DKD conducted the lab work. MN measured morphologies, MN conducted statistical analyses. MN, DKD, and LWS wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nystrand, M., Dowling, D.K. & Simmons, L.W. Complex Genotype by Environment interactions and changing genetic architectures across thermal environments in the Australian field cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus. BMC Evol Biol 11, 222 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-11-222

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-11-222

Keywords